Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-2lccl Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-25T17:23:29.541Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

On nudges that fail

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  12 April 2022

Nara Rela*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Pontifical Catholic University of Minas Gerais, Brazil

Abstract

The aim of this study is to respond to Cass Sunstein's question: ‘Why are some nudges ineffective, or at least less effective than choice architects hope and expect?’—particularly in view not only of the rational basis in decision-making but also of the direct influence of emotions on the behavior of those who must choose. In this study, I used findings from psychology surveys, specifically considering the influence of emotions on the fallibility of nudges in social interactions when wealth is compared. Special attention is dedicated to vanity, a combined emotion that leads to emotional choices, which arises in self-presentation and self-comparison when external signs of wealth are displayed. Imagination plays an important role in simulation to the extent that it causes failure in further nudges. In conclusion, I argue that vanity impels people to act differently, as expected of choice architects.

Type
Review Article
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Arnold, M. (1968), The nature of emotion. Baltimore: Penguin Books.Google Scholar
Ashraf, N., Camerer, C. and Loewenstein, G. (2005), ‘Adam Smith, behavioral economist’, Journal of Economic Perspective, 19(3): 131145.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Banerjee, S. and John, P. (2021), ‘Nudge plus: Incorporating reflection into behavioral public policy’, Behavioral Public Policy, 116. doi: 10.1017/bpp.2021.6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bilsbury, C. (2001), ‘Vanity as a psychological construct. Letter to Editor’, The Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 46(8): 760.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Christopher, A. and Schlenker, B. (2000), ‘The impact of perceived material wealth and perceiver personality on first impressions’, Journal of Economic Psychology, 21(1): 119.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deona, J. and Teroni, F. (2012), The emotions. A philosophical introduction. Oxon: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Diaktine, D. (2010), ‘Vanity and the love of system in theory of moral sentiments’, Journal of History of Economic Thought, 17(3): 383404.Google Scholar
Diatkine, D. and Walraevens, B. (2018), ‘From vanity to the love of systems, from luxury to the accumulation of capital, from the gaze of others to the endless process’, in Ege, R. and Igersheim, H. (eds), The individual and the other in economic thought, Oxon: Routledge, 1125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Egan, V. and McCorkindale, C. (2007), ‘Narcissism, vanity, personality and mating effort’, Personality and Individual Differences, 43: 21052115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elster, J. (1998), ‘Emotions and economic theory’, Journal of Economic Literature, 36(1): 4774.Google Scholar
Elster, J. (2010), ‘Emotional choice and rational choice’, in Goldie, P. (ed.), The Oxford handbook of philosophy of emotion, Oxford: Oxford University Press, 263281.Google Scholar
Epstein, S. (1994), ‘Integration of the cognitive and the psychodynamics unconscious’, American Psychologist 49: 709721.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Festinger, L. (1954), ‘A theory of social comparison processes’, Human Relations, 7(2): 117140.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frijda, N. (2004), ‘Emotions and action’, in Mnstead, A., Frijda, N., and Fischer, A. (eds), Feelings and emotions, Cambridge: Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 158173.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Furnham, A. and Argyle, M. (1998), Psychology of money. East Sussex: Routledge.Google Scholar
Galese, V. and Goldman, A. (1998), ‘Mirror neurons and the simulation theory of mind-reading’, Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2: 493501.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goffman, E. (1959), The presentation of self in everyday life. New York: The Overlook Press, 1725.Google Scholar
Goldman, A. (2006), Simulating minds. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldman, A. (2009a), ‘Mirroring, simulating and mindreading’, Mind & Language, 24(2): 235252.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldman, A. (2009b), ‘Précis of simulating minds: The philosophy, psychology, and neuroscience of mind reading’, Philosophical Studies, 144(3): 421434.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Guyer, J. and Vaughan-Johnston, T. (2018), ‘Upward and downward social comparisons: A brief historical overview’, in Zaigler-Hill, V. and Shackelford, T. K. (eds), Encyclopedia of personality and individual differences, Cham: Springer, https:doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-28099_1212-1.Google Scholar
Guyton, A. C. (1972), Structure and function of the nervous system. Philadelphia: W.B. Saunders.Google Scholar
Hausman, D. (2012), Preference, value, choice and welfare. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Infante, G., Lecouteux, G. and Sugden, R. (2016), ‘Preference purification and the inner rational agent: A critique of the conventional wisdom of behavioural welfare economics’, Journal of Economic Methodology, 23(1): 125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jacob, P. and Vignemont, F. (2017), ‘Vicarious experiences: Perception, mirroring or imagination?’, in Kiverstein, J. (ed.), The Routledge handbook of philosophy of the social mind, New York: Routledge, 498514.Google Scholar
James, W. (1890), The principles of psychology. Retrieved December 18, 2017 from: http://psychclassics.yorku.ca/James/Principles/prin10.htm.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kahneman, D., Sibony, O. and Sustein, C. (2021), Noise: A flaw in human judgment. New York: Little, Brown Spark.Google Scholar
