Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-wq484 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-27T22:38:47.761Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Memory for novel and familiar spatial and linguistic temporal distance information in hypoxic subjects

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 February 2009

Ramona O. Hopkins
Affiliation:
Department of Hyperbaric Medicine, LDS Hospital, Eighth Avenue and C Street, Salt Lake City, UT, 84143
Raymond P. Kesner
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Utah, 502 Social Behavioral Science Building, Salt Lake City, UT 84112
Michael Goldstein
Affiliation:
Western Neurological Associates, 1351 East 3900 South, Salt Lake City, UT 84106

Abstract

Hypoxia is known to cause damage to the hippocampus as well as memory impairments in humans. Subjects who have experienced a hypoxic episode and age-, gender-, and education-matched control subjects were tested for memory for spatial and linguistic temporal distance information using sentences and spatial locations. Each test contained a familiar component based on information that is meaningful and is thought to be stored as part of the knowledge system (prior knowledge) as well as a novel component based on new information. Subjects were presented a list of eight-word sentences or eight spatial locations (Xs) on a grid on a Macintosh computer and tested for memory for temporal distances. Temporal distance is defined as the number of items that occur between the two test items, in the study phase. Compared to control subjects, hypoxic subjects were impaired across all temporal distances on the novel spatial and linguistic tasks. As the temporal distance increased, hypoxic subjects showed some improvement in memory performance. In addition, memory of familiar temporal distance information was also assessed. Hypoxic subjects were impaired, compared to control subjects, for familiar temporal distance information. For hypoxic subjects there was a proportionally greater impairment for novel compared to familiar spatial and linguistic temporal distance information. There was a significant difference in their performance on the familiar temporal distance tasks compared to their performance on the novel tasks. Prior knowledge appears to attenuate the deficits seen in the familiar temporal distance tasks. It appears that hypoxia may cause more selective damage to the hippocampus and this damage is sufficient to produce profound memory impairments for primarily novel and less severe memory impairments for familiar spatial and linguistic temporal distance information. (JINS, 1995, I, 454–468.)

Type
Research Articles
Copyright
Copyright © The International Neuropsychological Society 1995

