Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-dfsvx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-26T19:51:21.381Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Patterns of Prospective Memory Impairment Among Individuals with Depression: The Influence of Cue Type and Delay Interval

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  07 March 2013

Yanqi Ryan Li*
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, University of Western Australia, Perth, Western Australia
Michael Weinborn
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, University of Western Australia, Perth, Western Australia
Shayne Loft
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, University of Western Australia, Perth, Western Australia
Murray Maybery
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, University of Western Australia, Perth, Western Australia
*
Correspondence and reprint requests to: Yanqi Ryan Li, School of Psychology, University of Western Australia, Crawley, WA 6009, Australia. E-mail: liy03@student.uwa.edu.au

Abstract

The present study investigated the impact of cue type and delay interval on prospective memory performance in depressed, compared to non-depressed, individuals using a clinically relevant measure, the Memory for Intentions Screening Test. The depressed group demonstrated impaired performance on time-based, but not event-based, prospective memory tasks relative to the nondepressed group. The depressed group also demonstrated impaired prospective memory on tasks with longer delay intervals (15 min), but not on tasks with shorter delay intervals (2 min). These data support theoretical frameworks that posit that depression is associated with deficits in cognitive initiative (i.e., reduced ability to voluntarily direct attention to relevant tasks) and thus that depressed individuals are susceptible to poor performance on strategically demanding tasks. The results also raise multiple avenues for developing interventions (e.g., implementation intentions) to improve prospective memory performance among individuals with depression, with potential implications for medication and other treatment adherence. (JINS, 2013, 19, 1–5)

Type
Brief Communication
Copyright
Copyright © The International Neuropsychological Society 2013 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Albiński, R., Kliegel, M., Sędek, G., Kleszczewska-Albińska, A. (2012). Positive effects of subclinical depression in prospective memory and ongoing tasks in young and old adults. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 19(1–2), 3557. doi:10.1080/13825585.2011.628377CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Altgassen, M., Henry, J.D., Bürgler, S., Kliegel, M. (2011). The influence of emotional target cues on prospective memory performance in depression. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 33(8), 910916. doi:10.1080/13803395.2011.574607CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Altgassen, M., Kliegel, M., Martin, M. (2009). Event-based prospective memory in depression: The impact of cue focality. Cognition & Emotion, 23(6), 10411055. doi:10.1080/02699930802284158CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anderson, J.R., Lebiere, C.J. (1998). The atomic components of thought. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Clark, L., Chamberlain, S.R., Sahakian, B.J. (2009). Neurocognitive mechanism in depression: Implications for treatment. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 32, 5774.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Einstein, G.O., McDaniel, M.A. (2005). Prospective memory: Multiple retrieval processes. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14(6), 286290. doi:10.1111/j.0963-7214.2005.00382.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grenard, J., Munjas, B., Adams, J., Suttorp, M., Maglione, M., McGlynn, E., Gellad, W. (2011). Depression and medication adherence in the treatment of chronic diseases in the United States: A meta-analysis. Journal of General Internal Medicine, 26(10), 11751182. doi:10.1007/s11606-011-1704-yCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hertel, P.T. (2000). The cognitive-initiative account of depression-related impairments in memory. In L.M. Douglas (Ed.), Psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 39, pp. 4771). San Diego: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Howell, D. (2005). Statistical methods for psychology (5th ed.). Belmont, CA: Thomson Wadsworth.Google Scholar
Kliegel, M., Jäger, T., Phillips, L., Federspiel, E., Imfeld, A., Keller, M., Zimprich, D. (2005). Effects of sad mood on time-based prospective memory. Cognition & Emotion, 19(8), 11991213. doi:10.1080/02699930500233820CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levine, B., Robertson, I.H., Clare, L., Carter, G., Hong, J., Wilson, B.A., Stuss, D.T. (2000). Rehabilitation of executive functioning: An experimental–clinical validation of Goal Management Training. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 6(03), 299312. doi:nullCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martin, B.A., Brown, N.L., Hicks, J.L. (2011). Ongoing task delays affect prospective memory more powerfully than filler task delays. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue canadienne de psychologie expérimentale, 65(1), 4856. doi:10.1037/a0022872CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McDaniel, M., Howard, D., Butler, K. (2008). Implementation intentions facilitate prospective memory under high attention demands. Memory & Cognition, 36(4), 716724. doi:10.3758/mc.36.4.716CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morgan, E.E., Weber, E., Rooney, A.S., Grant, I., Woods, S.P., Hiv Neurobehavioral Research Program Hnrp Group . (2012). Longer ongoing task delay intervals exacerbate prospective memory deficits in HIV-associated neurocognitive disorders (HAND). Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 34(4), 416427. doi:10.1080/13803395.2012.654764CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Poquette, A.J., Moore, D.J., Gouaux, B., Morgan, E.E., Grant, I., Woods, S.P. (2012). Prospective memory and antiretroviral medication non-adherence in HIV: An analysis of ongoing task delay length using the memory for intentions screening test. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 17. doi:10.1017/S1355617712001051Google ScholarPubMed
Raskin, S.A., Buckheit, C., Sherrod, C. (2010). MIST: Memory for Intentions Test professional manual. Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.Google Scholar
Raskin, S.A., Woods, S.P., Poquette, A.J., McTaggart, A.B., Sethna, J., Williams, R.C., Tröster, A.I. (2011). A differential deficit in time- versus event-based prospective memory in Parkinson's disease. Neuropsychology, 25(2), 201209. doi:10.1037/a0020999CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rude, S.S., Hertel, P.T., Jarrold, W., Covich, J., Hedlund, S. (1999). Depression-related impairments in prospective memory. Cognition & Emotion, 13(3), 267276. doi:10.1080/026999399379276CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weinborn, M., Woods, S.P., Nulsen, C., Park, K. (2011). Prospective memory deficits in Ecstasy users: Effects of longer ongoing task delay interval. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 33(10), 11191128. doi:10.1080/13803395.2011.614595CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weinborn, M., Woods, S.P., O'Toole, S., Kellogg, E.J., Moyle, J. (2011). Prospective memory in substance abusers at treatment entry: Associations with education, neuropsychological functioning, and everyday memory lapses. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 26(8), 746755. doi:10.1093/arclin/acr071CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Woods, S.P., Moran, L.M., Dawson, M.S., Carey, C.L., Grant, I. (2008). Psychometric characteristics of the memory for intentions screening test. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 22(5), 864878. doi:10.1080/13854040701595999CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed