Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-mwx4w Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-06-16T22:43:14.171Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Developing our understanding of nutrition in depression

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  27 May 2021

Nicolas Upton*
Affiliation:
Wrightington Wigan and Leigh NHS Foundation Trust, Royal Albert Edward Infirmary, Wigan Lane, WiganWN1 2NN, UK
*
*Corresponding author: Nicolas Upton, email: nicolas.upton@wwl.nhs.uk
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Research to date has convincingly demonstrated that nutrition impacts depression. Population-based studies have shown that diet, food types, dietary supplements, gut bacteria, endocrine systems and obesity all play a role in depression. While nutrition could provide an important therapeutic opportunity in depression, clinical trials have not shown clinically meaningful results, and it appears unlikely that nutrition is a central determinant of depression. Conversely, however, prior research is inconclusive to inferring that nutrition does not have a clinically significant effect. This would require elucidating precisely when nutrition affects depression which necessitates an alternative, more granular, model for the nutrition–depression interaction. The network theory of mental disorders, which studies how mental disorders arise from a causally related network of symptoms and external factors, is proposed as an alternative model for understanding the complexity of the nutrition–depression link. This approach would uncover which relationships, between aspects of nutrition and depression symptoms, warrant further study at a population and laboratory level. Furthermore, from within nutrition science, is a movement dubbed ‘New Nutrition Science’ (NNS) that aims to integrate biological, social and environmental determinants of nutrition. NNS is important to nutrition–depression research which has yet to reveal how social factors impact the nutrition–depression interaction. Network theory methodology is fully compatible with the network modelling already used in NNS. Embracing both network theory and NNS in future research will develop a full and complex understanding of nutrition in depression.

Type
Full Papers
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of The Nutrition Society

The link between nutrition and depression has been extensively studied. There is a great breadth of scientific inquiry into the relationship encompassing work on diet, food types, dietary supplements, gut bacteria, endocrine systems and obesity. Population-level studies have repeatedly demonstrated a link between aspects of nutrition and depression (e.g.(Reference Firth, Teasdale and Allott1Reference Rahimlou, Morshedzadeh and Karimi3)). This replicable finding has obvious clinical bearing. Indeed, while, in the current epoch, we are observing the ‘double burden of malnutrition’ (rising obesity and undernutrition)(Reference Hawkes and Fanzo4), a significant proportion of patients with depression consume high-energy, nutrient-poor diets(Reference Jacka, Cherbuin and Anstey5,Reference Teasdale, Ward and Rosenbaum6) , and psychiatric medications are well documented to affect appetite and satiety(Reference Teasdale, Ward and Rosenbaum6). Nutrition may be an important novel addition to the available interventions in depression.

Approximately one-third of patients experience depression that is resistant to typical pharmacological therapies(Reference Hillhouse and Porter7,Reference Ionescu, Rosenbaum and Alpert8) . Combination with psychotherapy improves symptoms and reduces remission rates, but a significant treatment-resistant population remains(Reference Cuijpers, Noma and Karyotaki9,Reference McLachlan10) . Other therapeutic options such as atypical antipsychotic medications, electroconvulsive therapy or ketamine may have troublesome side effects, require specialist psychiatric input or not be widely available(Reference McLachlan10,Reference Voineskos, Daskalakis and Blumberger11) . Nutritional interventions may offer a widely available adjunct to the first-line management of depression. Importantly, while other external stressors to mental health are not within a clinician’s remit to intervene on (e.g. social deprivation, relationship breakdown, traumatic life events), a nutritional change, while challenging, is achievable (e.g. in obesity(Reference Canuto, Garcez and Souza12Reference Lopes, Freitas and Carvalho14)).

However, two issues remain in giving population-level findings’ clinical bearing. The first is that, although population-level study has consistently shown an interaction between nutrition and depression, effect sizes are not clinically significant (e.g.(Reference Molendijk, Molero and Ortuno Sanchez-Pedreno15)). Given that changing diets is difficult to achieve(Reference Lopes, Freitas and Carvalho14), the current evidence does not support the widespread use of nutritional interventions in depression. The second issue is that understanding is lacking in how exactly nutrition should intervene in depression, that is, which aspects of nutrition impact which depressive symptoms. These two issues are highly related, as, if it is the case that clinically insignificant effect sizes at a population level are due to certain nutritional factors impacting depression meaningfully, but only affecting parts (select symptoms) of depression, or certain subgroups of patients with depression, then, in select clinical presentations, implementing dietary change would be indicated.

Although it remains possible that nutrition has an evenly distributed, clinically insignificant effect across depression symptoms, it is important to establish for certain whether this is the case, because nutritional interventions, if effective, would be widely available across a range of healthcare settings and because research into nutrition and depression continues apace, but with an uncertain rationale. Overall, the current evidence is inconclusive to inferring that nutrition does not have a clinically significant effect in depression.

Theoretical and laboratory-level research into the depression–nutrition link has provided important insight into how nutrition intervenes in depression and identified areas for further interrogation at a population level. However, bridging theoretical and laboratory research with population-level study has proved difficult. One issue is in defining the importance of a single cellular or metabolic event within a complex human diet. Another is in accounting for the varied environmental contexts that people are exposed to, which may change how laboratory findings, revealed under controlled conditions, operate. A case study of this challenge is observed with research into the gut–brain axis, where convincing animal model findings (e.g.(Reference Zheng, Zeng and Zhou16)) have shown variable benefit when translated into clinical trials (e.g. with probiotics, see ref.(Reference Fond, Lagier and Honore17)).

