Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
×
Home

Information:

  • Access
  • Cited by 122
  • Cited by
    This article has been cited by the following publications. This list is generated based on data provided by CrossRef.

    Robertson, Ian H. 2000. “Every cloud …”. British Journal of Psychiatry, Vol. 176, Issue. 05, p. 412.

    2000. Current Awareness. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, Vol. 15, Issue. 11, p. 1077.

    Preti, Antonio De Biasi, Francesca and Miotto, Paola 2001. Musical Creativity and Suicide. Psychological Reports, Vol. 89, Issue. 3, p. 719.

    PRETI, ANTONIO 2001. MUSICAL CREATIVITY AND SUICIDE. Psychological Reports, Vol. 89, Issue. 7, p. 719.

    Amaducci, L. Grassi, E. and Boller, F. 2002. Maurice Ravel and right-hemisphere musical creativity: influence of disease on his last musical works?. European Journal of Neurology, Vol. 9, Issue. 1, p. 75.

    PAPWORTH, MARK A. and JAMES, IAN A. 2003. Createativity and Mood: Towards a Model of Cognitive Mediation. The Journal of Creative Behavior, Vol. 37, Issue. 1, p. 1.

    Ahmed, Tabassum and Miller, Bruce 2003. Encyclopedia of the Neurological Sciences. p. 269.

    2003. The Neuropsychiatry of Alzheimer's Disease and Related Dementias. p. 217.

    Moriarty, Jo 2003. Innovative Practice. Dementia, Vol. 2, Issue. 1, p. 125.

    Kleiner-Fisman, Galit and Lang, Anthony E. 2004. Insights into brain function through the examination of art: the influence of neurodegenerative diseases. NeuroReport, Vol. 15, Issue. 6, p. 933.

    Mendez, Mario F 2004. Dementia as a window to the neurology of art. Medical Hypotheses, Vol. 63, Issue. 1, p. 1.

    Heaton, Pamela and Wallace, Gregory L. 2004. Annotation: The savant syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, Vol. 45, Issue. 5, p. 899.

    Bechtereva, N.P. Korotkov, A.D. Pakhomov, S.V. Roudas, M.S. Starchenko, M.G. and Medvedev, S.V. 2004. PET study of brain maintenance of verbal creative activity. International Journal of Psychophysiology, Vol. 53, Issue. 1, p. 11.

    Fornazzari, L. R. 2005. Preserved painting creativity in an artist with Alzheimer's disease. European Journal of Neurology, Vol. 12, Issue. 6, p. 419.

    Hicks-Moore, Sandee Lynn 2005. Relaxing Music at Mealtime in Nursing Homes. Journal of Gerontological Nursing, Vol. 31, Issue. 12, p. 26.

    Abrahams, Brett S. and Geschwind, Daniel H. 2005. Encyclopedia of Genetics, Genomics, Proteomics and Bioinformatics.

    Harciarek, Michal and Jodzio, Krzysztof 2005. Neuropsychological Differences Between Frontotemporal Dementia and Alzheimer's Disease: A Review. Neuropsychology Review, Vol. 15, Issue. 3, p. 131.

    Sheehan, William Thurber, Steven and Sewall, Barry 2006. Dissociative identity disorder and temporal lobe involvement: replication and a cautionary note. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, Vol. 40, Issue. 4, p. 374.

    Al Aloucy, M.-J. and Semani, D. 2006. Cognition et création : la mémoire du vent. NPG Neurologie - Psychiatrie - Gériatrie, Vol. 6, Issue. 35, p. 13.

    Snyder, Allan Bahramali, Homayoun Hawker, Tobias and Mitchell, D John 2006. Savant-like Numerosity Skills Revealed in Normal People by Magnetic Pulses. Perception, Vol. 35, Issue. 6, p. 837.

    ×

Figures:

Actions:

      • Send article to Kindle

        To send this article to your Kindle, first ensure no-reply@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about sending to your Kindle. Find out more about sending to your Kindle.

        Note you can select to send to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be sent to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

        Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

        Functional correlates of musical and visual ability in frontotemporal dementia
        Available formats
        ×

        Send article to Dropbox

        To send this article to your Dropbox account, please select one or more formats and confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your <service> account. Find out more about sending content to Dropbox.

        Functional correlates of musical and visual ability in frontotemporal dementia
        Available formats
        ×

        Send article to Google Drive

        To send this article to your Google Drive account, please select one or more formats and confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your <service> account. Find out more about sending content to Google Drive.

        Functional correlates of musical and visual ability in frontotemporal dementia
        Available formats
        ×
Export citation

Abstract

Background

The emergence of new skills in the setting of dementia suggests that loss of function in one brain area can release new functions elsewhere.

Aims

To characterise 12 patients with frontotemporal dementia (FTD) who acquired, or sustained, new musical or visual abilities despite progression of their dementia.

Method

Twelve patients with FTD who acquired or maintained musical or artistic ability were compared with 46 patients with FTD in whom new or sustained ability was absent.

Results

The group with musical or visual ability performed better on visual, but worse on verbal tasks than did the other patients with FTD. Nine had asymmetrical left anterior dysfunction. Nine showed the temporal lobe variant of FTD.

Conclusion

Loss of function in the left anterior temporal lobe may lead to facilitation of artistic or musical skills. Patients with the left-sided temporal lobe variant of FTD offer an unexpected window into the neurological mediation of visual and musical talents.

Footnotes

See editorial pp. 412–413, this issue.

Declaration of interest

This work was supported by the UCLA Alzheimer Disease Center AG–10123, the Sidell – Kagan Research Foundation and the Elins family through UCLA Medical School and the A.W. and Mary-Margaret Clausen Chair at UCSF.

Some patients with dementia maintain previously learned board, card-game, painting or musical skills despite progression of their illness (Cummings & Zarit, 1987; Beatty et al 1988, 1994; Edwards-Lee et al, 1997). We have described patients with frontotemporal dementia (FTD) who developed new artistic skills after the clinical onset of their dementing illness (Miller et al, 1998) and we hypothesised that visual creativity was more common in FTD subtypes with anterior temporal lobe dysfunction. This paper extends those original observations and confirms that visual or musical creativity is present primarily in the subset of patients with predominantly left anterior temporal lobe dysfunction.

METHOD

We reviewed the clinical, neuropsychological and neuroimaging features of every patient (n=69) with a clinical diagnosis of FTD seen at the UCLA Alzheimer's Disease Center. Diagnosis required a compatible clinical syndrome and imaging which showed anterior structural and/or functional deficits as specified in the new research criteria for FTD (Neary et al, 1998). We included patients with unilateral left-sided dysfunction presenting with primary progressive aphasia or semantic dementia (Hodges et al, 1992; Snowden et al, 1996). Age and psychiatric status were not inclusion or exclusion factors and dementia severity varied. All received behavioural, psychological and imaging as previously described (Miller et al, 1998). Care-givers were queried about preservation of skills such as language, navigation, painting, music, card and board games.

Neuropsychological tests were designed to assess executive skills, language, visuoperception, visuo-construction and memory (Pachana et al, 1996). Magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) was used to exclude focal brain lesions. Single photon emission computed tomography (SPECT) was performed with 133Xe, which gave an absolute measure of regional cerebral blood flow, and 99Tc-labelled hexamethyl-propyleneamineoxime (HMAO), which provided high resolution qualitative images.

Two types of statistical comparisons were performed. First, we compared the 12 patients with creative skills (with ability) with 12 Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE)-matched (Folstein et al, 1975) subjects with FTD (without ability) on tests we find best for distinguishing asymmetrical left-sided from right-sided FTD (Boone et al, 1999). These include the Boston Naming Test (Kaplan et al, 1983) (lower with left-sided lesions), verbal IQ minus performance IQ (Adams et al, 1984) (higher with right-sided lesions), and ‘D-word’ minus design fluency (Lezak, 1976) (higher with right-sided lesions). For ‘D-word’, patients were allowed one minute to name words that begin with the letter D. For design fluency, patients used one minute to make novel designs by connecting four dots with straight lines within a box with five dots. The Neuropsychiatric Inventory (NPI) allow measurement of psychiatric symptoms (Cummings et al, 1994).

