Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Home
Hostname: page-component-7ccbd9845f-zxw8g Total loading time: 0.878 Render date: 2023-02-01T03:14:06.447Z Has data issue: true Feature Flags: { "useRatesEcommerce": false } hasContentIssue true

Positive Psychosocial Factors and Cognition in Ethnically Diverse Older Adults

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  19 September 2017

Laura B. Zahodne*
Affiliation:
University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, Michigan
Caitlin W.-M. Watson
Affiliation:
University of California, San Diego, California
Sonia Seehra
Affiliation:
Columbia University, New York, New York
Michelle N. Martinez
Affiliation:
Dartmouth College, Hanover, New Hampshire
*
Correspondence and reprint requests to: Laura B. Zahodne, Department of Psychology, University of Michigan, 530 Church Street, Ann Arbor, MI 48104. E-mail: lzahodne@umich.edu

Abstract

Objectives: Cross-sectional and longitudinal evidence from largely non-Hispanic White cohorts suggests that positive psychosocial factors, particularly self-efficacy and social support, may protect against late-life cognitive decline. Identifying potentially protective factors in racial/ethnic minority elders is of high importance due to their increased risk of Alzheimer’s disease. The overall goal of this study was to characterize cross-sectional associations between positive psychosocial factors and cognitive domains among Black, Hispanic, and White older adults. Methods: A total of 548 older adults (41% Black, 28% Hispanic, 31% White) in the Washington Heights-Inwood Columbia Aging Project completed cognitive and psychosocial measures from the NIH Toolbox and standard neuropsychological tests. Multiple-group regressions were used to compare cross-sectional associations between positive psychosocial factors and cognition across racial/ethnic groups, independent of demographics, depressive symptoms, and physical health. Results: Positive associations between self-efficacy and language did not significantly differ across race/ethnicity, although the bivariate association between self-efficacy and language was not significant among Hispanics. Additional positive associations were observed for Whites and Blacks, but not Hispanics. Negative associations between emotional support and purpose in life and working memory were seen only in Hispanics. Conclusions: Results confirm and extend the link between self-efficacy and cognition in late life, particularly for White and Black older adults. Previous studies on positive psychosocial factors in cognitive aging may not be generalizable to Hispanics. Longitudinal follow-up is needed to determine whether negative relationships between certain psychosocial factors and cognition in Hispanics reflect reverse causation, threshold effects, and/or negative aspects of having a strong social network. (JINS, 2018, 24, 294–304)