An alternative theoretical model for the relationship between nutrition and depression is proposed – drawing from the network theory of mental disorders, outlined by Borsboom(Reference Borsboom18). The network theory of mental disorders claims that mental disorders are not single entities but dependent on an interaction of multiple internal and external phenomena. This approach applied to studying the nutrition depression link would (a) reveal a more granular understanding of how nutrition affects depression at the population level and (b) provide a framework in which to embed laboratory-level findings to understand how they are contingent on wider human physiology and environmental contexts. It is not claimed here that prior research either at the laboratory or population level has been inadequate in methodology or scientific rigour. Indeed, it is hoped that network theory methodology would reveal relationships, between aspects of nutrition and depression symptoms, that warrant further study at a population and laboratory level.

Meanwhile, from within the nutrition canon, a paradigm that increasingly incorporates social factors into nutrition research (see(Reference Cannon and Leitzmann19)) could further our understanding of the nutrition–depression link. Social factors, often controlled for in studying the nutrition–depression interaction, could be relevant in two important ways. Firstly, if nutrition is a key way in which certain social determinants affect depression, then, in those social contexts, nutritional interventions are more likely to be effective (see graphical abstract, depicting the two pathways by which social factors could affect depression). Secondly, it is important to reveal whether the impact of nutrition on depression is dependent on social factors, that is, are social factors effect modifiers on the causal pathway between nutrition and depression. Network theory methodology could again be employed here and is fully compatible with the network modelling already used in nutrition science to understand the multifarious (including social) causes of nutritional health.

Nutrition and depression, what is known currently?

Research covering a large swathe of nutrition literature has identified the relevance of nutrition to depression. These include studies on diet, food types, dietary supplements, gut bacteria, endocrine systems and obesity in depression.

Healthy diets reduce the risk of depression onset and reduce depressive symptoms(Reference Khalid, Williams and Reynolds2,Reference Molendijk, Molero and Ortuno Sanchez-Pedreno15,Reference Adan, van der Beek and Buitelaar20Reference Jesus, Silva and Cagigal22) . Additionally, unhealthy diets increase depression risk and depressive symptoms(Reference Jacka21,Reference Jesus, Silva and Cagigal22) . For example, Molendijk et al. (Reference Molendijk, Molero and Ortuno Sanchez-Pedreno15), in a large meta-analysis, demonstrated that population adherence to healthy diets had a significant linear relationship (P < 0·01) with reducing depression risk (OR: 0·64–0·78). Jacka(Reference Jacka21) and Conner et al. (Reference Conner, Brookie and Carr23), meanwhile, have shown dietary interventions improve depressive symptoms in randomised controlled trials (RCT).

Certain food types also affect depression. Foods with low glycaemic index are associated with lower risk of depression(Reference Rahimlou, Morshedzadeh and Karimi3). Meanwhile, foods with high inflammatory potential, measured using the dietary inflammatory index, have shown to negatively affect depression in a large (n 43 685) female cohort(Reference Lucas, Chocano-Bedoya and Shulze24).

Dietary supplements Mg, Zn, Fe, n-3 fatty acids and vitamin B9 have all shown some benefit in depression(Reference Firth, Teasdale and Allott1,Reference Li, Lv and Wang25,Reference Li, Li and Song26) , although there is some complexity in how dosing of supplements changes their impact(Reference Firth, Teasdale and Allott1).

The ‘gut–brain axis’, the interactions between the brain and microbiome, is a growing research area. Probiotics (capsules containing certain bacterial strains) have shown significance in reducing depression symptoms(Reference Firth, Teasdale and Allott1,Reference Liu, Walsh and Sheehan27Reference Wallace and Milev30) , although there is inconsistency in these findings, which is further discussed below(Reference Vaghef-Mehrabany, Maleki and Behrooz29).

Relatively few population-based human studies have examined the endocrine system in depression. The gut hormone ghrelin may slightly reduce depression symptoms(Reference Kluge, Schüssler and Dresler31), possibly due to its effects in improving sleep(Reference Kluge, Schüssler and Dresler31,Reference Morin, Hozer and Costemale-Lacoste32) . Given the role of obesity(Reference Pereira-Miranda, Costa and Queiroz33) and the metabolic syndrome(Reference Pan, Keum and Okereke34) in depression, pioglitazone, used in type II diabetes mellitus, has also been studied. In a meta-analysis, pioglitazone, which has anti-inflammatory and insulin-sensitising properties, induced higher remission rates of depressive episodes (OR: 3·3) even in patients without the metabolic syndrome(Reference Colle, de larminat and Rotenberg35).

Interpretation and clinical relevance

The above findings convincingly indicate that nutrition impacts depression. However, beyond using these findings to inform public health guidance (as advocated by refs.(Reference Khalid, Williams and Reynolds2,Reference Molendijk, Molero and Ortuno Sanchez-Pedreno15,Reference Jacka21,Reference Pourmotabbed, Moradi and Babaei36) ) authors argue that clinical applications should be postponed until research can clarify the mechanisms by which nutrition affects depression(Reference Firth, Teasdale and Allott1,Reference Khalid, Williams and Reynolds2,Reference Molendijk, Molero and Ortuno Sanchez-Pedreno15,Reference Adan, van der Beek and Buitelaar20,Reference Jesus, Silva and Cagigal22,Reference Li, Lv and Wang25,Reference Li, Li and Song26) . There remain challenges both at the population level of study and at the theoretical or laboratory level of study in generating this kind of mechanistic understanding.