Also, we compared SPECT scans from the subjects with ability with those from the entire FTD cohort (in total, 57 SPECT scans were available). Scans were classified as bilateral, left-, or right-sided by two clinicians blind to patient identity. Patients were noted who had anterior temporal lobe hypoperfusion but sparing of dorsolateral frontal cortex, the temporal lobe variant of FTD (Edwards-Lee et al, 1997). Perfusion patterns in the subjects with ability v. the entire FTD population were compared.

RESULTS

Preserved creativity in patients with visual skills learned earlier in life

Patient 1: A 74-year-old left-handed female was referred for assessment of progressive aphasia. Most of her life she showed a mixture of scientific proficiency and shyness. A brilliant inventor, she had exceptional visuo-spatial abilities, successfully designing a chemical detector which she perfected throughout life. Yet she never enjoyed reading, and was shy. Language difficulties began at age 68 years, and by 71 she began to lose the meaning of words. She named objects by the superordinate category, calling most machines “instruments” and food items “ materials”. She stopped reading, but continued to perfect her inventions, receiving new patents for innovations during her 74th year. She became withdrawn, but disinhibition emerged. She became hypochondriacal and an aversion to meat persisted. On examination she was pleasant and cooperative but passive. Her MMSE score was 21. The only points lost on the MMSE were related to language. The Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure copy was normal, and visual memory for the item near normal. She generated six D-words, and 15 novel designs. Comprehension was good for two-step commands. She was profoundly anomic obtaining 1/60 words on the modified Boston Naming task. She read well except for words that did not follow regular phonetic rules (gnat or yacht). A SPECT revealed bitemporal, left greater than right, hypoperfusion with frontal sparing.

Patient 2: A 70-year-old right-handed woman was referred for assessment of progressive aphasia. She spoke eight languages, and played bridge at a professional level. Symptoms began around age 64 when she noticed difficulty with memory. On IQ testing at age 68, arithmetic was in the 99th percentile, comprehension the 50th percentile and general information the 16th percentile. Verbal learning delayed recall was in the 1st percentile. The Boston Naming test was at the 59th percentile. Visual reproduction abilities and the Wisconsin Card Sort Test were normal. Focal left temporal lobe atrophy was apparent on MRI. Word-finding trouble increased, but her visual and social skills remained intact and she continued playing bridge and chess. She became obsessed with playing computer chess and solitaire and considered suicide. Her MMSE score was 25. She missed three recall items, could not remember the name of the city and called a watch a key. Her digit span was eight forward. She could follow two, but not three-step commands and could not name bed or flower. Visual memory recall was impaired, but recognition was good. She generated two D-words and nine novel designs. Her SPECT showed bitemporal hypoperfusion, left greater than right, with frontal sparing.

Patient 3: A 76-year-old right-handed male was referred for assessment of a progressive five-year language and memory disorder. Previously an aerospace engineer, he designed aeroplanes and owned patents for items he had invented. Memory worsened and he developed slurred speech and decreased speech volume. Because of apathy he stopped pursuing hobbies. Navigation skills remained normal, but he was highly productive related to visual talents and successfully designed and built a lighting system and a garage. On examination he was cooperative but placid. His MMSE score was 22. He missed points for recall, orientation and repetition. Verbal IQ testing showed comprehension in the 2nd percentile and arithmetic in the 84th percentile. His performance IQ was 108 with picture completion in the 95th percentile, picture arrangement in the 99th percentile and block design the 84th percentile. He named 16/60 words on the Boston Naming Test. Controlled oral word association was in the 2nd percentile, verbal memory <1st percentile. He did well on most frontal systems tasks. A SPECT showed symmetrical bitemporal hypoperfusion with frontal sparing.

Musical ability which developed in the setting of dementia

Patient 4: A 49-year-old, right-handed man developed progressive aphasia. Previously gifted at foreign languages, he obtained an MS in linguistics. Subsequently, he worked as a mechanic. At age 42 he became withdrawn. His diet changed and meals consisted of chips and sweets. He constantly whistled, mastering classical and popular pieces. He composed musical songs about his bird. At work he became known as ‘the whistler’. Previously shy, he became inappropriately exuberant. At age 47 naming and memory problems emerged and he covered his house with reminder notes. He was forced to retire owing to odd behaviour and moved in with his brother. He became childlike and euphoric, constantly whistling and singing songs about his bird. A compulsive tendency to use the bathroom emerged. His MMSE score was 17. On the Boston Naming Test he named 4/60 items. Memory testing was impaired but he easily learned the floor structure of the medical office building. He generated two D-words but nine novel designs. Complex designs were copied perfectly and he performed well on the Modified Trails. SPECT showed left greater than right temporal hypoperfusion with frontal sparing.