Type
Research Articles
Copyright
Copyright © The International Neuropsychological Society 2017 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Aday, L. (2001). At risk in America. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
Bandura, A. (1989). Regulation of cognitive processes through perceived self-efficacy. Developmental Psychology, 25, 729735.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barnes, L.L., Mendes de Leon, C.F., Wilson, R.S., Bienias, J.L., & Evans, D.A. (2004). Social resources and cognitive decline in a population of older African Americans and whites. Neurology, 63, 23222326.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Benton, A.L. (1955). The Visual Retention Test. New York: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Bolger, N., & Amarel, D. (2007). Effects of social support visibility on adjustment to stress: experimental evidence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92, 458475.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bonomi, A.E., Cella, D.F., Hahn, E.A., Bjordal, K., Sperner-Unterweger, B., Gangeri, L., & Zittoun, R. (1996). Multilingual translation of the Functional Assessment of Cancer Therapy (FACT) quality of life measurement system. Quality of Life Research, 5, 309320.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boyle, P.A., Buchman, A.S., Barnes, L.L., & Bennett, D.A. (2010). Effect of a purpose in life on risk of incident Alzheimer disease and mild cognitive impairment in community-dwelling older persons. Archives of General Psychiatry, 67, 304310.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buschke, H., & Fuld, P.A. (1974). Evaluating storage, retention, and retrieval in disordered memory and learning. Neurology, 24, 10191025.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carretti, B., Borella, E., Zavagnin, M., & De Beni, R. (2011). Impact of metacognition and motivation on the efficacy of strategic memory training in older adults: Analysis of specific, transfer, and maintenance effects. Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics, 52, 192197.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Casaletto, K.B., Umlauf, A., Marquine, M., Beaumont, J.L., Mungas, D., Gershon, R., & Heaton, R.K. (2016). Demographically corrected normative standards for the Spanish language version of the NIH Toolbox cognition battery. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 22, 364374.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Del Ser, T., Gonzálex-Montalvo, J.I., Martínez-Espinosa, S., Delgado-Villapalos, C., & Bermejo, F. (1997). Estimation of premorbid intelligence in Spanish people with the Word Accentuation Test and its application to the diagnosis of dementia. Brain and Cognition, 33, 343356.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Díaz-Venegas, C., Downer, B., Langa, K.M., & Wong, R. (2016). Racial and ethnic differences in cognitive function among older adults in the USA. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 31, 10041012.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ellwardt, L., Aartsen, M., Deeg, D., & Steverink, N. (2013). Does loneliness mediate the relation between social support and cognitive functioning in later life? Social Science and Medicine, 98, 11161124.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eremenco, S.L., Cella, D., & Arnold, B.J. (2005). A comprehensive method for the translation and cross-cultural validation of health status questionnaires. Evaluation & the Health Professions, 28, 212232.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gallo, L.C., & Matthews, K.A. (2003). Understanding the association between socioeconomic status and physical health: do negative emotions play a role? Psychological Bulletin, 129, 1051.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gallo, L.C., Penedo, F.J., Espinosa de los Monteros, K., & Arguelles, W. (2009). Resiliency in the face of disadvantage: do Hispanic cultural characteristics protect health outcomes? Journal of Personality, 77, 17071736.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Giles, L.C., Anstey, K.J., Walker, R.B., & Luszcz, M.A. (2012). Social networks and memory over 15 years of follow up in a cohort of older Australians: results from the Australian Longitudinal Study of Ageing. Journal of Aging Research, 856048, 1e7.Google Scholar
Glei, D.A., Landau, D.A., Goldman, N., Chuang, Y.L., Rodriguez, G., & Weinstein, M. (2005). Participating in social activities helps preserve cognitive function: An analysis of a longitudinal, population-based study of the elderly. International Journal of Epidemiology, 34, 864871.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hollinger, K.R., Franke, C., Arenivas, A., Woods, S.R., Mealy, M.A., Levy, M., & Kaplin, A.I. (2016). Cognition, mood, and purpose in life in neuromyelitis optica spectrum disorder. Journal of the Neurological Sciences, 362, 8590.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kats, D., Patel, M.D., Palta, P., Meyer, M.L., Gross, A.L., Whitsel, E.A., & Heiss, G. (2016). Social support and cognition in a community-based cohort: The Atheroschlerosis Risk in Communities (ARIC) study. Age and Ageing, 45, 475480.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lӧvdén, M., Ghisletta, P., & Lindenberger, U. (2005). Social participation attenuates decline in perceptual speed in old and very old age. Psychology and Aging, 20, 423434.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Manly, J.J., Bell-McGinty, S., Tang, M.-X., Schupf, N., Stern, Y., & Mayeux, R. (2005). Implementing diagnostic criteria and estimating frequency of mild cognitive impairment in an urban community. Archives of Neurology, 62, 17391746.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marin, G., & Marin, B.V. (1991). Research with Hispanic populations. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mattis, S. (Ed.) (1976). Mental status examination for organic mental syndrome in the elderly patient. New York: Grune & Stratton.Google Scholar
