Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-vvkck Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-25T11:22:09.997Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Memory profiles predict dementia over 23–28 years in normal but not successful aging

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  25 November 2019

Maria Josefsson*
Affiliation:
Centre for Demographic and Ageing Research at Umeå University (CEDAR), Umeå, Sweden
Anna Sundström
Affiliation:
Centre for Demographic and Ageing Research at Umeå University (CEDAR), Umeå, Sweden Department of Psychology, Umeå University, Umeå, Sweden
Sara Pudas
Affiliation:
Department of Integrative Medical Biology, Umeå University, Umeå, Sweden Umeå Center for Functional Brain Imaging, Umeå, Sweden
Annelie Nordin Adolfsson
Affiliation:
Department of Clinical Sciences, Psychiatry, Umeå University, Umeå, Sweden
Lars Nyberg
Affiliation:
Department of Integrative Medical Biology, Umeå University, Umeå, Sweden Umeå Center for Functional Brain Imaging, Umeå, Sweden Department of Radiation Sciences, Umeå University, Umeå, Sweden
Rolf Adolfsson
Affiliation:
Department of Clinical Sciences, Psychiatry, Umeå University, Umeå, Sweden
*
Correspondence should be addressed to: Maria Josefsson, Centre for Demographic and Ageing Research, Umeå University, 901 87 Umeå, Sweden. Phone: +46 (0)90 786 67 78. Email: maria.josefsson@umu.se.
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Objectives:

Prospective studies suggest that memory deficits are detectable decades before clinical symptoms of dementia emerge. However, individual differences in long-term memory trajectories prior to diagnosis need to be further elucidated. The aim of the current study was to investigate long-term dementia and mortality risk for individuals with different memory trajectory profiles in a well-characterized population-based sample.

Methods:

1062 adults (aged 45–80 years) who were non-demented at baseline were followed over 23–28 years. Dementia and mortality risk were studied for three previously classified episodic memory trajectory groups: maintained high performance (Maintainers; 26%), average decline (Averages; 64%), and accelerated decline (Decliners; 12%), using multistate modeling to characterize individuals’ transitions from an initial non-demented state, possibly to a state of dementia and/or death.

Results:

The memory groups showed considerable intergroup variability in memory profiles, starting 10–15 years prior to dementia diagnosis, and prior to death. A strong relationship between memory trajectory group and dementia risk was found. Specifically, Decliners had more than a fourfold risk of developing dementia compared to Averages. In contrast, Maintainers had a 2.6 times decreased dementia risk compared to Averages, and in addition showed no detectable memory decline prior to dementia diagnosis. A similar pattern of association was found for the memory groups and mortality risk, although only among non-demented.

Conclusion:

There was a strong relationship between accelerated memory decline and dementia, further supporting the prognostic value of memory decline. The intergroup differences, however, suggest that mechanisms involved in successful memory aging may delay symptom onset.

Type
Original Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
© International Psychogeriatric Association 2019

Introduction

Several prospective studies suggest that memory deficits are detectable decades before clinical symptoms of dementia emerge (Boraxbekk et al., Reference Boraxbekk, Lundquist, Nordin, Nyberg, Nilsson and Adolfsson2015; Elias et al., Reference Elias, Beiser, Wolf, Au, White and D’agostino2000; Rajan et al., Reference Rajan, Wilson, Weuve, Barnes and Evans2015). This long preclinical stage provides a critical window for intervention and suggests that thorough neuropsychological assessments are important for early detection. Some studies have observed accelerated decline in multiple cognitive functions in the prodromal phase, starting 3–6 years before clinical symptoms (Albert et al., Reference Albert2014; Amieva et al., Reference Amieva2004; Cloutier et al., Reference Cloutier, Chertkow, Kergoat, Gauthier and Belleville2015; Li et al., Reference Li2017; Wilson et al., Reference Wilson, Leurgans, Boyle and Bennett2011). These studies have followed initially cognitively normal individuals longitudinally and examined if an accelerated cognitive rate of change precedes clinical diagnosis. Although previous studies have focused on the nature of memory change in relation to dementia diagnosis, individual differences in longer-term memory trajectories prior to diagnosis need to be further elucidated. Studying the relationship between memory decline and dementia is complicated by the presence of competing events, that is, any event that precludes the event of interest from occurring. For example, in older populations, mortality is a competing event to dementia. An underlying assumption in classic survival analysis methods, such as Cox proportional hazards regression, is that death is unrelated to dementia risk. However, dementia and death share many risk factors, thus, the competing risk of death may bias the estimation of the true effects if it is not considered (Chang et al., Reference Chang, Zhao, Lee and Ganguli2012; Marzona et al., Reference Marzona2016; Prince et al., Reference Prince, Albanese, Guerchet and Prina2014). The multistate model (MSM) has been developed for the purpose of examine longitudinal time-to-event data of competing events (Austin et al., Reference Austin, Lee and Fine2016). While the theory of MSMs has advanced substantially over the last several years, only a few studies are available in cognitive aging research (Marioni et al., Reference Marioni, Valenzuela, Van den Hout, Brayne and Matthews2012; Rouanet et al., Reference Rouanet, Joly, Dartigues, Proust-Lima and Jacqmin-Gadda2016). In previous studies of the Betula prospective cohort study (Nilsson et al., Reference Nilsson1997), we demonstrated longitudinal evidence suggesting an average reduction in episodic memory after the age of 60 years (Rönnlund et al., Reference Rönnlund, Nyberg, Bäckman and Nilsson2005). However, there are large interindividual differences (Habib et al., Reference Habib, Nyberg and Nilsson2007; Josefsson et al., Reference Josefsson, de Luna, Pudas, Nilsson and Nyberg2012). In the current study, we extend previous work by investigating the association between longitudinal memory profiles, in three previously identified (Josefsson et al., Reference Josefsson, de Luna, Pudas, Nilsson and Nyberg2012) episodic memory trajectory groups, and the risk for dementia, dementia-free death, or death among demented. We apply a MSM where individuals pass from an initial healthy state, possibly through a dementia state, to the final absorbing state death. In addition, we compare memory performance trajectories with concurrent changes in mini-mental state examination (MMSE) (Folstein et al., Reference Folstein, Folstein and McHugh1975) prior to dementia onset and prior to death.

