Hostname: page-component-588bc86c8c-2zrg9 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2023-11-30T14:42:16.637Z Has data issue: false Feature Flags: { "corePageComponentGetUserInfoFromSharedSession": true, "coreDisableEcommerce": false, "useRatesEcommerce": true } hasContentIssue false

Preservation of the Semantic Verbal Fluency Advantage in a Large Population-Based Sample: Normative Data from the TILDA Study

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 April 2016

Roisin M. Vaughan*
Affiliation:
St. Ita’s Hospital, Portrane, Co. Dublin, Ireland
Robert F. Coen
Affiliation:
Mercer’s Institute of Research on Ageing, St. James’s Hospital, Dublin 8, Ireland
RoseAnne Kenny
Affiliation:
The Irish Longitudinal Study on Ageing (TILDA), Trinity College, Dublin, Ireland
Brian A. Lawlor
Affiliation:
Mercer’s Institute of Research on Ageing, St. James’s Hospital, Dublin 8, Ireland
*
Correspondence and reprint requests to: Roisin M. Vaughan, St. Ita’s Hospital, Portrane, Co. Dublin. E-mail: roisvaughan@gmail.com

Abstract

Objectives: It is widely believed that phonemic fluency is more difficult than naming exemplars from a semantic category. Normative data in this regard are scarce, and there is considerable disagreement in the literature regarding the pattern in normal ageing and neurodegenerative conditions. Our objective was to provide normative data for semantic phonemic discrepancy scores from a large sample of older adults. Methods: A total of 5780 community-dwelling older adults were included in this prospective, longitudinal study. Discrepancy scores were calculated by subtracting phonemic fluency score from semantic fluency score for each participant. Quantile regression was used to estimate normative values stratified for age. Results: Subjects did better on testing of semantic fluency. The average discrepancy score was 9.18±6.89 words, (range, −20 to 37; n=5780). At the fiftieth percentile, those in their fifth decade produced 10 more “animals” than “letter F” words. Subjects scored one word less per decade, with an average of seven more “animal” words produced by those in their eighth decade. Conclusions: Our study is the first to provide normative data and confirms that, for animal versus letter F fluency, the semantic advantage persists into later life in a population-based sample of community-dwelling older adults. Given that a majority of clinical samples have confirmed a reverse of this pattern in Alzheimer’s dementia (i.e., loss of semantic advantage in Alzheimer’s disease, yielding a phonemic advantage), our findings support the clinical utility of brief fluency tests and encourage further research into their use in diagnosis and prediction of progression to dementia. (JINS, 2016, 22, 1–7)