Patient 5: A 78-year-old right-handed man began to mix up the names of his children, dogs and horses. He received little formal education but was a gifted linguist, learning Chinese, Italian and several Russian languages with an accent that led him to be mistaken for a native speaker. He enjoyed playing word puzzles because they allowed him to “understand the psychology of the designer”. Despite receiving limited musical training, at 68 years began to compose classical music. His mind was ‘taken over’ during composition, and he ‘sensed’ different tonal intervals that he put to music. Some pieces were publicly performed and composing continued even after his language and puzzle skills decreased. On examination he was stooped, quiet and remote. His MMSE score was 25. Verbal output was decreased with word-finding circumlocutions. He could not remember the name of any compositions that he had performed and remembered only one of three words after three minutes, but described recent events with accuracy. A SPECT showed moderate left temporal and mild left frontal hypoperfusion.

Previously developed musical and visual ability which persisted in the setting of dementia

Patient 6: A 71-year-old right-handed male was seen five years after the onset of aphasia. At 66 he became quieter and by 69 was exhibiting word-finding difficulty. At 70 reading and writing ceased and comprehension diminished. A talented musician, he could compose songs that captured the personality of acquaintances. He continued public concerts until age 71; only in the year prior to evaluation did this skill deteriorate. An excellent chess and crossword puzzle player, during the two years prior to evaluation his interest in these games increased. At evaluation he still paid bills, and successfully navigated. On examination his MMSE score was 15. Digit span was six. Speech was fluent with many verbal perseverations. He named 4/15 objects and followed two-step commands. He failed formal verbal and non-verbal testing, but remembered the participants in a professional basketball game from two days earlier. No D-words and one design was generated. He could add 389 and 676. Positron emission tomography showed selective left anterior temporal hypometabolism.

(Patients 7-11 developed new skills as visual artists and patient 12 showed preservation of visual and musical skills. All were previously described (Edwards-Lee et al, 1997; Miller et al, 1998). Their neuropsychological and SPECT data are included here for analysis.)

Statistical analyses

Statistical testing was performed with Fisher's exact test and Student's t-test. Table 1 shows the age at onset of FTD the age when the abilities emerged, the type of ability and the location for the anatomical dysfunction suggested by SPECT (or positron emission tomography in Patient 6, and pathology in Patient 8) is described. Seven developed new skills (five visual and two musical) in the setting of dementia, while five maintained visual and/or musical abilities (two visual, one musical, two both visual and musical) despite progression of dementia. Nine of the 12 patients with ability showed left-sided predominant hypoperfusion. Only 12 of the 45 patients without ability were left-sided. This difference was highly significant using Fisher's exact test (χ2=7.55; P <0.006). Eight of 12 subjects with ability had a temporal lobe variant pattern. In the remaining subjects with FTD without ability (45 in total), nine showed the temporal lobe variant. This difference was highly significant using a two-tailed Fisher's exact test (P<0.0035).

Table 1 Clinical and imaging features of creative patients with frontotemporal dementia

Patient Age illness begins (years) Age skills emerge (years) Skill Anatomical deficits Handedness
1 45 20 Inventor Left temporal Left
2 70 20 Inventor Left temporal Right
3 64 20 Bridge Left temporal Right
4 44 44 Whistler, songs Left temporal Right
5 70 68 Composer Left temporal Right
6 66 Teens Pianist Left temporal Right
7 53 53 Painter Left frontotemporal Left
8 65 55 Painter Right frontotemporal1 Left
9 47 38 Photos Right frontotemporal Right
10 56 45 Crafts Left frontotemporal Right
11 56 58 Painter Bilateral temporal Right
12 46 Teens Pianist, weaver Left temporal Left