Methods

The Betula study

The Betula study is a longitudinal population-based prospective study initiated in 1988, with the objectives to examine health and neurocognitive functions during adult life and old age, and to determine risk factors and preclinical signs of dementia (Nilsson et al., Reference Nilsson1997; Reference Nilsson, Adolfsson, Bäckman, de Frias, Molander and Nyberg2004). Participants considered for the present study were enrolled at the first (1988–1990) and second (1993–1995) wave of data collection, from samples 1 (n = 1000) and 3 (n = 966), evenly sex and age distributed over 10 age cohorts ranging from 35 to 85 years of age. Cognitive testing and health assessments have been conducted every 5 years and adjacent to each of the test waves, the occurrence of dementia was determined for each individual. The study was approved by the Regional Ethical Review Board in Umeå and written consent for study participation was obtained from each participant.

Dementia diagnosis assessments

In the Betula study, presence of dementia has been evaluated adjacent to each test wave, most recently in 2016/2017. Diagnoses were ascertained through a process based on diverse sources of information including reading of medical records from multiple clinical disciplines, substantially from the primary care, medicine, neurology, and geriatric clinics within the county. Thus, other clinicians' assessments, information on medical and social status, medication history, results from available neuroradiological examinations, and evident clinical signs of neurocognitive impairment were integrated in the assessment of determining possible dementia. For the study participants participating in the health and cognitive test assessments, the dementia evaluation also included collateral information from the neuropsychological testing and health examinations. Dementia was not diagnosed unless the disease was characterized by a progressive course. Moreover, for study participants participating in the health and cognitive test assessments, the dementia evaluation also included the test staff's observations, significant drop in performance/low test results on cognitive tests and MMSE scores, as well as the study participants’ subjective perception of memory failure and/or concern of dementia.

The described diagnostic procedure was performed for each participant at baseline to rule out presence of dementia, and every 5 years in order to follow the expected progressive course of previously ascertained diagnosis, and furthermore to identify new cases with dementia and determine the year of disease onset. Disease onset was defined as the time at which an individual fulfilled the core criteria for dementia, that is, when the clinical symptoms became sufficiently severe to interfere with social functioning and instrumental activities of daily living (McKhann et al., Reference McKhann2011). The same research geropsychiatrist (co-author R. A.) coordinated the diagnostic evaluation throughout the study period and was responsible for the final diagnoses, which were defined according to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th edition (DSM-IV) criteria (American Psychiatric Association, 2000).

Cognitive measures

For the present study, two assessments were used to characterize cognitive function. First, a composite episodic memory score (EMS). The sum of scores on five different episodic memory tasks: (1) immediate free recall of 16 visually and orally presented short sentences, (2) delayed cued recall of nouns from the previously presented sentences, (3) immediate free recall of 16 enacted sentences, (4) delayed cued recall of nouns from the enacted sentences, and (5) immediate free recall of a list of 12 orally presented nouns (details are found in Nilsson et al., Reference Nilsson1997). The EMS score could range from 0 (minimum) to 76 (maximum). Second, MMSE (Folstein, Folstein and McHugh, Reference Folstein, Folstein and McHugh1975), a screening instrument commonly used to assess general cognitive dysfunction.