Type
Research Articles
Copyright
Copyright © The International Neuropsychological Society 2016 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Baldo, J.V., Schwartz, S., Wilkins, D., & Dronkers, N.F. (2006). Role of frontal versus temporal cortex in verbal fluency as revealed by voxel-based lesion symptom mapping. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 12(6), 896900. doi:10.1017/S1355617706061078 Google Scholar
Barry, D., Bates, M.E., & Labouvie, E. (2008). FAS and CFL forms of verbal fluency differ in difficulty: A meta-analytic study. Applied Neuropsychology, 15(2), 97106. doi:10.1080/09084280802083863 Google Scholar
Brandt, J., & Manning, K.J. (2009). Patterns of word-list generation in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease. Clinical Neuropsychology, 23, 870879.Google Scholar
Canning, S.J.D., Leach, L., Stuss, D., Ngo, L., & Black, S.E. (2004). Diagnostic utility of abbreviated fluency measures in Alzheimer disease and vascular dementia. Neurology, 62, 556562.Google Scholar
Cerhan, J.H., Ivnik, R.J., Smith, G.E., Tangalos, E.C., Petersen, R.C., & Boeve, B.F. (2002). Diagnostic utility of letter fluency, category fluency, and fluency difference scores in Alzheimer’s disease. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 16(1), 3542. doi:10.1076/clin.16.1.35.8326 Google Scholar
Coen, R.F., Maguire, C., Swanwick, G.R., Kirby, M., Burke, T., Lawlor, B., &Coakley, D. (1996). Letter and category fluency in Alzheimer’s disease: A prognostic indicator of progression? Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 7(5), 246250. doi:10.1159/000106887 Google Scholar
Cottingham, M.E., & Hawkins, K.A. (2010). Verbal fluency deficits co-occur with memory deficits in geriatric patients at risk for dementia: Implications for the concept of mild cognitive impairment. Behavioural Neurology, 22(3-4), 7379. doi:10.3233/ben-2009-0246 Google Scholar
Cronin, H., O’Regan, C., Finucane, C., Kearney, P., & Kenny, R.A. (2013). Health and aging: Development of the Irish Longitudinal Study on Ageing health assessment. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 61(Suppl. 2), S269S278. doi:10.1111/jgs.12197 Google Scholar
Crum, R.M., Anthony, J.C., Bassett, S.S., & Folstein, M.F. (1993). Population-based Norms for the Mini-Mental State Examination by age and educational level. Journal of the American Medical Association, 269(18), 23862391.Google Scholar
Delis, D.C., Kaplan, E., & Kramer, J.H. (2001). Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System (D-KEFS). San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Folstein, M.F., Folstein, S.E., McHugh, P.R., & Fanjiang, G. (2001). MMSE. Mini-Mental State Examination. User’s Guide. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.Google Scholar
Gladsjo, J.A., Schuman, C.C., Evans, J.D., Peavy, G.M., Miller, S.W., & Heaton, R.K. (1999). Norms for letter and category fluency: Demographic corrections for age, education, and ethnicity. Assessment, 6(2), 147178. doi:10.1177/107319119900600204 Google Scholar
Hart, S., Smith, C.M., & Swash, M. (1988). Word fluency in patients with early dementia of Alzheimer type. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 27(2), 115124. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8260.1988.tb00759.x Google Scholar
Henry, J.D., & Crawford, J.R. (2004). A meta-analytic review of verbal fluency performance following focal cortical lesions. Neuropsychology, 18(2), 284295.Google Scholar
Kearney, P.M., Cronin, H., O’Regan, C., Kamiya, Y., Savva, G.M., Whelan, B., &Kenny, R. (2011). Cohort profile: The Irish Longitudinal Study on Ageing. International Journal of Epidemiology, 40(4), 877884. doi:10.1093/ije/dyr116 Google Scholar
Koenker, R., & Bassett, G. (1978). Regression quantiles. Econometrica, 46(1), 3350.Google Scholar
Kozora, E., & Cullum, C.M. (1995). Generative naming in normal aging: Total output and qualitative changes using phonemic and semantic constraints. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 9(4), 313320. doi:10.1080/13854049508400495 Google Scholar
Lafosse, J.M., Reed, B.R., Mungas, D., Sterling, S.B., Wahbeh, H., & Jagust, W.J. (1997). Fluency and memory differences between ischemic vascular dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychology, 11, 514522.Google Scholar
Laws, K.R., Duncan, A., & Gale, T.M. (2010). “Normal” semantic-phonemic fluency discrepancy in Alzheimer’s disease? A meta-analytic study. Cortex, 46(5), 595601. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2009.04.009 Google Scholar
Lonie, J.A., Herrmann, L.L., Tierney, K.M., Donaghey, C., O’Carroll, R., Lee, A., &Ebmeier, K.P. (2009). Lexical and semantic fluency discrepancy scores in aMCI and early Alzheimer’s disease. Journal of Neuropsychology, 3(1), 7992. doi:10.1348/174866408X289935 Google Scholar
Marra, C., Ferraccioli, M., & Gainotti, G. (2007). Gender-related dissociations of categorical fluency in normal subjects and in subjects with Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychology, 21, 207211.Google Scholar
Mitrushina, M.N., Boone, B.B., & Razani, J. (2005). Handbook of normative data for neuropsychological assessment. Cambridge: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Monsch, A.U., Bondi, M.W., Butters, N., Salmon, D.P., Katzman, R., & Thal, L.J. (1992). Comparisons of verbal fluency tasks in the detection of dementia of the Alzheimer type. Archives of Neurology, 49(12), 12531258. doi:10.1001/archneur.1992.00530360051017 Google Scholar
Murphy, K.J., Rich, J.B., & Troyer, A.K. (2006). Verbal fluency patterns in amnestic mild cognitive impairment are characteristic of Alzheimer’s type dementia. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 12(4), 570574. doi:10.1017/S1355617706060590 Google Scholar
Nasreddine, Z.S., Phillips, N.A., Bédirian, V., Charbonneau, S., Whitehead, V., Collin, I., Cummings, J.L., Chertkow, H. (2005). The Montreal Cognitive Assessment, MoCA: A brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 53(4), 695699. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2005.53221.x Google Scholar
Norris, M.P., Blankenship-Reuter, L., Snow-Turek, A.L., & Finch, J. (1995). Influence of depression on verbal fluency performance. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 2(3), 206215. doi:10.1080/13825589508256598 Google Scholar
Nutter-Upham, K.E., Saykin, A.J., Rabin, L.A., Roth, R.M., Wishart, H.A., Pare, N., &Flashman, L.A. (2008). Verbal fluency performance in amnestic MCI and older adults with cognitive complaints. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 23(3), 229241. doi:10.1016/j.acn.2008.01.005 Google Scholar
Ober, B.A., Dronkers, N.F., Koss, E., Delis, D.C., & Friedland, R.P. (1986). Retrieval from semantic memory in Alzheimer-type dementia. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 8(1), 7592. doi:10.1080/01688638608401298 Google Scholar
Ruff, R.M., Light, R.H., Parker, S.B., & Levin, H.S. (1996). Benton controlled oral word association test: Reliability and updated norms. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 11(4), 329338. doi:10.1093/arclin/11.4.329 Google Scholar
Teng, E., Leone-Friedman, J., Lee, G.J., Woo, S., Apostolova, L.G., Harrell, S., &Lu, P.H. (2013). Similar verbal fluency patterns in amnestic mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 28(5), 400410. doi:10.1093/arclin/act039 Google Scholar
Weakley, A., Schmitter-Edgecombe, M., & Anderson, J. (2013). Analysis of verbal fluency ability in amnestic and non-amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 28(7), 721731. doi:10.1093/arclin/act058 Google Scholar
Weiss, E.M., Ragland, J.D., Brensinger, C.M., Bilker, W.B., Deisenhammer, E.A., & Delazer, M. (2006). Sex differences in clustering and switching in verbal fluency tasks. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 12(04), 502509. doi:10.1017/S1355617706060656 Google Scholar
Supplementary material: File

Vaughan supplementary material

Vaughan Supplementary Revised

Download Vaughan supplementary material(File)
File 72 KB