Table 2 summarises the pertinent neuropsychological findings. Average score on verbal IQ minus performance IQ was ‒15.0 (s.d.=9.9) in the ability group and +4.4 (s.d.=9.2) in the group without ability (t=4.4; P=0.0004). Average D-words minus visual designs was ‒5.8 (s.d.=5.2) in the group with ability v. +2.7 (s.d.=2.7) in the group without ability (t=4.6; P=0.0002). The Boston Naming mean scores were 20.3 (s.d.=21.6) in those with ability and 46.5 (s.d.=16.0) in those without ability (Student's t=3.1; P=0.006). These results suggest more left-sided dysfunction in the group with ability. Table 3 summarises neuropsychiatric features. Both groups scored highly on the disinhibition, apathy and aberrant motor behaviour scales. The only statistically significant item was the depression scale; higher in the group with ability (P=0.023).

Table 2 Neuropsychological features of creative subjects with frontotemporal dementia

Patients Years since creativity Mode of creativity MMSE Modified Rey-0 copy D/design fluency VIQ-PIQ Boston Naming
1 Current Inventing 21 Normal 6/15 -30 1/60
2 Current Inventing 22 Normal 0/12 -24 6/15
3 Current Bridge 25 Normal 2/9 -17 4/60
4 Current Limericks 17 Normal 2/9 -12 4/60
5 Current Piano 15 Normal 0/1 - 6/15
7 One Painting 16 Normal 2/7 -20 0/60
9 Five Photos 26 Normal 6/7 +4 50/60
10 Five Crafts 9 Normal 0/1 -8 1/15
11 Current Painting 15 Normal 2/17 -10 32/60
12 Current Piano, weaving 1 Normal 0/0 -18 0/15

Table 3 Neuropsychiatric features of creative subjects with frontotemporal dementia

Patient Mode Compulsion Depression Family HX1 Premorbid strengths
1 Inventing Picks teeth, spits No Yes Brilliant scientist
2 Inventing None No Yes Scientist, inventor
3 Plays bridge Solitaire Yes Yes Multilingual, excellent game player
4 Whistler, songs Toileting, eating No Yes Multilingual
5 Music composer Unknown No Yes Multilingual
6 Pianist, chess Crosswords, chess Yes Yes Pianist, bridge player
7 Painting Searches for coins No Possible None known
8 Painting None Yes Yes Intelligent
9 Photographs Collects wax Yes Yes Advertising designer
10 Handicrafts Folds plates Yes Yes Artistic
11 Painting Searches for coins, dietary fads Yes Possible Memory for stocks
12 Plays piano, weaves Word games, copies Bible, dietary Yes No Pianist and weaver

DISCUSSION

Clinical overview

We describe the clinical features of 12 patients with FTD with new or preserved musical or visual ability. These remarkable patients represent 17% of our population with FTD, suggesting that musical or visual creativity is not rare with this dementia syndrome. Analysis of the clinical, neuropsychological, neuropsychiatric and neuroimaging features of these patients suggests an anatomical correlate for musical or visual talent in FTD.

Despite the diversity of visual and musical talents exhibited in this population, they shared many features. Talents were musical or visual and never manifested in the verbal sphere. Work lacked a symbolic or abstract component, and painters copied or remembered realistic landscapes, animals or people or perfected visual designs. A bridge champion and a chess master continued playing these games by manipulating visual images of cards or chess pieces learned earlier in life. For musicians, notes came automatically as sounds without conscious verbal manipulation. These processes have in common the recall of previously learned information or images. Subsequently this information is manipulated without the mediation of language.

Despite progressive cognitive and social impairment, patients showed increasing interest in the fine detail of objects, shapes, sounds, and visual or musical patterns. All neglected social and occupational responsibilities to pursue creative activities and their interests became progressively more restricted. Painting, photography, inventing, bridge, chess, composing, or music became obsessive preoccupation, and this obsessive interest with frequent repetition of the creative activity was an important factor in the development of a quality product.