Memory trajectory groups

The 1558 participants from the original Betula samples S1 and S3, who completed two or more test sessions, were previously classified as having maintained high episodic memory performance (“Maintainers”; 18%), average decline (“Averages”; 68%), or accelerated decline (“Decliners”; 13%). The model was based on their performance on up to five repeated EMS over 15–20 years and corrected for dropout. Individuals were classified as having episodic memory decline/maintenance if their rates of change fell below/above 1 standard deviation (SD) of a model prediction that accounted for their age and individual baseline performance (Josefsson et al., Reference Josefsson, de Luna, Pudas, Nilsson and Nyberg2012; Pudas et al., Reference Pudas, Persson, Josefsson, de Luna, Nilsson and Nyberg2013). For individuals diagnosed with dementia, only EMS prior to diagnosis were considered.

APOE genotyping

Apolipoprotein E (APOE) genotypes were determined by a polymerase chain reaction. For details of the procedure, see Josefsson et al. (Reference Josefsson, Larsson, Nordin, Adolfsson and Olofsson2017).

Statistical analysis

We used linear mixed models (Fitzmaurice et al., Reference Fitzmaurice, Davidian, Verbeke and Molenberghs2008) to analyze the association between the memory groups and 15–20 years memory change prior to dementia diagnosis or death. Fixed-effect terms included an overall intercept, indicator variables for the decline and maintained-memory groups, plus interactions with time measured in years to diagnosis or death. For the non-demented and non-deceased group, the end point was considered the last test occasion at the fifth wave. In addition, age, age-squared and an indicator for the first testing occasion were included as a fixed effect to account for practice effects. The model also included a subject-specific random intercept, to account for within-subject correlations between repeated measurements. To characterize the association between dementia, mortality, and memory trajectory groups, we adopted a MSM using age as the timescale. In the present study, we considered the two MSMs that are illustrated in Figure 1. First, the illness-death model in which each subject starts in the healthy state and depending on event, transition to either a state of dementia, considering both Alzheimer’s disease (AD) and vascular dementia (VaD), or to the final absorbing state death. If a subject is first diagnosed with dementia, they can also transition to the final state death. In the second model, AD and VaD were treated as two separate states as preliminary analyses indicated that there were some differences in memory trajectories and predictors between the groups. For each permitted transition in the MSM, there is a corresponding time-to-event model, with cause-specific hazard rates (HR). Subject-specific characteristics included in the model are: binary indicator variables for Maintainers and Decliners, sex, carrier of the APOE ε4 allele, and low education level, corresponding to below median years of education. We employ a semi-parametric MSM where the hazards are assumed to be proportional and each transition has its own baseline hazard and specific covariate effects. Preliminary analysis revealed a violation of the proportional hazards assumption for the Maintainer variable. To overcome this problem, we performed median-split age-stratified analyses. Considering an old age cohort and an old–old age cohort, below 87 years of age and 87 or older, respectively, at the study end. An alpha level of 0.05 was considered statistically significant. Analyses were carried out using R (R Core Team, 2017). Mixed models were fitted using the lme4 package. p-values were calculated using Satterthwaite’s approximation (Goodnight, Reference Goodnight1978) implemented in the lmerTest package. The mstate package was used to fit the MSMs to the data.

Figure 1. Graphical representation of the multistate models. (Panel A) The illness-death model. (Panel B) Each subject starts in the healthy state. From there, they can possibly either be diagnosed with Alzheimer’s (AD) or vascular dementia (VaD), or die. A subject first diagnosed with dementia, they can also transition to death.

Study sample

The inclusion criteria for the present study were (1) completing the episodic memory testing at two or more test sessions, in order to have been classified into a memory trajectory group, (2) undergoing APOE genotyping, and (3) the expected age at the study end to be 70 or older. The oldest cohort (n = 14), aged 85 years at enrollment, was excluded to avoid healthy survivor bias. Further, participants with other dementia diagnoses than AD and VaD were excluded. Specifically, Lewy body dementia (n = 6), dementia due to Parkinson’s disease (n = 5), dementia not otherwise specified (n = 14), frontotemporal dementia (n = 2), and progressive supranuclear paralysis (n = 1).