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Akin, O. (1980). Models of architectural knowledge. London: Pion.Google Scholar
Auer, R.N., Jensen, M.L., & Wishaw, I.Q. (1989). Neurobehavioral deficit due to ischemic brain damage limited to half of the CAI sector of the hippocampus. Journal of Neuroscience, 9, 16411647.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bauer, P.J. (1992). Holding it all together: How enabling relations facilitate young children's event recall. Cognitive Development, 7, 128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bauer, P.J. & Hertsgaard, L.A. (1993). Increasing steps in recall of events: Factors facilitating immediate and long-term memory in 13.5 and 16.5 month-old children. Child Development, 64, 12041223.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benton, A.L. (1968). Differential behavioral effects in frontal lobe disease. Neuropsychologia, 6, 5360.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Benton, A.L. & Hamsher, deS. (1978). Multilingual Aphasia Examination. lowa City: University of lowa, Manual, revised.Google Scholar
Berg, E.A. (1948). A simple objective test for measuring flexibility in thinking. Journal of General Psychology, 39, 1522.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Breslow, R., Kocsis, J., & Belkin, B. (1980). Memory deficits in depression: Evidence using the Wechsler Memory Scale. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 51, 541542.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caine, E.D. (1986). The neuropsychology of depression: The pseudodementia syndrome. In Grant, I. & Adams, K.M. (Eds.), Neuropsychological assessment of neuropsychiatric disorders (pp. 221243). New York: Oxford.Google Scholar
Caramazza, A. & Berndt, R.S. (1987). Semantic and syntactic processes in aphasia: A review of the literature. Psychological Bulletin, 5, 809918.Google Scholar
Caramazza, A. & Zurif, E.G. (1976). Dissociation of algorithmic and heuristic processes in language comprehension: Evidence from aphasia. Brain and Language, 3, 572582.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cave, C.B. & Squire, L.R. (1991). Equivalent impairment of spatial and nonspatial memory following damage to the human hippocampus. Hippocampus, 1, 329340.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chi, M.T.H. & Glaser, R. (1979). Encoding process characteristics of experts and novices in physics. Paper presented at the meeting of the American Educational Research Association, San Francisco, CA.Google Scholar
Chiba, A.A. (1993). Cognitive function of medial-temporal structures: An attributed model. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Utah.Google Scholar
Dailey, C.A. (1956). Psychological finding five years after head injury. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 12, 440443.3.0.CO;2-L>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davis, H.P. & Volpe, B.T. (1990). Memory performance after ischemic or neurotoxin damage of the hippocampus. In Squire, L.R. & Lindenlaub, E. (Eds.), The Biology of Memory. Symposia Medica Hoechst 23 (pp. 149). New York: Verlag.Google Scholar
Denrrian, S.B. (1984). Denman Neuropsychological Scale Manual. Charleston, South Carolina.Google Scholar
De Renzi, E., Faglioni, P., & Previdi, P. (1977). Spatial memory and hemispheric locus of lesion. Cortex, 13, 424433.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Drewe, E.A. (1974). The effect of type and area of brain lesion on Wisconsin Card Sorting Test performance. Cortex, 10, 159170.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Edgan, D.E. & Schwartz, B.J. (1979). Chunking in recall of symbolic drawings. Memory and Cognition, 7, 149158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fivush, R. & Mandler, J.M. (1985). Developmental changes in the understanding of temporal sequence. Child Development, 56, 14371446.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fivush, R. & Slackman, E.A. (1986). The acquisition and development of scripts. In Nelson, K. (Ed.), Event knowledge: Structure and function in development (pp. 7196). Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Grant, D.A. & Berg, E.A. (1948). A behavioral analysis of degree of reinforcement and ease of shifting to new responses in a Weigl-type card sorting problem. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 38, 404411.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hirst, W. & Volpe, B.T. (1982). Temporal order judgments with amnesia. Brain and Cognition, 1, 294306.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hopkins, R.O. & Kesner, R.P. (submitted). Data-based and knowledge-based memory for temporal order information in subjects with hypoxic brain injury.Google Scholar
Hopkins, R.O., Kesner, R.P., & Goldstein, M. (1995). Item and order recognition memory for words, pictures, abstract pictures, spatial locations and motor responses in hypoxic subjects. Brain and Cognition, 27, 180201.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hopkins, R.O., Weaver, L.K., & Kesner, R.P. (1993). Long term memory impairments and hippocampal magnetic res-onance imaging in carbon monoxide poisoned subjects. Abstracts Undersea and Hyperbaric Medical Society Annual Scientific Meeting, 20, 15.Google Scholar
Hubert, F.A., & Piercy, M. (1976). Recognition memory in amnesic patients: Effect of temporal context and familiarity of material. Cortex, 12, 320.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ishimaru, H., Nabeshima, T., Katoh, A., Suzuki, H., Fukuta, T., & Kameyama, T. (1991). Effects of successive carbon monoxide exposures on delayed neuronal death in mice under the maintenance of normal body temperature. Biochemical and Biophysical Research Communications, 179, 836840.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnson, D. (1993). A comparison of data-based and knowledge-based memory in early versus middle stage Alzheimer's Disease. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Utah.Google Scholar
Jones-Gotman, M. (1986). Memory for designs: The hippocampal contribution. Neuropsychologia, 6, 255265.Google Scholar