Anatomical/physiological/neurochemical considerations

Frontotemporal dementia causes selective anterior frontal and/or temporal degeneration, whereas in Alzheimer's disease, early pathology involves entorhinal and posterior parietal and temporal areas (Brun, 1993). In many patients with Alzheimer's disease artistic and musical skills dissipate rapidly (Cummings & Zarit, 1987) associated with diminished function in posterior parietal and temporal regions, brain areas that contribute to visuoconstructive, linguistic and musical skills (Brun, 1993). However, in some patients with Alzheimer's disease previously learned musical skills remain intact despite progressive loss of other cognitive functions (Beatty et al, 1994).

In contrast to the decline in visuo-spatial skills characteristic of Alzheimer's disease, patients with FTD often retain visuo-constructive abilities as pathology is lacking in posterior parietal regions (Brun, 1993). Sparing of these areas allowed our subjects to paint, build, weave and play or compose music, despite advancing dementia. Yet, even though visuo-constructive skills are preserved in many patients with FTD, diminished, not enhanced, creativity is the rule with FTD (Snowden et al, 1996), suggesting that individuals who maintain or develop new creative skills have a distinctive form of this disorder.

Eight had the temporal lobe variant of FTD (Edwards-Lee et al, 1997), which represents almost 50% (8/17) of our patients with this anatomical subtype. Furthermore, another three had a temporally predominant imaging or perfusion deficit even though the frontal lobes were involved by the time of our evaluation. Also, all seven subjects with ability studied with SPECT when talents were present showed a pure temporal lobe perfusion deficit. In contrast, in patients with FTD without ability, and in the patients with ability whose productivity had ceased, widespread frontal and temporal hypoperfusion was seen with SPECT. Involvement of the temporal but sparing of frontal regions was a unifying feature of the patients with ability.

Organisation and motivation are mediated via dorsolateral and medial frontal areas. Goldman-Rakic (1996) observed that: “ Visuospatial processes engaged in humans by activities such as chess playing, following maps, recalling one's location with respect to landmarks, or painting and drawing from memory… rely on the dorsolateral prefrontal convexity…”

Sparing of working memory (Waltz et al, 1999) and episodic memory (Hodges et al, 1992) characterise the temporal variant of FTD and probably aided in the musical and visual creativity. Organising a musical piece or painting requires working memory. Similarly, the painters' capacity to draw scenes remembered from the past, and the game players' and musicians' ability to recall previously learned visual and musical patterns suggested that components of episodic memory were spared. In contrast, semantic memory, which requires the integrity of the left anterior temporal lobe (Hodges et al, 1992; McCarthy & Warrington, 1994; Snowden et al, 1996), was impaired.

Most of the patients in the group with ability showed asymmetrical left hemisphere degeneration, which is congruent with the studies suggesting that left hemisphere injury does not invariably decimate musical skills. The composers Shebalin and Langlais suffered strokes in the left temporal-parietal regions which, despite inducing aphasia, did not negatively influence note reading or musical composing abilities (Sergent, 1993), and Ravel, who suffered from a focal degenerative disorder of the left hemisphere, continued composing while aphasic and agraphic (Sergent, 1993). Polk & Kertesz (1993) described a patient with progressive aphasia in whom “organized, although reiterative music production” continued despite aphasia progression. Their patients were reminiscent of Patients 4-6 and 12 in whom music production was possible despite progressive aphasia. In contrast, McFarland & Fortin (1982) described an accomplished organist who lost his ability to play familiar melodies following infarction of the right superior temporal and supramarginal gyri. The right hemisphere is dominant for spontaneous production of music (Gordon & Bogen, 1974) and was spared (if not enhanced) in our subjects.

Right hemisphere injury devastates the ability to copy, or paint, even in experienced artists (Schnider et al, 1993) while left hemisphere injury does not routinely injure painting skills (Alajouanine, 1948). Kaczmarek (1991) described a painter who suffered a left hemisphere stroke with aphasia who lost the ability “to create the highly symbolic pictures he used to paint before the stroke”. Yet, he continued to draw realistically. Kaczmarek noted that there was “ impairment of symbolic processing with fairly well preserved technical skills…” Realistic reproduction of music, or paintings without symbolism was a feature of the creativity in our cohort.