Results

Study population

A total of 1062 participants met the inclusion criteria for this study, 55% of whom were females. The mean age of participants at enrollment was 61.5 years (SD 10.0). During the follow-up period, 188 individuals developed dementia (AD, n = 113 and VaD, n = 75), 407 died without dementia diagnosis, and among those who developed dementia 141 (75%) died during the study period (AD, n = 77 and VaD, n = 64). Remaining 467 individuals were non-demented/non-deceased at the study end point. Of the included participants, 683 participants (64%) were previously classified (Josefsson et al., Reference Josefsson, de Luna, Pudas, Nilsson and Nyberg2012) as having an average episodic memory decline (Averages), 256 (26%) as having maintained memory performance (Maintainers), and 123 (12%) as having accelerated memory decline (Decliners). The mean age at onset of dementia was 81.5 years for Averages, whereas Decliners were 4.0 years younger (p < 0.001), and Maintainers were 4.1 years older (p = 0.001). For the deceased, non-dementedgroup, median age at event of death was 81.0 years for Averages, Decliners were 2.7 years younger (p = 0.088), and Maintainers were 2.8 years older (p = 0.004). There was no significant difference in proportions of Maintainers, Averages, and Decliners between AD and VaD (p = 0.725), and no significant differences for the memory groups in age at onset between AD and VaD. Thus, having a more beneficial memory trajectory was consistently associated with better outcomes in terms of older age at disease onset or death. The characteristics of the analytic sample, divided by memory groups, are shown in Table 1.

Table 1. Study participants demographic, cognitive, and health data across memory groups. n (%), mean ± standard deviation

Abbreviations: AD, Alzheimer’s disease; VaD, vascular dementia.

Memory trajectories prior to dementia onset or death

The study participants were divided into four groups according to their follow-up status, (1) AD group; individuals who developed AD within the studied period, (2) VaD group; individuals who developed VaD within the study period, (3) A group of deceased individuals who died without the presence of dementia during the studied period, and (4) Healthy group; non-demented and non-deceased individuals at the study end point in 2016. A set of linear mixed models were further fitted to assess memory trajectory profiles, in EMS and MMSE, for each of the four groups. The memory trajectory groups showed only minor discrepancies in MMSE over time. That is, similar performance up to less than 5 years prior to onset or death, and nonsignificant memory group differences in decline. However, in stark contrast the results for EMS revealed considerable inter-individual variability in cognitive profiles prior to dementia onset, for both AD and VaD, and prior to death (see Figure 2). Notably, Maintainers had a high, stable memory performance prior to AD diagnosis and prior to death. Prior to VaD diagnosis, the Maintainer group revealed an incipient decline. Both Averages and Decliners revealed a decline in EMS starting 10–15 years prior to dementia diagnosis or event of death, where Decliners showed an accelerated decline compared to Averages. The rates of change did not differ significantly for AD and VaD across the cognitive profiles. In the healthy group, Averages and Decliners revealed a decline in memory performance over time, Averages although at a higher level and showing less steep decline. In comparison, Maintainers revealed a stable performance over time.

Figure 2. Predicted episodic memory score (EMS) trajectories stratified by memory groups. Parameters are derived from mixed models after controlling for age and practice effects. (Panel A) Dementia, AD: individuals who developed AD within the studied period. (Panel B) Dementia, VaD: individuals who developed VaD within the studied period. (Panel C) Dementia-free deceased group: non-demented individuals who died during the studied period. (Panel D) Healthy: non-demented and non-deceased individuals at the last test wave.