Population-level challenges

Despite much population-level research being top-tier according to the hierarchy of evidence (meta-analyses/systematic reviews(Reference Burns, Rohrich and Chung37)), studies have yet to generate a mechanistic understanding of how nutrition and depression interact. Additionally, although this research has demonstrated that nutrition impacts depression, it has not revealed clinically meaningful effect sizes. For example, in a meta-analysis, methylfolate reduced depression symptoms 0·78 points on the HAM-D 17 depression scale(Reference Roberts, Carter and Young38), meanwhile another meta-analysis showed the number needed to treat with a high quality diet to prevent one case of depression was 47 participants(Reference Molendijk, Molero and Ortuno Sanchez-Pedreno15).

Given these small effect sizes, it is unlikely that any one nutritional intervention acts on a ‘central determinant’ of depression – as argued by Molendijk et al. (Reference Molendijk, Molero and Ortuno Sanchez-Pedreno15). The theoretical model, in which nutritional interventions act on depression as a single construct, is dominant in the population-level research canon. In these studies, ‘depression’, as an entity, is positioned as a latent variable, affected by a nutritional change, whose improvement is inferred from a change in depressive symptoms or rate of depression onset as a measurable outcome variable. While this is the logical starting point for evaluating the interaction between nutrition and depression, it is unlikely that understanding of nutrition’s clinical significance in depression will be advanced further using the same model. It is common in the literature to suggest further population-level research without proposing an alternative model for understanding the nutrition–depression link (e.g. with more participants(Reference Adan, van der Beek and Buitelaar20), or more targeted RCT(Reference Firth, Teasdale and Allott1,Reference Jesus, Silva and Cagigal22) or using randomised prevention trials(Reference Molendijk, Molero and Ortuno Sanchez-Pedreno15)).

Specifically, there are two reasons why positioning depression as a latent variable hinders understanding whether nutritional interventions could be clinically significant: (1) studies cannot provide a detailed interrogation of exactly how nutrition affects depression and (2) studies are required to control for variables, whose interaction with depression and nutrition is of therapeutic interest (e.g. socio-economic factors). Put differently, for a clinician it is important to know if nutrition does not affect all presentations of depression equally and whether certain contexts impact nutrition’s interaction with depression.

In a similar vein, Cartwright(Reference Cartwright39) argues RCT are an excellent method for advancing an ‘it works somewhere’ claim but do not develop an understanding of when an intervention will work, which depends on understanding the wide range of circumstances that determine the efficacy of an intervention. This level of understanding is not developed with RCT as their structure controls for factors that might contaminate an intervention effect on an outcome. It is possible, as some authors have reported, to use meta-analysis to try to tease apart the heterogeneity in the literature. Meta-analysis allows the identification of populations that are sources of heterogeneity in a cohort of multiple populations. For example, Firth et al. (Reference Firth, Teasdale and Allott1) identified that n-3 fatty acids had no efficacy in populations with physical health co-morbidities, and Li et al. (Reference Li, Lv and Wang25) identified that Mg had its strongest effect on depression in Asian countries. These findings, however, show a degree of post-hoc analysis and are not theoretically driven. Meta-analyses, again, are not the best method of understanding heterogeneity(Reference Higgins40) or, therefore, predicting when interventions will work.

Concluding their review, Jacka(Reference Jacka21) posits a future challenge is to ‘refine, replicate and scale up clinical and population level dietary interventions’. This will not be possible through population-level research without a shift in the theoretical model, given that nutrition, as discussed, is unlikely to be a central determinant in depression.

Translational challenges

Laboratory and theoretical research has investigated how nutrition might impact depression through gut bacteria, local inflammation, neurotransmitters and gut hormones(Reference Koopman and El Aidy41Reference Logan44). Some of this work has already translated to clinical trials, for example, with the use of supplements, probiotics and pioglitazone to alleviate depression (see the previous section).

There is a continued difficulty, however, in translating this research to real-world contexts. Human nutrition is complex, composed of innumerable interacting nutrients and affected by external factors (see(Reference Scrinis45)). Laboratory and theoretical research, meanwhile, interrogates interactions in highly controlled conditions.

This problem of ecological validity is due to initial research and subsequent translational work being unable to account for the complexity of human diets and the contexts in which they occur. Research on the gut microbiome in depression demonstrates this.

Challenges in gut bacteria–depression research

A replicable finding is that transplanting the microbiome from patients with depression to healthy animals induces depressive symptoms (e.g.(Reference Zheng, Zeng and Zhou16); for review, see ref.(Reference Yang, Li and Gui46)). Certain bacterial strains (Faecalibacterium, Coprococcus and Dialister bacteria) have also been identified at a human population level to be associated with depression(Reference Valles-Colomer, Falony and Darzi47). Exploring this relationship further at a molecular level with animal models of depression has revealed that unfavourable bacterial selection in the gut (termed dysbiosis) negatively affects depressive symptoms. This effect is shown to be mediated by the innate immune system(Reference Wong, Inserra and Lewis48), meanwhile certain bacteria (e.g. Clostridium butyricum) influence neurotransmitter metabolism with concurrent changes in depressive symptoms(Reference Sun, Wang and Hu49).