Paradoxical functional facilitation

To explain the unexpected occurrence of behavioural improvement following brain injury, Kapur (1996) used the term “paradoxical functional facilitation”. He noted that “ in normal subjects, inhibitory and excitatory mechanisms interact in a complex harmony…. The role of inhibitory processes may be critical in mediating specific restorative paradoxical functional facilitation effects”. Paradoxical functional facilitation may have contributed to the unexpected emergence of talent in our patients.

The temporal gyri process visual and auditory information (Baylis et al, 1987) and specific cells in the inferior temporal gyrus respond to distinctive visual stimuli including shape and movement. There are many connections to and from polar temporal cortex, and posterior temporal cortex. The role of anterior temporal cortex in processing of visual and musical information is unknown, but enhanced artistic skill in patients with anterior temporal and orbital frontal degeneration suggests that these areas inhibit dorsolateral frontal and posterior temporal and parietal regions involved with these processes.

Paradoxical increases in brain perfusion occur in patients with focal cerebral lesions, (Weiller et al, 1992), whereas visual stimulation reduces blood flow in prefrontal regions (Kawashima et al, 1995). Patient 11 described painfully intense visual images, suggesting abnormally intense experiencing of visual images. Patients 7 and 9 found coins with uncanny ability. At age 68, patient 10 suddenly had musical patterns which triggered musical composition. We hypothesise that selective degeneration of left anterior temporal cortex led to decreased inhibition of the more right-sided and posteriorly located visual and musical systems, and dorsolateral frontal regions involved with working memory, thereby enhancing artistic interest and productivity.

Often, talent in one area is accompanied by dysfunction in other spheres and unusual brain organisation may have been important in this subgroup. Four were left-handed and, as a group, they were exceptionally gifted in linguistic, musical or visual tasks prior to the onset of their dementia. The two inventors showed life-long visual talent, yet both were socially withdrawn. Brain asymmetries (possibly congenital) may have predisposed to talents that developed or persisted. Some exhibited lifelong eccentricity suggesting that the illness began early in life and progressed with remarkable slowness. Did loss of function in the left anterior temporal lobe lead to brain remodelling and consequent enhancement in function in the right hemisphere or posterior parts of brain necessary for production of music and art?

Neuropsychiatric features and influences

On the NPI, the group with ability had greater disinhibition than the group without ability, although this difference did not reach statistical significance. Also, disabling compulsions were common in our patients. Two artists searched for coins, a photographer crafted candle wax into miniature animals, a craftsman folded paper plates, a bridge player and pianist repetitively played solitaire and chess for hours at a time, and a weaver and pianist compulsively copied from the Bible and played ‘search and circle’ games. Their artistic achievements were, in part, attributable to their compulsive repetition of musical or visual tasks.

Psychiatric illness is common among artists and Post (1994) found that depression was more common than expected among writers, whereas “severe personality deviations” were more frequent among visual artists. Depression was more common in our patients with ability although the relationship between the depression, disinhibition and compulsions and the emergence of creativity is unknown.

Clinical Implications and Limitations

CLINICAL IMPLICATIONS

  • Selective degeneration of the left anterior temporal lobe is often accompanied by loss of semantic knowledge, but preservation or enhancement of visual and musical abilities.

  • Visual and musical abilities should be encouraged in the setting of left anterior temporal injury or dysfunction.

  • Dementia is not invariably associated with relentless loss of all intellectual abilities.

LIMITATIONS

  • The data in this study were analysed retrospectively.

  • Some of the individuals in this study were highly intelligent prior to the onset of their symptoms, suggesting that the emergence of talent may occur only in select patients.

  • The physiological basis underlying the ability to develop new artistic functions in the setting of dementia remains unknown.