Multistate model

We used a MSM to investigate differences in the progression to dementia and/or death for the memory groups. Two sets of models were fitted; first a model considering dementia (comprising AD and VaD together), and a second model considering AD and VaD separately. Stratified analyses were conducted for an old, 71–86 years of age at the study end (n = 519), and an old–old cohort, 87–107 years of age (n = 543). In the old cohort, 74% were non-demented and alive at the study end among Averages, as compared to 81% among Maintainers and 51% among Decliners. In the old–old cohort, corresponding numbers were 15% among Averages, 23% among Maintainers, and 3% among Decliners. The results for the first model (comprising AD and VaD together) are shown in Figure 3. Specifically, in the old cohort, Decliners had a 6.5 times increased risk (95% confidence interval (CI); 3.02–14.40) of developing dementia compared to Averages. Notably, n = 0 of the Maintainers developed dementia. In the old–old cohort, similar patterns were shown. That is, Decliners had a 4.0 times increased risk (95% CI; 2.75–5.88) of developing dementia compared to Averages. Maintainers had a 2.6 times decreased risk (HR = 0.38, 95% CI; 0.23–0.63) of developing dementia compared to Averages. For the transition to a dementia-free death in both the old and the old–old cohort all effects were nonsignificant, Decliners; HR = 1.4, 95% CI; 0.79–2.50 in the old cohort, and HR = 1.2, 95% CI; 0.53–1.41 in the old–old cohort, and Maintainers; HR = 0.86, 95% CI; 0.53–1.41 in the old cohort and HR = 0.8, 95% CI; 0.62–1.02 in the old–old cohort. Further, for all transitions from dementia to a deceased state, the memory trajectory groups had small and nonsignificant effects. Thus, memory group was not significantly associated with mortality. To investigate if the results differed for AD and VaD subgroups, we modeled these groups separately. The results in the old cohort revealed that Decliners had a 6.6 times increased risk (95% CI; 3.02–14.40) of developing AD compared to Averages, and a 6.2 times increased risk (95% CI; 1.83–21.31) of developing VaD. In the old–old cohort, similar patterns were shown. That is, Decliners had a 3.7 times increased risk (95% CI; 2.19–6.15) of developing AD compared to Averages, and a 4.5 times increased risk (95% CI; 2.56–7.87) of developing VaD. Maintainers had a 2.3 times decreased risk (HR = 0.44, 95% CI; 0.24–0.81) of developing AD compared to Averages, while the risk of developing VaD was 3.4 times decreased (HR = 0.29, 95% CI; 0.12–0.70). For the transition from a VaD to a deceased state, Maintainers had a 2.6 times increased risk of death (95% CI; 0.92–7.07) in the old–old cohort. For all other transitions to a deceased state, the memory trajectory groups had small and nonsignificant effects. Thus, only small differences were found for AD and VaD. To further investigate if our findings were affected by other characteristics, we considered a model including the memory groups, as well as, sex, education, and APOE ε4 as predictor variables. For the memory groups, similar results were found as for the second model (treating AD and VaD separately) for the transition to either AD or VaD state. In addition, for the transition from a VaD to deceased state, Maintainers had a 3.0 times increased risk of death (95% CI; 1.01–8.72) in the old–old cohort. All other transitions were small and nonsignificant for the memory groups. APOE ε4 carriers had a 9.0 times increased risk (95% CI; 3.39–24.14) in the old cohort and 3.0 increased risk (95% CI; 1.94–4.59) in the old–old cohort of developing AD compared to noncarriers. The APOE genotype was not significantly associated with any other transitions. Female sex was associated with a 2.8 times (95% CI; 1.14–2.59) and 2.0 times (95% CI; 1.37–2.25) increased risk of developing AD in the old and old–old cohort, respectively. In the old cohort, male sex conferred a significant 1.7 times increased risk of death without dementia (95% CI; 1.14–2.54) and in the old–old cohort a 2.0 times increased risk (95% CI; 1.58–2.51). Hence, among non-demented men died at a younger age than females. For the transition to dementia death, gender had small and nonsignificant effects. The absence of association between the memory groups and death was somewhat surprising. Thus, in a follow-up analysis, we analyzed if similar patterns were found for the subset of only non-demented individuals (through the studied period) using ordinary Cox regression. The results showed a significant association between the memory groups and death only in the old–old cohort, Decliners; HR = 1.6, 95% CI; 0.90–2.86, in the old cohort, and HR = 2.03, 95% CI; 1.30–3.16, in the old–old cohort, Maintainers; HR = 0.82, 95% CI; 0.50–1.34 in the old cohort, and HR = 0.73, 95% CI; 0.57–0.93 in the old–old cohort.

Figure 3. Hazard rates (HR) and 95% confidence intervals (95% CIs) from the multistate model for transitions from a healthy state to a demented and further to a deceased state, or a dementia-free deceased state. (Panel A) Old cohort and (Panel B) Old–old cohort.