The diversity of evidence implicating gut bacteria in depression has justified the study of probiotic treatment in depression. Systematic review and meta-analysis of these studies have shown positive effects of probiotics on mood(Reference Fond, Lagier and Honore17,Reference Liu, Walsh and Sheehan27,Reference Nikolova, Zaidi and Young28,Reference Wallace and Milev30) . However, there remains a significant heterogeneity in the literature base in terms of the strength of this interaction(Reference Fond, Lagier and Honore17,Reference Liu, Walsh and Sheehan27,Reference Wallace and Milev30) , and one recent, updated review of RCT concluded that there was no enough evidence currently to support or refute the anti-depressant potential of probiotics (only seven of thirty-two studies showed a significant anti-depressive effect of probiotics(Reference Vaghef-Mehrabany, Maleki and Behrooz29)).

Two explanations arise from the literature as to why there is a problem in translating prior research into clinical gains. The first is that there has been a focus on a few key bacterial strains (which are those contained in probiotics); the gut microbiome, meanwhile, is composed of hundreds of bacterial strains(Reference Koopman and El Aidy41). Fond et al. (Reference Fond, Lagier and Honore17) argue probiotics’ limited bacterial content could be the cause for their mixed results in depression and that transplanting the entire faecal microbiome may offer better results. Here, prior research and its translational work fail to account for the complexity of nutrition. Indeed, probiotics are one of a growing group of single-agent nutritional interventions treated as pharmacological agents, termed ‘nutraceuticals’ (alongside vitamins, antioxidants, etc.)(Reference Jacka21,Reference Scrinis50) . Although interesting, they are only a small part of an individual’s overall diet, and investigating them in isolation may obscure their exact potential.

The second explanation for the therapeutic inconsistency of probiotics is that research cannot account for the context in which human nutrition occurs. Dysbiosis, the unfavourable shift in gut microbiome composition, has been shown to be affected by inflammation(Reference Wong, Inserra and Lewis48), western diet(Reference Noble, Hsu and Kanoski51) and possibly urban environments(Reference Logan44). It is not unreasonable to suggest that probiotics will be ineffective when environmental factors overwhelmingly negatively impact gut bacterial composition.

The above example demonstrates that the translational potential of laboratory and theoretical research into the nutrition–depression interaction is dependent on being observant of the complexity and context of human diets.

Network approach to nutrition in depression

A network approach to studying nutrition and depression would enable us to deepen our understanding of the nutrition–depression relationship while retaining ecological validity in our approach.

What is network theory?

The network theory of mental disorders, proposed by Borsboom(Reference Borsboom18), characterises mental disorders, including depression, as symptoms (shown as network nodes) that are causally related (via network edges) to other symptoms. Stable disorder states arise from strongly activated symptoms keeping each other activated by feedback relations, creating a self-sustaining network. The model includes external factors that can activate one or more symptoms and be part of creating or maintaining stable disorder states.

In part, network theory has arisen from increasingly sophisticated statistical methodology(Reference Fried and Cramer52), and from how representing mental illnesses graphically as a network allows us to understand their complexity in a way that is hard to achieve otherwise(Reference Epskamp, Cramer and Waldorp53).

Likely, the most controversial aspect of the model is that it rejects the notion that mental disorders arise from a common cause, proposing instead that a disorder is the causal interactions between symptoms(Reference Borsboom18). However, with network theory methodology it is possible to accept a mixture of these two models cooperating. For example, a common cause may activate a cluster of core symptoms that interact with others to create the full disorder profile(Reference Fried and Cramer52).

What could network theory offer the study of nutrition and depression?

Firstly, a network approach would permit studying the causal relationships between a range of nutritional variables and individual components of depression (i.e. symptoms, or even parts of symptoms (e.g. as described by Bentall(Reference Bentall54))). An example network is shown in Fig. 1(a). The strength of causal interactions (edges) between variables (network nodes) is depicted by the thickness of arrows. It is important to recognise that one pitfall of models that study multiple interactions is that they are at risk of overfitting data to the study population and reducing generalisability and replicability of findings. This can be controlled for, however, with statistical methods that reduce false-positive rates – such as reducing all small coefficients to zero, or to give up on weighted comparisons (i.e. identifying the strength of interactions) and instead settle for binary (present/absent) associations(Reference Fried and Cramer52). An example binary network model is shown in Fig. 1(b). In either case, a more granular understanding of the nutrition–depression relationship is revealed while avoiding the problematic assumption that nutrition is a central determinant in depression (as was outlined above).

Fig. 1. (a) Nutritional components (food GI, energy intake and diet variety) and depression symptoms (anergia, low mood and loss of pleasure) are depicted as network nodes. Within these two categories, straight lines identify where nodes within each category are likely to co-occur. Arrows depict causal interactions between nutritional components and depression symptoms. The thickness of arrows depicts the strength of interactions. (b) The same network as in (1a) is shown. Here, however, causal interactions between nutritional components and depression symptoms have been reduced to present/absent interactions in a simplified, binary network. GI, glycaemic index.

Secondly, having identified which are the strongest edges between network nodes, further study can be directed towards areas that will generate the most clinical gain. What form this further study takes is not mandated by network methodology, while network edges describe a causal relationship, they are otherwise theory free. This is attractive as the nutrition–depression interaction is characterised by several contributing research fields. One could envisage, for example, in Fig. 1(b), the edge between food glycaemic index and anergia being most easily described by biochemical mechanisms, whereas the edge between diet variety and loss of pleasure being most easily described by psychological mechanisms. The process of selecting important edges within a network to study further, by default, gives any theoretical or laboratory-level study of those edges more ecological validity and, consequently, better translational potential.

As well as addressing some of the limitations of previous research, a network approach offers further benefits that are of note. Networks are either constructed at a group level (partial correlation networks dubbed Pairwise Markov Random Fields) at a single time point, or at an intra-individual level (vector autoregressive model) where networks interrogate how symptoms relate to each other/external variables over time. The opportunity to study, with intra-individual level networks, how depression symptoms relate to nutrition over time is particularly appealing as the impact of nutrition on health is a prolonged process. As an example of such a study, Yang et al. (Reference Yang, Ram and Gest55) examined, over the course of a year, how social interactions affected mood. Across the year, participants measured their mood and related parameters after every interaction during three intense 21-d bursts, using smartphones. This allowed researchers to generate detailed conclusions of how mood and social dynamics influence each other over time. A similar protocol would generate understanding of how depression symptoms relate to food behaviour over a prolonged time course. Indeed, a problem faced in studying nutrition–depression interactions is that of reverse causality – it is a known phenomenon that people suffering from depression tend towards high-energy, nutrient-poor diets (Reference Jacka, Cherbuin and Anstey5,Reference Jacka, Cherbuin and Anstey56) . Although Jacka et al. (Reference Jacka, Cherbuin and Anstey5) have shown that this tendency does not explain away the nutrition–depression link, intra-individual network modelling would tease out whether a bidirectional relationship or feedback exists between nutrition and depression over time.

Lastly, RCT are a costly way of carrying out research. Generating enough ‘it works here’ claims through RCT to be able to make confident ‘when it will work’ claims would be a costly process in both time and money. The number of interactions that can be interrogated in a single network theory study would provide an important shortcut(Reference Alegria, NeMoyer and Bague57). Given the amount of nutrition–depression research already undertaken, it is possible that data already exist that could be analysed afresh, using a network approach, to gain new insights at a minimal cost.

Changes in nutrition research are relevant to mental health research

Nutrition research has broadened since its inception to incorporate social and environmental factors, and this has bearing on how we research nutrition in depression.

Developments in nutritional science

While nutrition has historically been a biologically driven research field, it is now argued ‘nutrition in principle and practice should be a biological and also an environmental and social science’ – this is the viewpoint of ‘the New Nutrition Science project’ (NNS)(Reference Cannon and Leitzmann19).

This change is a reaction to the rise in non-communicable ill health (e.g. obesity and diabetes) and the ‘double burden of malnutrition’ (rising obesity and undernutrition)(Reference Hawkes and Fanzo4). Public health guidance, based on an early biomedical understanding of nutrition, has not succeeded in curbing these trends. While the biological effects of food in the body are important, public health policy falls short when it is confined to dietary advice. Many complex social and environmental factors are central in determining the food that people have access to and eat on a daily basis(Reference Dixon58).

An example of New Nutrition Science in action is shown by Patel et al. (Reference Patel, Kerr and Shumba59). In a nutritional intervention in rural Malawi, researchers sought to improve child malnutrition by addressing the distribution of household work between sexes. Men were encouraged to be more involved in the preparation and cooking of food through cultural events and ‘recipe days’; children in those communities that adopted the scheme showed improved growth measurements across a 7-year period. Here, one cause of malnutrition, sex inequality, was identified and targeted as a specific cultural determinant of malnutrition. Clearly, this cause of malnutrition could not be identified and addressed through a purely biological understanding of nutrition.

Relevance of New Nutrition Science to researching the depression–nutrition interaction

New Nutrition Science is relevant to researching the nutrition–depression relationship. Given that social determinants have bearing on nutrition, understanding the nutrition–depression link requires accounting for social factors. There are two ways in which we can characterise how social determinants might be relevant.

Firstly, we could examine how a nutritional intervention in depression is dependent on social context: social factors may be effect modifiers of the depression–nutrition interaction. There are a number of ways that a measured effect modifier (socio-economic factors) can be causally related to the effect of one variable (nutrition) on another (depression)(Reference VanderWeele and Robins60) to outline a helpful classification). For example, Pourmotabbed et al. (Reference Pourmotabbed, Moradi and Babaei36) in systematic review showed that food insecurity increases depression risk (adjusting for other social variables - age, sex, race/ethnicity, income, education, living arrangement, etc.); one could imagine that the impact of a nutritional intervention on depression would vary across the degree of food insecurity at baseline. This would be an example of direct effect modification(Reference VanderWeele and Robins60). Alternatively, Logan(Reference Logan44) advances the idea that urban environments cause unfavourable shifts in gut bacteria (gut dysbiosis) - which is attributed to increasing depression risk (see above). Here, urban environments would be an indirect effect modifier, acting through gut dysbiosis, to modify the effect of a nutritional intervention (say, probiotics) on depression(Reference VanderWeele and Robins60).

Alternatively, we can describe the relevance of social factors as impacting depression via nutrition. For example, in an observational study, one might detect lower rates of depression in poor rural farming communities compared with rates of depression in poor urban environments. Some of the effect of social context on depression could occur via nutrition (e.g. to take from the above example, lower energy intake and unhealthy gut flora in the urban group) as well as by other means (e.g. more violent crime in urban areas).

Jacka et al. (Reference Jacka, Cherbuin and Anstey56) have already made strides to outline the extent that social factors affect the depression–nutrition interaction. Indeed, they found that ‘socioeconomic factors explained 25·2 % of the effect of prudent diet and 66·0 % of the effect of western diet on depression symptom scores’. Future research could help elucidate how to use nutritional interventions clinically in depression by exploring which social factors impact depression via nutrition. In patients from these backgrounds, nutritional interventions would be more strongly indicated.