Lezak, M.D. (1983). Neuropsychological assessment. (2nd ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Kahn, R.L., Sarit, S.H., Hilbert, N.M., & Niederehe, G. (1975). Memory complaint and impairment in the aged: The effect of depression and altered brain function. Archives of General Psychiatry, 32, 15691573.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Katoh, A., Nabeshima, T., Ishimaru, H., Ohtsuka, H., Fukuta, T., & Kameyama, T. (1990). Carbon monoxide (CO)-induced delayed amnesia and delayed neuronal death. In Nagatsu, T. (Ed.), Basic, clinical, and therapeutic aspects of Alzheimer's and Parkinson's diseases, Volume 1 (pp. 757760). New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
Kesner, R.P. & DiMattia, B.V. (1987). Neurobiology of an attribute model of memory. In Morrison, A.R. & Epstein, A.N. (Eds.), Progress in psychobiology and physiological psychology (pp. 207277). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Kinsbourne, M. & Wood, F. (1975). Short-term memory processes and the amnesic syndrome. In Deutsch, D. & Deutsch, J.A. (Eds.), Short term memory (pp. 257291). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Madsen, J.G. & Kesner, R.P. (1995). The temporal-distance effect in subjects with dementia of the Alzheimer's type. Alzheimer Disease and Associated Disorders, 9, 94100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McEntee, W.J. & Mair, R.G. (1980). Memory enhancement in Korsakoff's psychosis by clonidine: Further evidence of a noradrenergic deficit. Annals of Neurology, 7, 466470.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Milceli, G., Caltagirone, C., Gainotti, G., Masullo, C., & Silveri, M.C. (1981). Neuropsychological correlates of localized cerebral lesions in nonaphasic brain-damaged patients. Journal of Clinical Neuropsychology, 3, 5363.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Milner, B. (1963). Effects of different brain lesions on card sorting. Archives of Neurology, 9, 90100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Milner, B. (1970). Memory and the medial temporal regions of the brain. In Pribram, R.H. & Broadbent, P.E. (Eds.), Biology of memory (pp. 2950). New York: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Milner, B. (1974). Hemispheric specialization: Scope and limits. In Neurosciences (pp. 75–88). The Massachusetts Institute of Technology: The Colonial Press, Inc.Google Scholar
Olton, D.S. (1983). Memory functions and the hippocampus. In Seifert, W. (Ed.), Neurobiology of the hippocampus (pp. 335373). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Olton, D.S. & Shapiro, M.L. (1992). Mnemonic dissociations: The power of parameters. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 4, 200207.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Perret, E. (1974). The left frontal lobe of man and the suppression of habitual responses in verbal categorical behavior. Neuropsychologia, 12, 323330.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Plaistowe, M.M. & Dyson, S.E. (1993). Repeated exposure of carbon monoxide: A behavioral and morphological study. Abstracts New Zealand Biological Society, 30.Google Scholar
Press, G.A., Amaral, D.G., & Squire, L.R. (1989). Hippocampal abnormalities in amnesic patients revealed by highresolution magnetic resonance imaging. Nature, 341,5457.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ramier, A.M. & Hecaen, H. (1970). Role respectif des atteintes frontales et de la lateralisation lesion-nelle dans les deficits de la “fluence verbale.” Revue Neurologique, 123, 1722.Google Scholar
Robinson, A.L., Heaton, R.K., Lehman, R.A.W., & Stilson, D.W. (1980). The utility of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test in detecting and localizing frontal lobe lesions. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 48, 605614.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saffran, E.M. (1982). Neuropsychological approaches to the study of language. British Journal of Psychology, 73, 317337.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schwartz, M.F., Saffran, E.M., & Marin, O.S.M. (1980). The word order problem in agrammatism: I. Comprehension. Brain and Language, 10, 249262.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scoville, W.B. & Milner, B. (1957). Loss of recent memory after bilateral hippocampal lesions. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 20, 1121.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shimamura, A.P., Janowsky, J.S., & Squire, L.R. (1990). Memory for the temporal order of events in patients with frontal lobe lesions and amnesic patients. Neuropsychologia, 28, 803813.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, M.L. & Milner, B. (1981). The role of the right hippocampus in the recall of spatial location. Neuropsychologia, 19, 781793.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Squire, L.R. (1987). Memory and brain. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Taylor, L.B. (1979). Psychological assessment of neurosurgical patients. In Rasmussen, T. & Marino, R. (Eds.), Functional Neurosurgery. New York: Raven Press.Google Scholar
Tulving, E. (1987). Multiple memory systems and consciousness. Human Neurobiology, 6, 6780.Google ScholarPubMed
Volpe, B.T. & Petito, C.K. (1985). Dementia with bilateral medial temporal lobe ischemia. Neurology, 36, 408411.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yesavage, J.A., Brind, T.L., Rose, T.L., & Lum, O. (1983). Development and validation of a geriatric depression screening scale: A preliminary report. Journal Psychiatric Research, 17, 3749.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zola-Morgan, S. & Squire, L.R. (1990). Neuropsychological investigations of memory and amnesia: Findings from humans and nonhuman primates. In Diamond, A. (Ed.), The development and neural bases of higher cognitive functions (pp. 434456). New York: New York Academy of Sciences.Google Scholar
Zola-Morgan, S., Squire, L.R., & Amaral, D.G. (1986). Human amnesia and the medial temporal region: Enduring memory impairment following a bilateral lesion limited to field CAI of the hippocampus. Journal of Neuroscience, 6, 29502967.CrossRefGoogle Scholar