Discussion

This population-based study investigated long-term dementia outcomes for three memory trajectory groups (Maintainers, Averages, and Decliners) in a well-characterized population-based sample of older adults who were non-demented at baseline. The main finding was a strong relationship between longitudinal change in episodic memory performance and dementia risk. For those who developed dementia, this could generally be traced in cognitive patterns 10–15 years before clinical diagnosis. In contrast, only little decline was observed in MMSE during the same period, suggesting that broad screening measures, such as MMSE, lack the sensitivity to detect or follow the course of the preclinical phase of dementia. Importantly, the results highlight the heterogeneity of cognitive trajectories in older adults, and a significant proportion of elderly showed evidence of successful cognitive aging, which is consistent with findings from past studies (Hayden et al., Reference Hayden2014; Muniz Terrera et al., Reference Muniz Terrera, Brayne and Matthews2010; Nyberg and Pudas, Reference Nyberg and Pudas2018; Zahodne et al., Reference Zahodne2015). Furthermore, the Maintainers showed virtually no signs of memory impairment as individuals’ progress toward dementia, suggesting that the absence of quantifiable cognitive decline does not necessarily mean protection against dementia. These results are in line with the theory of cognitive reserve (Stern, Reference Stern2002). Specifically, cognitive reserve is thought to delay the onset of symptoms despite accumulating pathology, with a more rapid decline once pathology reaches a critical threshold. In the current study, individuals in the Maintainers group also had a higher education level, which is the most common proxy for cognitive reserve, than Decliners and Averages, further supporting the theory. Another complementary theory suggests that brain maintenance postpones brain pathology, rather than coping with its presence (Nyberg et al., Reference Nyberg, Lövdén, Riklund, Lindenberger and Bäckman2012). Nevertheless, more research is needed to confirm and further characterize distinguishing characteristics of this high-performing group. In contrast, the Decliner group revealed an accelerated decline in cognitive function starting a decade before dementia onset. Notably, in the old–old cohort, only a small proportion (3%) in the group with accelerated decline was non-demented and alive at the study end. These results highlight the strong predictive relationship between cognitive decline and unfavorable aging outcomes. These findings, based on unusually long follow-up, confirm that accelerated episodic memory impairment provides an early indication of dementia and shed light on the time course of impairment. In addition, as expected, the Decliners were significantly more often APOE ε4 carriers and had lower education level, suggesting these factors modulates individuals’ memory trajectories in their progression to dementia, which is in line with previous studies (Amieva et al., Reference Amieva2004; Chang et al., Reference Chang, Zhao, Lee and Ganguli2012). This study is innovative in its design in that it uses a multistate modeling approach that gives a more precise and complete picture of the course of the disease. A surprising finding was that episodic memory decline was not a risk factor for the progression to death. This may be explained by that the MSM approach considers all individuals, demented as well as non-demented prior to onset, as opposed to ordinary Cox regression where usually only a subset of individuals who remain non-demented throughout the study period is considered. In a follow-up analysis of only non-demented individuals, we found an association between the memory groups and the risk of death, which are in line with previous studies (e.g. Laukka et al., Reference Laukka, MacDonald and Bäckman2006; Laukka et al., Reference Laukka, MacDonald and Bäckman2008; Thorvaldsson et al., Reference Thorvaldsson, Hofer, Berg, Skoog, Sacuiu and Johansson2008). These results suggest that accelerated terminal decline is driven by non-demented individuals and that episodic memory decline is less prognostic for mortality among demented, who are all already affected with more advanced forms of decline. We do, however, observe large heterogeneity in terminal decline trajectories among non-demented, that is, similar heterogeneity in trajectory patterns that were observed prior to dementia onset was also observed in the time preceding dementia-free death. These findings are in line with previous work suggesting that person-specific factors such as genetics and comorbidity may contribute to differences in trajectories prior to death (Wilson et al., Reference Wilson, Beck, Bienias and Bennett2007; Fabbri et al., Reference Fabbri2016; Gerstorf and Ram, Reference Gerstorf and Ram2013). We extend previous studies of competing risks for dementia and death (Li et al., Reference Li2017; Prince et al., Reference Prince, Albanese, Guerchet and Prina2014) by studying two types of dementia disorders, AD and VaD. Although, similar associations were found between the memory groups and AD or VaD risk, the predictors of AD and VaD were different, where female gender and APOE ε4 were risk factors for AD but not for VaD. Our results demonstrate that there are marked similarities in memory profiles between the disorders along with some differences with regard to strong risk factors, which is in line with previous studies (Karantzoulis and Galvin, Reference Karantzoulis and Galvin2011; Looi and Sachdev, Reference Looi and Sachdev1999; Pilon et al., Reference Pilon2016). Thus, a compound analysis may bias the estimation of true covariate effects. Moreover, in this study, we saw a somewhat higher prevalence of VaD (7.1%) than usually is observed in population-based studies. The population in northern Sweden is characterized by high serum cholesterol levels, probably reflecting a higher intake of saturated fat, and a high consumption of smokeless tobacco. These factors may explain a substantial part of the increased risk in coronary heart disease morbidity and mortality found in previous studies (Huhtasaari et al., Reference Huhtasaari, Asplund and Wester1988), and is a possible explanation for higher prevalence of VaD in this cohort.

In this study, diagnoses of dementia were ascertained through reading of medical records from multiple clinical disciplines. The diagnostic assessment also included collateral information from the health and cognitive assessments and predefined criteria for identifying individuals with potential increased risk of dementia. The method used has its limitations and may lead to an underestimation of the number of demented individuals, especially mild cases, and individuals with good cognitive reserve. However, diagnoses were evaluated at repeated occasions in order to establish a reliable dementia status and time of onset, minimizing the risk of misclassification. Importantly, before these results might have clinical relevance, it is necessary to evaluate the predictive performance of episodic memory decline in a clinical setting.

Conflicts of interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interests.

Funding

Financial support was provided through a regional agreement between Umeå University and Västerbotten County Council (ALF).

Description of authors’ roles

Study conception and design: MJ, LN, RA. Dementia diagnostics: RA. Data analysis interpretation of data: MJ, AS, SP. Drafting of manuscript: MJ, AS, SP, ANA. Critical revision: MJ, AS, SP, ANA, LN, RA.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by the Swedish Research Council for Health, Working Life and Welfare, and is part of the program Paths to Healthy and Active Ageing (Dnr 2013 – 2056). The Betula Project is supported by Knut and Alice Wallenberg foundation (KAW) and the Swedish Research Council (K2010-61X-21446-01).

References

Albert, M. et al. (2014). Cognitive changes preceding clinical symptom onset of mild cognitive impairment and relationship to ApoE genotype. Current Alzheimer Research, 11, 773784.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders-IV-TR. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.Google Scholar
Amieva, H. et al. (2004). Annual rate and predictors of conversion to dementia in subjects presenting mild cognitive impairment criteria defined according to a population-based study. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 18, 8793.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Austin, P.C., Lee, D. and Fine, J.P. (2016). Introduction to the analysis of survival data in the presence of competing risks. Circulation, 133, 601609.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boraxbekk, C.J., Lundquist, A., Nordin, S., Nyberg, L., Nilsson, L.G. and Adolfsson, R. (2015). Free recall episodic memory performance predicts dementia ten years prior to clinical diagnosis, findings from the Betula longitudinal study. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders Extra, 5, 191202.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chang, C.-C.H., Zhao, Y., Lee, C.-W. and Ganguli, M. (2012). Smoking, death, and Alzheimer’s disease: a case of competing risks. Alzheimer Disease and Associated Disorders, 26, 300.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cloutier, S., Chertkow, H., Kergoat, M.J., Gauthier, S. and Belleville, S. (2015). Patterns of cognitive decline prior to dementia in persons with mild cognitive impairment. Journal of Alzheimer Disease, 47, 901913.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elias, M.F., Beiser, A., Wolf, P.A., Au, R., White, R.F. and D’agostino, R.B. (2000). The preclinical phase of Alzheimer disease: a 22-year prospective study of the Framingham Cohort. Archives of Neurology, 57, 808813.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fabbri, E. et al. (2016). Association of accelerated multi-morbidity and age-related cognitive decline in older, non-demented participants from the Baltimore longitudinal study of aging. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 64, 965972.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fitzmaurice, G., Davidian, M., Verbeke, G. and Molenberghs, G. (2008). Longitudinal Data Analysis. Boca Raton, FL: Chapman & Hall/CRC.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Folstein, M.F., Folstein, S.E. and McHugh, P.R. (1975). “Mini-mental state”: a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12, 189198.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gerstorf, D. and Ram, N. (2013). Inquiry into terminal decline: five objectives for future study. Gerontologist, 53, 727737.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodnight, J.H. (1978). Tests of Hypotheses in Fixed-Effects Linear Models. SAS Technical Report R-101. Cary, NC: SAS Institute Inc.Google Scholar
Habib, R., Nyberg, L. and Nilsson, L.-G. (2007). Cognitive and non-cognitive factors contributing to the longitudinal identification of successful older adults in the Betula study. Aging Neuropsychology and Cognition, 14, 257273.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hayden, K M. et al. (2014). Pre-clinical cognitive phenotypes for Alzheimer disease: a latent profile approach. The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 22, 13641374.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huhtasaari, F., Asplund, K. and Wester, P.O. (1988). Cardiovascular risk factors in the Northern Sweden MONICA Study. Acta Medica Scandinavica, 224, 99108.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Josefsson, M., de Luna, X., Pudas, S., Nilsson, L.-G. and Nyberg, L. (2012). Genetic and lifestyle predictors of 15-year longitudinal change in episodic memory. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 60, 23082312.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Josefsson, M., Larsson, M., Nordin, S., Adolfsson, R. and Olofsson, J. (2017). APOE-ε4 effects on longitudinal decline in olfactory and non-olfactory cognitive abilities in middle-aged and old adults. Scientific Reports, 7, 1286.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Karantzoulis, S. and Galvin, J.E. (2011). Distinguishing Alzheimer’s disease from other major forms of dementia. Expert Review of Neurotherapeutics, 11, 15791591.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Laukka, E.J., MacDonald, S.W. and Bäckman, L. (2006). Contrasting cognitive trajectories of impending death and preclinical dementia in the very old. Neurology, 66, 833838.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Laukka, E.J., MacDonald, S.W. and Bäckman, L. (2008). Terminal-decline effects for select cognitive tasks after controlling for preclinical dementia. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 16, 355365.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Li, G. et al. (2017). Cognitive trajectory changes over 20 years before Dementia diagnosis: a large cohort study. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 65, 26272633.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Looi, J.C. and Sachdev, P.S. (1999). Differentiation of vascular dementia from AD on neuropsychological tests. Neurology, 53, 670670.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marioni, R.E., Valenzuela, M.J., Van den Hout, A., Brayne, C. and Matthews, F.E. (2012). Active cognitive lifestyle is associated with positive cognitive health transitions and compression of morbidity from age sixty-five. PLoS One, 7, e50940.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marzona, I. et al. (2016). Risk of dementia and death in patients with atrial fibrillation: a competing risk analysis of a population-based cohort. International Journal of Cardiology, 220, 440444.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McKhann, G M. et al. (2011). The diagnosis of dementia due to Alzheimer’s disease: recommendations from the National Institute on Aging Alzheimer’s Association workgroups on diagnostics guidelines for Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimer's & Dementia, 7, 263269.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muniz Terrera, G., Brayne, C., Matthews, F. and the CC75C Study Collaboration Group. (2010). One size fits all? Why we need more sophisticated analytical methods in the explanation of trajectories of cognition in older age and their potential risk factors. International Psychogeriatrics, 22, 291299.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nilsson, L.-G. et al. (1997). The Betula prospective cohort study: memory, health, and aging. Aging Neuropsychology and Cognition, 4, 132.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nilsson, L.-G., Adolfsson, R., Bäckman, L., de Frias, C.M., Molander, B. and Nyberg, L. (2004). Betula: a prospective cohort study on memory, health and aging. Aging Neuropsychology and Cognition, 11, 134148.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nyberg, L., Lövdén, M., Riklund, K., Lindenberger, U. and Bäckman, L. (2012). Memory aging and brain maintenance. Trends in Cognitive Science, 16, 292305.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nyberg, L. and Pudas, S. (2018). Successful memory aging. Annual Review of Psychology, 70, 219243.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pilon, M.-H. et al. (2016). Differences in rate of cognitive decline and caregiver burden between Alzheimer’s disease and vascular dementia: a retrospective study. Neurology (E-Cronicon), 2, 278.Google ScholarPubMed
Prince, M., Albanese, E., Guerchet, M. and Prina, M. (2014). World Alzheimer Report 2014: Dementia and Risk Reduction: An Analysis of Protective and Modifiable Risk Factors. London: Alzheimer Disease International.Google Scholar
Pudas, S., Persson, J., Josefsson, M., de Luna, X., Nilsson, L.-G. and Nyberg, L. (2013). Brain characteristics of individuals resisting age-related cognitive decline over two decades. Journal of Neuroscience, 33, 86688677.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
R Core Team. (2017). R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.Google Scholar
Rajan, K.B., Wilson, R.S., Weuve, J., Barnes, L.L. and Evans, D.A. (2015). Cognitive impairment 18 years before clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer disease dementia. Neurology, 85, 898904.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rönnlund, M., Nyberg, L., Bäckman, L. and Nilsson, L.-G. (2005). Stability, growth, and decline in adult life span development of declarative memory: cross-sectional and longitudinal data from a population-based study. Psychology and Aging, 20, 318.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rouanet, A., Joly, P., Dartigues, J.F., Proust-Lima, C. and Jacqmin-Gadda, H. (2016). Joint latent class model for longitudinal data and interval-censored semi-competing events: application to dementia. Biometrics, 72, 11231135.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stern, Y. (2002). What is cognitive reserve? Theory and research application of the reserve concept. Journal of International Neuropsychological Society, 8, 448460.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thorvaldsson, V., Hofer, S.M., Berg, S., Skoog, I., Sacuiu, S. and Johansson, B. (2008). Onset of terminal decline in cognitive abilities in individuals without dementia. Neurology, 71, 882887.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilson, R.S., Beck, T.L., Bienias, J.L. and Bennett, D.A. (2007). Terminal cognitive decline: accelerated loss of cognition in the last years of life. Psychosomatic Medicine, 69, 131137.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilson, R.S., Leurgans, S.E., Boyle, P.A. and Bennett, D.A. (2011). Cognitive decline in prodromal Alzheimer disease and mild cognitive impairment. Archives of Neurology, 68, 351356.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zahodne, L.B. et al. (2015) Late-life memory trajectories in relation to incident dementia and regional brain atrophy. Journal of Neurology, 262, 24842490.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Figure 1. Graphical representation of the multistate models. (Panel A) The illness-death model. (Panel B) Each subject starts in the healthy state. From there, they can possibly either be diagnosed with Alzheimer’s (AD) or vascular dementia (VaD), or die. A subject first diagnosed with dementia, they can also transition to death.

Figure 1

Table 1. Study participants demographic, cognitive, and health data across memory groups. n (%), mean ± standard deviation

Figure 2

Figure 2. Predicted episodic memory score (EMS) trajectories stratified by memory groups. Parameters are derived from mixed models after controlling for age and practice effects. (Panel A) Dementia, AD: individuals who developed AD within the studied period. (Panel B) Dementia, VaD: individuals who developed VaD within the studied period. (Panel C) Dementia-free deceased group: non-demented individuals who died during the studied period. (Panel D) Healthy: non-demented and non-deceased individuals at the last test wave.

Figure 3

Figure 3. Hazard rates (HR) and 95% confidence intervals (95% CIs) from the multistate model for transitions from a healthy state to a demented and further to a deceased state, or a dementia-free deceased state. (Panel A) Old cohort and (Panel B) Old–old cohort.