How then to develop a full and detailed understanding of the complex interaction of nutrition, depression and social factors? Network modelling may again provide a solution. Network modelling has been used to identify the different factors influencing food behaviour(Reference Hummel and Hoffmann61). Meanwhile, social network studies have investigated how food behaviour spreads across social networks(Reference Christakis and Fowler62), for example, obesity developing through peer groups in schools(Reference Valente, Fujimoto and Chou63) and eating disorders developing across friendship groups(Reference Forney64,Reference Simone, Long and Lockhart65) . Furthermore, directed acyclic graphs that are used in epidemiology and to model effect modification(Reference VanderWeele and Robins60,Reference VanderWeele, Hernán and Robins66) are also used by the proponents of the network theory of mental disorders(Reference Borsboom and Cramer67).

One could imagine either (a) at a population-level study, incorporating social factors (e.g. social isolation) within a network model of the nutrition–depression interaction (see Fig. 2), or (b) at an intra-individual level, studying how food behaviour–depression links are impacted by other individuals in a social network over time. Work of this kind combining ‘slow and fast networks’ has already been done studying the interaction between background personality and depressive episodes(Reference Lunansky, van Borkulo and Borsboom68). Similarly, network modelling has been employed to study the interaction between neighbourhood social environment and mental health(Reference McElroy, McIntyre and Bentall69) and the bidirectional relationship between social media use and depressive symptoms(Reference Aalbers, McNally and Heeren70).

Fig. 2. In the above example, the network node ‘social isolation’ is causally related via network edge to anergia and diet variety. One can envisage the knock-on effect this would have on loss of pleasure in particular.

Conclusion

Progress has been made in investigating the relationship between depression and nutrition, although problems remain. Population-level research has not revealed the mechanisms that account for the relationship and cannot, therefore, reliably predict when interventions will be effective. Theoretical research has focussed on individual causal pathways, which makes their results hard to generalise to less-controlled contexts. The solution to these problems is to adopt a model of depression, derived from the network theory of mental disorders, and an understanding of nutrition, which incorporates social and environmental factors. These are highly compatible paradigms and, between them, allow the incorporation of multiple causal pathways into a testable mechanistic model. Looking forward, this is the most promising route to determining exactly when and whether dietary interventions can be used to combat depression.

Acknowledgements

I am very grateful to Mr Joe Gough for his insightful comments on the manuscript. This research received no specific grant from any funding agency, commercial or not-for-profit sectors.

This manuscript was written, in its entirety, by the sole author Nicolas Upton.

There are no conflicts of interest.