Khalil, E. (1996), ‘Respect, admiration, aggrandizement: Adam Smith as economic psychologist’, Journal of Economic Psychology, 17(5): 555557.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lacan, J. ([1959]1997), The seminar of Jacques Lacan: The ethics of psychoanalysis. New York: W.W. Norton.Google Scholar
Lazarus, R. (1991), Emotion and adaptation. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Lea, S. and Webley, P. (1997), ‘Pride in economic psychology’, Journal of Economic Psychology, 18: 323340.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
LeBel, E. P. (2004), The vanity constructs re-examined: LeBel's vanity scale. Unpublished manuscript, University of Waterloo. Retrieved May 2020 from: https://osf.io/r5c28.Google Scholar
Lerner, J., Li, Y., Valdesolo, P. and Kassam, K. (2015), ‘Emotion and decision making’, Annual Review of Psychology, 66: 799823.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lipovetsky, G. (2014), A felicidade paradoxal – Ensaio sobre a sociedade do hiperconsumo (The paradoxical happiness – Essay on hyperconsumption Society). Lisboa: Edições70.Google Scholar
Lowenstein, G. and Lerner, J. (2003), ‘The role of affect in decision making’, in Davidson, R. J., Scherer, K. R., and Goldsmith, H. H. (eds), Handbook of affective sciences, New York: Oxford University Press, 619642.Google Scholar
Luban, D. (2012), ‘Adam Smith on vanity’, Modern Intellectual History, 9(2): 275302.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Martins, J. (2004), A Lógica das Emoções na Ciência e na Vida (The Logic of Emotions in Science and Life). Petrópolis, Brazil: Editora Vozes.Google Scholar
Mercier, H. and Sperber, D. (2017), The enigma of reason. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Morse, S. and Gergen, L. (1970), ‘Social comparison, self-consistency, and the concept of self’, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 16(1): 148156.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Netemeyer, R., Burton, S. and Lichtenstein, D. (1995), ‘Trait aspects of vanity: Measurement and relevance to consume behavior’, Journal of Consumer Research, 21(4): 612626.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nussbaum, M. (2001), Upheavals of thought. The intelligence of emotions. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ortony, A., Clore, G. and Collins, A. (1988), The cognitive structure of emotions. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Parkinson, B. (1996), ‘Emotions are social’, British Journal of Psychology, 87: 663683.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Payne, K. (2017), The broken ladder. New York: Viking Penguin.Google Scholar
Salvatore, S. (2019), Symbolic universes at European Society. RE.CRI.RE. Project. Retrieved April 23, 2021 from: https://ec.europa.eu›rest›cms›upload›240920.Google Scholar
Schlenker, B. (1984), ‘Identities, identifications, and relationships’, in Derlega, V. J. (ed.), Communication, intimacy, and close relationships, Orlando: Academic Press, 71104.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schlenker, B. (2012), ‘Self-presentation’, in Leary, M.R. and Tangney, J.P. (eds), Handbook of self and identity, 2nd ed, New York: Guilford, 542570.Google Scholar
Schlenker, B. and Leary, M. (1982), ‘Social anxiety and self-presentation: Conceptualization and model’, Psychological Bulletin, 92(3): 641669.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scitovsky, T. (1992), The joyless economy. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Shanton, K. and Goldman, A. (2010), ‘Imulation theory’, Wiley interdisciplinary Review: Cognitive Science, 1: 527538.Google Scholar
Slovic, P., Finucane, M., Peters, E. and McGregor, D. (2007), ‘The affect heuristic’, European Journal of Operational Research, 177: 13331352.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, A. [1759] (1982), The theory of moral sentiments, Raphael, D. D. and Macfie, A. L. (eds), Indianapolis: Liberty Fund.Google Scholar
Sroufe, L. A. (1995), ‘Emotional development’, in The organization of emotional life in the early years, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, https://doi.org.10/1017/CBO9780511527661.Google Scholar
Stellar, J., Gordon, A., Piff, P., Cordaro, D., Anderson, C., Bai, Y., Maruskin, L. and Keitner, D. (2017), ‘Self-transcendent emotions and their social functions: Compassion, gratitude, and awe blind us to others through prosociality’, Emotion Review, 9(3): 200207.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sugden, R. (2018), The community of advantage. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sunstein, C. (2017), ‘Nudges that fail’, Journal of Behavioral Public Policy, 1(1): 425.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tedeschi, J. T. and Norman, N. (1985), ‘Social power, self-presentation, and the self’, in Schlenker, B. (ed.), The self and social life, New York: McGraw-Hill, 293322.Google Scholar
Thaler, R. (1994), Quasi rational economics. New York: Russel Sage Foundation.Google Scholar
Thaler, R. and Sunstein, C. (2008), Nudge. Improving decisions about health, wealth, and happiness. New York: Penguin Books.Google Scholar
The Lancet (2021). Spatial, temporal, and demographic patterns in prevalence of smoking tobacco use and attributable disease burden in 204 countries and territories, 1990–2019: A systematic analysis from the Global Burden of Disease Study 2019. Retrieved September 2021 from: https://www.thelancet.com/journals/lancet/article/PIIS0140–6736(21)01169-7/fulltext.Google Scholar
Tracy, J. and Robins, R. (2007), ‘The psychological structure of pride: A tale of two facets’, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92(3): 506525.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Veblen, T. [1899] (2007), The theory of the leisure class. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Walraevens, B. (2019), ‘Vanité, Orgueil et Self-Deceit: L'Estime de Soi Excessive Dans La Thèorie des Sentiments Moraux D'Adam Smith’, Revue de Philosophie Èconomique, 20: 339.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whyte, K., Caplan, A., Selinger, E. and Sadowski, J. (2012), ‘Nudge, nudge or shove, shove – The right way for nudges to increase the supply of donated cadaver organs’, The American Journal of Bioethics, 12(2): 3138.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed