Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-22dnz Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-28T09:15:12.641Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Further evidence for a comparable memory advantage of self-performed tasks in Korsakoff's syndrome and nonamnesic control subjects

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 August 2005

MASARU MIMURA
Affiliation:
Department of Neuropsychiatry, Showa University School of Medicine, Tokyo, Japan
SHIN-ICHI KOMATSU
Affiliation:
Faculty of Education, Shinshu University, Nagano, Japan
MOTOICHIRO KATO
Affiliation:
Department of Neuropsychiatry, Keio University School of Medicine, Tokyo, Japan
HARUO YOSHIMASU
Affiliation:
Department of Neuropsychiatry, Showa University School of Medicine, Tokyo, Japan
YASUSHI MORIYAMA
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Komagino Hospital, Tokyo, Japan
HARUO KASHIMA
Affiliation:
Department of Neuropsychiatry, Keio University School of Medicine, Tokyo, Japan

Abstract

Two experiments were carried out to examine memory in persons with amnesia using self-performed tasks. In Experiment 1, persons with Korsakoff's syndrome and nonamnesic participants with alcoholism learned action phrases not involving real objects by either self-performed tasks or verbal tasks. As indexed by free recall and recognition tests, a memory advantage favoring self-performed tasks was confirmed in both participant groups. In Experiment 2, persons with Korsakoff's syndrome, nonamnesic alcoholic participants, and young control participants learned object names under three different study conditions that differed from one another as to whether actions for each name were verbally generated and whether actions actually were performed. Verbal generation with or without performing the action facilitated recognition, whereas recall advantage was found only in the verbal generation-plus performance condition. These findings confirm a comparable memory advantage of self-performed tasks for a group with Korsakoff's syndrome and a group of nonamnesic comparison participants. Action memory therefore has potential therapeutic implications for memory rehabilitation. (JINS, 2005, 11, 545–553.)

Type
Research Article
Copyright
© 2005 The International Neuropsychological Society