References

Firth, J, Teasdale, SB, Allott, K, et al. (2019) The efficacy and safety of nutrient supplements in the treatment of mental disorders: a meta-review of meta-analyses of randomized controlled trials. World Psychiatry 18, 308324.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Khalid, S, Williams, CM & Reynolds, SA (2016) Is there an association between diet and depression in children and adolescents? A systematic review. Br J Nutr 116, 20972108.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rahimlou, M, Morshedzadeh, N, Karimi, S, et al. (2018) Association between dietary glycemic index and glycemic load with depression: a systematic review. Eur J Nutr 57, 23332340.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hawkes, C & Fanzo, J (2017) Global Nutrition Report 2017: Nourishing the SDGs. Development Initiatives Research Ltd. Global Nutrition Report. https://globalnutritionreport.org/reports/2017-global-nutrition-report/ Google Scholar
Jacka, FN, Cherbuin, N, Anstey, KJ, et al. (2015) Does reverse causality explain the relationship between diet and depression? J Affect Disord 175, 248250.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Teasdale, SB, Ward, PB, Rosenbaum, S, et al. (2017) Solving a weighty problem: systematic review and meta-analysis of nutrition interventions in severe mental illness. Br J Psychiatry 210, 110118.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hillhouse, TM & Porter, JH (2015) A brief history of the development of antidepressant drugs: from monoamines to glutamate. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol 23, 121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ionescu, DF, Rosenbaum, JF & Alpert, JE (2015) Pharmacological approaches to the challenge of treatment-resistant depression. Dialogues Clin Neurosci 17, 111126.Google Scholar
Cuijpers, P, Noma, H, Karyotaki, EH, et al. (2020) A network meta-analysis of the effects of psychotherapies, pharmacotherapies and their combination in the treatment of adult depression. World Psychiatry 19, 92107.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McLachlan, G (2018) Treatment resistant depression: what are the options?. BMJ 363, 5354.Google ScholarPubMed
Voineskos, D, Daskalakis, ZJ & Blumberger, DM (2020) management of treatment-resistant depression: challenges and strategies. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat 16, 221234.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Canuto, R, Garcez, A, Souza, RV de, et al. (2021) Nutritional intervention strategies for the management of overweight and obesity in primary health care: a systematic review with meta-analysis. Obes Rev 22, e13143.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Habib-Mourad, C, Ghandour, LA, Maliha, C, et al. (2020) Impact of a one-year school-based teacher-implemented nutrition and physical activity intervention: main findings and future recommendations. BMC Public Health 20, 256.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lopes, MS, Freitas, PP, Carvalho, MCR, et al. (2021) Challenges for obesity management in a unified health system: the view of health professionals. Fam Pract 38, 410.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Molendijk, M, Molero, P, Ortuno Sanchez-Pedreno, F, et al. (2018) Diet quality and depression risk: a systematic review and dose-response meta-analysis of prospective studies. J Affect Disord 226, 346354.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zheng, P, Zeng, B, Zhou, C, et al. (2016) Gut microbiome remodeling induces depressive-like behaviors through a pathway mediated by the host’s metabolism. Mol Psychiatry 21, 786796.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fond, GB, Lagier, J-C, Honore, S, et al. (2020) Microbiota-orientated treatments for major depression and schizophrenia. Nutrients 12, 1024.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Borsboom, D (2017) A network theory of mental disorders. World Psychiatry 16, 513.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cannon, G & Leitzmann, C (2005) The new nutrition science project. Public Health Nutr 8, 673694.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Adan, RAH, van der Beek, EM, Buitelaar, JK, et al. (2019) Nutritional psychiatry: towards improving mental health by what you eat. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol 29, 13211332.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jacka, FN (2017) Nutritional psychiatry: where to next? EBioMedicine 17, 2429.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jesus, M, Silva, T, Cagigal, C, et al. (2019) Dietary patterns: a new therapeutic approach for depression? Curr Pharm Biotechnol 20, 123129.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Conner, TS, Brookie, KL, Carr, AC, et al. (2017). Let them eat fruit! The effect of fruit and vegetable consumption on psychological well-being in young adults: A randomized controlled trial. Plos One 12, e0171206. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0171206 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lucas, M, Chocano-Bedoya, P, Shulze, MB, et al. (2014) Inflammatory dietary pattern and risk of depression among women. Brain Behav Immun 36, 4653.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Li, B, Lv, J, Wang, W, et al. (2017) Dietary magnesium and calcium intake and risk of depression in the general population: a meta-analysis. Aust N Z J Psychiatry 51, 219229.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Li, Z, Li, B, Song, X, et al. (2017) Dietary zinc and iron intake and risk of depression: a meta-analysis. Psychiatry Res 251, 4147.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liu, RT, Walsh, RFL & Sheehan, AE (2019) Prebiotics and probiotics for depression and anxiety: a systematic review and meta-analysis of controlled clinical trials. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 102, 1323.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nikolova, V, Zaidi, SY, Young, AH, et al. (2019) Gut feeling: randomized controlled trials of probiotics for the treatment of clinical depression: systematic review and meta-analysis. Ther Adv Psychopharmacol 9, UNSP 2045125319859963.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vaghef-Mehrabany, E, Maleki, V, Behrooz, M, et al. (2020) Can psychobiotics “mood” ify gut? An update systematic review of randomized controlled trials in healthy and clinical subjects, on anti-depressant effects of probiotics, prebiotics, and synbiotics. Clin Nutr 39, 13951410.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wallace, CJK & Milev, R (2017) The effects of probiotics on depressive symptoms in humans: a systematic review. Ann Gen Psychiatry 16, 14.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kluge, M, Schüssler, P, Dresler, M, et al. (2011) Effects of ghrelin on psychopathology, sleep and secretion of cortisol and growth hormone in patients with major depression. J Psychiatr Res 45, 421426.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morin, V, Hozer, F & Costemale-Lacoste, J-F (2018) The effects of ghrelin on sleep, appetite, and memory, and its possible role in depression: a review of the literature. Enceph-Rev Psychiatr Clin Biol Ther 44, 256263.Google ScholarPubMed