Melchior, M., Berkman, L.F., Niedhammer, I., Chea, M., & Goldberg, M. (2003). Social relations and self-reported health: A prospective analysis of the French Gazel cohort. Social Science & Medicine, 56, 18171830.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pillemer, S.C., & Holtzer, R. (2016). The differential relationships of dimensions of perceived social support with cognitive function among older adults. Aging and Mental Health, 20, 727735.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosen, W. (1981). The Rosen Drawing Test. Bronx, NY: Veterans Administration Medical Center.Google Scholar
Salsman, J.M., Butt, Z., Pilkonis, P.A., Cyranowski, J.M., Zill, N., Hendrie, H.C., & Cella, D. (2013). Emotion assessment using the NIH Toolbox. Neurology, 80, S76S86.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Seeman, T., McAvay, G., Merrill, S., Albert, M., & Rodin, J. (1996). Self-efficacy beliefs and change in cognitive performance: MacArthur Studies of Successful Aging. Psychology and Aging, 11, 538551.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Seeman, T.E., Lusignolo, T.M., Albert, M., & Berkman, L. (2001). Social relationships, social support, and patterns of cognitive aging in healthy, high-functioning older adults: MacArthur Studies of Successful Aging. Health Psychology, 20, 243255.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Siedlecki, K.L., Manly, J.J., Brickman, A.M., Schupf, N., Tang, M.X., & Stern, Y. (2010). Do neuropsychological tests have the same meaning in Spanish speakers as they do in English speakers? Neuropsychology, 24, 402411.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Silverstein, M., Chen, X., & Heller, K. (1996). Too much of a good thing? Intergenerational social support and the psychological well-being of older parents. Journal of Marriage and Family, 58, 970982.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stern, Y., Andrews, H., Pittman, J., Sano, M., Tatemichi, T., Lantigua, R., & Mayeux, R. (1992). Diagnosis of dementia in a heterogeneous population. Development of a neuropsychological paradigm-based diagnosis of dementia and quantified correction for the effects of education. Archives of Neurology, 49, 453460.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tang, M.-X., Cross, P., Andrews, H., Jacobs, D.M., Small, S., Bell, K., & Mayeux, R. (2001). Incidence of AD in African-Americans, Caribbean Hispanics, and Caucasians in northern Manhattan. Neurology, 56, 4956.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taylor, R.J., Chatters, L.M., Toler Woodward, A., & Brown, E. (2013). Racial and ethnic differences in extended family, friendship, fictive kin and congregational informal support networks. Family Relations, 62, 609624.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
U.S. Census Bureau. (2004). U.S. interim projections by age, sex, race, and Hispanic origin. Retrieved from http://www.census.gov/ipc/www/usinterimproj/ Google Scholar
Weintraub, S., Dikmen, S.S., Heaton, R.K., Tulsky, D.S., Zelazo, P.D., Bauer, P.J., & . . . Gershon, R.C. (2013). Cognition assessment using the NIH Toolbox. Neurology, 80, S54S64.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilson, R.S., Boyle, P.A., Segawa, E., Yu, L., Begeny, C.T., Anagnos, S.E., & Bennett, D.A. (2013). The influence of cognitive decline on well-being in old age. Psychology and Aging, 28, 304313.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wright, R.A., & Dismukes, A. (1995). Cardiovascular effects of experimentally induced efficacy (ability) appraisals at low and high levels of avoidant task demand. Psychophysiology, 32, 172176.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zahodne, L.B., Meyer, O.L., Choi, E., Thomas, M.L., Willis, S.L., Marsiske, M., & . . . Parisi, J.M. (2015). External locus of control contributes to racial disparities in memory and reasoning training gains in ACTIVE. Psychology & Aging, 30, 561572.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zahodne, L.B., Nowinski, C., Gershon, R., & Manly, J.J. (2014). Which psychosocial factors best predict cognitive performance in older adults? Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 20, 487495.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Supplementary material: File

Zahodne et al. supplementary material

Tables S1-S3

Download Zahodne et al. supplementary material(File)
File 39 KB
17
Cited by

Save article to Kindle

To save this article to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Positive Psychosocial Factors and Cognition in Ethnically Diverse Older Adults
Available formats
×

Save article to Dropbox

To save this article to your Dropbox account, please select one or more formats and confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you used this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your Dropbox account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Positive Psychosocial Factors and Cognition in Ethnically Diverse Older Adults
Available formats
×

Save article to Google Drive

To save this article to your Google Drive account, please select one or more formats and confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you used this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your Google Drive account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Positive Psychosocial Factors and Cognition in Ethnically Diverse Older Adults
Available formats
×
×

Reply to: Submit a response

Please enter your response.

Your details

Please enter a valid email address.

Conflicting interests

Do you have any conflicting interests? *