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Backman, L., Nilsson, L.-G., & Chalom, D. (1986). New evidence on the nature of the encoding of action events. Memory & Cognition, 14, 339346.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baddeley, A. & Wilson, B.A. (1994). When implicit learning fails: Amnesia and the problem of error elimination. Neuropsychologia, 32, 5368.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Cohen, R.L., Peterson, M., & Mantini-Atkinson, T. (1987). Interevent differences in event memory: Why are some events more recallable than others? Memory & Cognition, 15, 109118.Google Scholar
Earles, J.L. (1996). Adult age differences in recall of performed and nonperformed items. Psychology and Aging, 11, 638648.Google Scholar
Engelkamp, J. (1998). Memory for actions. Hove, UK: Psychology Press.
Engelkamp, J. & Zimmer, H.D. (1994). Human memory: A multimodal approach. Kirkland, WA: Hogrefe & Huber.
Engelkamp, J. & Zimmer, H.D. (1997). Sensory factors in memory for subject-performed tasks. Acta Psychologica, 96, 4360.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fleischman, D.A., Gabrieli, J.D.E., Reminger, S., Rinaldi, J., Morrell, F., & Wilson, R. (1995). Conceptual priming in perceptual identification for patients with Alzheimer's disease and a patient with right occipital lobectomy. Neuropsychology, 9, 187197.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gerardin, E., Sirigu, A., Lehéricy, S., Poline, J.B., Gaymard, B., Marsault, C., Agid, Y., & Le Bihan, D. (2000). Partially overlapping neural networks for real and imagined hand movements. Cerebral Cortex, 10, 10931104.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Glisky, E.L., Schacter, D.L., & Butters, M.A. (1994). Domain-specific learning and remediation of memory disorders. In M.J. Riddoch & G.W. Humphreys (Eds.), Cognitive neuropsychology and cognitive rehabilitation (pp. 527548). Hove, UK: L. Erlbaum Associates.
Glisky, E.L., Schacter, D.L., & Tulving, E. (1986). Learning and retention of computer-related vocabulary in memory-impaired patients: Method of vanishing cues. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 8, 292312.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grafton, S.T., Fadiga, L., Arbib, M.A., & Rizzolatti, G. (1997). Premotor cortex activation during observation and naming of familiar tools. Neuroimage, 6, 231236.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hutton, S., Sheppard, L., Rusted, J.M., & Ratner, H.H. (1996). Structuring the acquisition and retrieval environment to facilitate learning in individuals with dementia of the Alzheimer type. Memory, 4, 113130.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Japan Society for Higher Brain Dysfunction (1997). Manual for the Standard Language Test of Aphasia. Tokyo: Shinko Igaku Shuppan.
Japan Society for Higher Brain Dysfunction (2001). Manual for the Standard Performance Test of Apraxia. Tokyo: Shinko Igaku Shuppan.
Karlsson, T., Backman, L., Herlitz, A., & Nilsson, L.-G. (1989). Memory improvement at different stages of Alzheimer's disease. Neuropsychologia, 27, 737742.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kessels, R.P.C. & de Haan, E.H.F. (2003). Implicit learning in memory rehabilitation: A meta-analysis on errorless learning and vanishing cues methods. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 25, 805814.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Komatsu, S., Mimura, M., Kato, M., Wakamatsu, N., & Kashima, H. (2000). Errorless and effortful processes involved in the learning of face-name associations by patients with alcoholic Korsakoff's syndrome. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 10, 113132.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kopelman, M.D. (1995). The Korsakoff syndrome. British Journal of Psychiatry, 166, 154173.Google Scholar
Larsson, C. & Ronnberg, J. (1987). Memory disorders as a function of traumatic brain injury. Scandinavian Journal of Rehabilitation Medicine, 19, 99104.Google Scholar
Lekeu, F., Van der Linden, M., Moonen, G., & Salmon, E. (2002). Exploring the effect of action familiarity on SPTs recall performance in Alzheimer's disease. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 24, 10571069.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lichty, W., Bressie, S., & Krell, R. (1988). When a fork is not a fork: Recall of performed activities as a function of age, generation, and bizarreness. In M.M. Gruneberg, P.E. Morris, & R.N. Sykes (Eds.), Practical aspects of memory: Current research and issues (Vol. 1, pp. 506511). Chichester, UK: Wiley.
Lipinska, B., Backman, L., Mantyla, T., & Viitanen, M. (1994). Effectiveness of self-generated cues in early Alzheimer's disease. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 19, 809819.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Martin, A. & Chao, L.L. (2001). Semantic memory and the brain: Structure and processes. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 11, 194201.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mimura, M., Komatsu, S., Kato, M., Yoshimasu, H., Wakamatsu, N., & Kashima, H. (1998). Memory for subject performed tasks in patients with Korsakoff syndrome. Cortex, 34, 297303.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Multhaup, K.S. & Balota, D.A. (1997). Generation effects and source memory in healthy older adults and in adults with dementia of the Alzheimer type. Neuropsychology, 11, 382391.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nilsson, L.-G. & Cohen, R.L. (1988). Enrichment and generation in the recall of enacted and non-enacted instructions. In M.M. Gruneberg, P.E. Morris, & R.N. Sykes (Eds.), Practical aspects of memory: Current research and issues (Vol. 1, pp. 427432). Chichester, UK: Wiley.
Nilsson, L.-G., Nyberg, L., Klingberg, T., Aberg, C., Persson, J., & Roland, P.E. (2000). Activity in motor areas while remembering action events. NeuroReport, 11, 21992201.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Norris, M.P. & West, R.L. (1991). Age differences in the recall of actions and cognitive activities: The effects of presentation rate and object cues. Psychological Research, 53, 188194.Google Scholar
Spitzer, R.L., Williams, J.B.W., Gibbon, M., & First, M.B. (1990). Structured clinical interview for DSM-III-R (SCID): User's guide. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association Press.
Wechsler, D. (1981). Manual for the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale–Revised. New York: Psychological Corporation.
Wechsler, D. (1987). Wechsler Memory Scale; revised manual. New York: Psychological Corporation.
Wilson, B.A., Baddeley, A., Evans, J., & Shiel, A. (1994). Errorless learning in the rehabilitation of memory impaired people. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 4, 307326.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zimmer, H.D., Helstrup, T., & Engelkamp, J. (2000). Pop-out into memory: A retrieval mechanism that is enhanced with the recall of subject-performed tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26, 658670.Google Scholar