Pereira-Miranda, E, Costa, PRF, Queiroz, VAO, et al. (2017) Overweight and obesity associated with higher depression prevalence in adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J Am Coll Nutr 36, 223233.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pan, A, Keum, N, Okereke, OI, et al. (2012) Bidirectional association between depression and metabolic syndrome a systematic review and meta-analysis of epidemiological studies. Diabetes Care 35, 11711180.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Colle, R, de larminat, D, Rotenberg, S, et al. (2017) Pioglitazone could induce remission in major depression: a meta-analysis. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat 13, 916.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pourmotabbed, A, Moradi, S, Babaei, A, et al. (2020) Food insecurity and mental health: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Public Health Nutr 23, 17781790.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burns, PB, Rohrich, RJ & Chung, KC (2011) The levels of evidence and their role in evidence-based medicine. Plast Reconstr Surg 128, 305310.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roberts, E, Carter, B & Young, AH (2018) Caveat emptor: Folate in unipolar depressive illness, a systematic review and meta-analysis. J Psychopharmacol (Oxf) 32, 377384. https://doi.org/10.1177/0269881118756060 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cartwright, N (2011) A philosopher’s view of the long road from RCTs to effectiveness. The Lancet 377, 14001401.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Higgins, JPT (2008) Commentary: heterogeneity in meta-analysis should be expected and appropriately quantified. Int J Epidemiol 37, 11581160.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koopman, M & El Aidy, S (2017) Depressed gut? The microbiota-diet-inflammation trialogue in depression. Curr Opin Psychiatry 30, 369377.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lach, G, Schellekens, H, Dinan, TG, et al. (2018) Anxiety, depression, and the microbiome: a role for gut peptides. Neurotherapeutics 15, 3659.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lang, UE, Beglinger, C, Schweinfurth, N, et al. (2015) Nutritional aspects of depression. Cell Physiol Biochem 37, 10291043.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Logan, AC (2015) Dysbiotic drift: mental health, environmental grey space, and microbiota. J Physiol Anthropol 34, 23.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scrinis, G (2013) The era of good-and-bad nutritionism. In Nutritionism, The Science and Politics of Dietary Advice, pp. 7398. New York: Columbia University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yang, Z, Li, J, Gui, X, et al. (2020) Updated review of research on the gut microbiota and their relation to depres sion in animals and human beings. Mol Psychiatry 25, 27592772.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Valles-Colomer, M, Falony, G, Darzi, Y, et al. (2019) The neuroactive potential of the human gut microbiota in quality of life and depression. Nat Microbiol 4, 623632.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wong, M-L, Inserra, A, Lewis, MD, et al. (2016) Inflammasome signaling affects anxiety- and depressive-like behavior and gut microbiome composition. Mol Psychiatry 21, 797805.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sun, J, Wang, F, Hu, X, et al. (2018) Clostridium butyricum attenuates chronic unpredictable mild stress-induced depressive-like behavior in mice via the gut-brain axis. J Agric Food Chem 66, 84158421.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scrinis, G (2013) The era of functional nutritionism. In Nutritionism, The Science and Politics of Dietary Advice, pp. 157190. New York: Columbia University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Noble, EE, Hsu, TM & Kanoski, SE (2017) Gut to brain dysbiosis: mechanisms linking western diet consumption, the microbiome, and cognitive impairment. Front Behav Neurosci 11, 9. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2017.00009 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fried, EI & Cramer, AOJ (2017) Moving forward: challenges and directions for psychopathological network theory and methodology. Perspect Psychol Sci 12, 9991020.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Epskamp, S, Cramer, AOJ, Waldorp, LJ, et al. (2012) qgraph: network visualizations of relationships in psychometric data. J Stat Softw 48, 118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bentall, RP (2003) Chapter 9: Madness and emotion. In Madness Explained. Manchester: Allen Lane.Google Scholar
Yang, X, Ram, N, Gest, SD, et al. 2018. Socioemotional Dynamics of Emotion Regulation and Depressive Symptoms: A Person-Specific Network Approach [WWW Document]. Complexity. https://www.hindawi.com/journals/complexity/2018/5094179/ (accessed January 2020).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jacka, FN, Cherbuin, N, Anstey, KJ, et al. (2014) Dietary patterns and depressive symptoms over time: examining the relationships with socioeconomic position, health behaviours and cardiovascular risk. PLOS ONE 9, e87657.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alegria, M, NeMoyer, A, Bague, IF, et al. (2018) Social determinants of mental health: where we are and where we need to go. Curr Psychiatry Rep 20, 95.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dixon, J (2016) Critical nutrition studies within critical agrarian studies: a review and analysis. J Peasant Stud 43, 11121120.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Patel, R, Kerr, RB, Shumba, L, et al. (2015) Cook, eat, man, woman: understanding the New Alliance for Food Security and Nutrition, nutritionism and its alternatives from Malawi. J Peasant Stud 42, 2144.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
VanderWeele, TJ & Robins, JM (2007) Four types of effect modification: a classification based on directed acyclic graphs. Epidemiology 18, 561568.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hummel, E & Hoffmann, I (2016) Complexity of nutritional behavior: capturing and depicting its interrelated factors in a cause-effect model. Ecol Food Nutr 55, 241257.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Christakis, NA & Fowler, JH (2007) The spread of obesity in a large social network over 32 years. N Engl J Med 357, 370379.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Valente, T, Fujimoto, K, Chou, C, et al. (2009) Adolescent affiliations and adiposity: a social network analysis of friendships and obesity. J Adolesc Health 45, 202204.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Forney, K (2019) Examining similarities in eating pathology, negative affect, and perfectionism among peers: a social network analysis. Appetite 137, 236243.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simone, M, Long, E & Lockhart, G (2018) The dynamic relationship between unhealthy weight control and adolescent friendships: a social network approach. J Youth Adolesc 47, 13731384.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
VanderWeele, TJ, Hernán, MA & Robins, JM (2008) Causal directed acyclic graphs and the direction of unmeasured confounding bias. Epidemiology 19, 720728.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Borsboom, D & Cramer, AOJ (2013) Network analysis: an integrative approach to the structure of psychopathology. Annu Rev Clin Psychol 9, 91121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lunansky, G, van Borkulo, C & Borsboom, D (2021) Personality, resilience, and psychopathology: a model for the interaction between slow and fast network processes in the context of mental health. Eur J Personal 34, 969987.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McElroy, E, McIntyre, JC, Bentall, RP, et al. (2019) Mental health, deprivation, and the neighborhood social environment: a network analysis. Clin Psychol Sci 7, 719734.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aalbers, G, McNally, RJ, Heeren, A, et al. (2019) Social media and depression symptoms: a network perspective. J Exp Psychol-Gen 148, 14541462.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Fig. 1. (a) Nutritional components (food GI, energy intake and diet variety) and depression symptoms (anergia, low mood and loss of pleasure) are depicted as network nodes. Within these two categories, straight lines identify where nodes within each category are likely to co-occur. Arrows depict causal interactions between nutritional components and depression symptoms. The thickness of arrows depicts the strength of interactions. (b) The same network as in (1a) is shown. Here, however, causal interactions between nutritional components and depression symptoms have been reduced to present/absent interactions in a simplified, binary network. GI, glycaemic index.

Figure 1

Fig. 2. In the above example, the network node ‘social isolation’ is causally related via network edge to anergia and diet variety. One can envisage the knock-on effect this would have on loss of pleasure in particular.