References

Adams, R. I., Smigielski, J. & Jenkins, R. L. (1984) Development of a Satz-Mogel short form of the WAIS–R. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 52, 908.
Alajouanine, T. (1948) Aphasia and artistic realization. Brain, 17, 229241.
Baylis, G. C., Rolls, E. T. & Leonard, C. M. (1987) Functional subdivisions of the temporal lobe neocortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 7, 330342.
Beatty, W., Zavadil, K. D., Bailly, R. C., et al (1988) Preserved musical skill in a severely demented patient. International Journal of Clinical Neuropsychology, 10, 158164.
Beatty, W., Winn, P., Adams, R. I., et al (1994) Preserved cognitive skills in dementia of the Alzheimer type. Archives of Neurology, 41, 10401046.
Boone, K. B., Miller, B. L., Lee, A., et al (1999) Neuropsychological patterns in right versus left frontotemporal dementia. Journal of the International Neuropsychology Society, 5, 616622.
Brun, A. (1993) Frontal lobe dementia of the non-Alzheimer type revisited. Dementia, 4, 126139.
Cummings, J. L. & Zarit, J. M. (1987) Probable Alzheimer's disease in an artist. Journal of the American Psychiatric Association, 258, 27312734.
Cummings, J. L., Mega, M., Gray, K., et al (1994) The Neuropsychiatric Inventory: comprehensive assessment of psychopathology in dementia. Neurology, 44, 23082314.
Edwards-Lee, T., Miller, B. L., Benson, D. F., et al (1997) The temporal lobe variant of frontotemporal dementia. Brain, 120, 10271040.
Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E. & McHugh, P. R. (1975) ‘Mini-Mental State’: A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatry Research, 12, 185198.
Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1996) Regional and cellular fractionation of working memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 93, 1347313480.
Gordon, H. W. & Bogen, J. E. (1974) Hemispheric lateralization of singing after intracarotid sodium amylobarbitone. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 37, 727738.
Hodges, J. R., Patterson, K., Oxbury, S., et al (1992) Semantic dementia. Progressive fluent aphasia with temporal lobe atrophy. Brain, 115, 17831806.
Kaczmarek, B. J. (1991) Aphasia in an artist: a disorder of symbolic processing. Aphasiology, 4 & 5, 351371.
Kaplan, E., Goodglass, H. & Weintraub, S. (1983) Boston Naming Test. Philadelphia, PA: Lea & Febiger.
Kapur, N. (1996) Paradoxical functional facilitation in brain-behaviour research. A critical review. Brain, 119, 17751790.
Kawashima, R., O'Sullivan, B. T. & Roland, P. E. (1995) Positron-emission tomography studies of cross-modality inhibition in selective attentional tasks: closing the “mind's eye”. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 192, 5969–1972.
Lezak, M. D. (1976) Neuropsychological Assessment. New York: Oxford University Press.
McCarthy, R. A. & Warrington, E. K. (1994) Disorders of semantic memory. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 346, 8996.
McFarland, H. R. & Fortin, D. (1982) Amusia due to right temporoparietal infarct. Archives of Neurology, 39, 725727.
Miller, B. L., Cummings, J. L., Boone, K., et al (1998) Emergence of artistic talent in frontotemporal dementia. Neurology, 51, 978981.
Neary, D., Snowden, S., Gustafson, L., et al. (1998) Frontotemporal lobar degeneration: a consensus on clinical diagnostic criteria. Neurology, 51, 15461552.
Pachana, N., Boone, K. B., Miller, B. L., et al. (1996) Comparison of neuropsychological functioning in Alzheimer's disease and frontotemporal dementia. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 2, 505510.
Polk, M. & Kertesz, A. (1993) Music and language in degenerative disease of the brain. Brain & Cognition, 22, 98117.
Post, F. (1994) Creativity and psychopathology. A study of 291 world-famous men. British Journal of Psychiatry, 165, 2234.
Schnider, A., Regard, M., Benson, D. F., et al (1993) Effects of a right-hemisphere stroke on an artist's performance. Neuropsychiatry, Neuropsychology & Behavioural Neurology, 6, 249255.
Sergent, J. (1993) Music, the brain and Ravel. Trends in Neuroscience, 16, 168171.
Snowden, J. S., Neary, D. & Mann, D. M. A. (eds) (1996) Frontotemporal Lobar Degeneration. New York: Churchill-Livingstone.
Waltz, J., Knowlton, B., Holyoak, K., et al (1999) A system for relational reasoning in human prefrontal cortex. Psychological Science, 10, 119125.
Weiller, C., Chollet, F., Friston, K., et al (1992) Functional reorganization of the brain in recovery from striatocapsular infarction in man. Annals of Neurology, 31, 472472.