Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-skm99 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-28T23:25:23.869Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Self-reported mid- to late-life physical and recreational activities: Associations with late-life cognition

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  18 September 2023

Brandon E. Gavett*
Affiliation:
Department of Neurology, University of California Davis, Sacramento, CA, USA School of Psychological Science, University of Western Australia, Crawley, WA, Australia
Keith F. Widaman
Affiliation:
Graduate School of Education, University of California, Riverside, CA, USA
Cathryn McKenzie
Affiliation:
School of Psychological Science, University of Western Australia, Crawley, WA, Australia
Fransia S. De Leon
Affiliation:
School of Medicine, University of California Davis, Sacramento, CA, USA
Evan Fletcher
Affiliation:
Department of Neurology, University of California Davis, Sacramento, CA, USA
Sarah Tomaszewski Farias
Affiliation:
Department of Neurology, University of California Davis, Sacramento, CA, USA
Dan Mungas
Affiliation:
Department of Neurology, University of California Davis, Sacramento, CA, USA
*
Corresponding author: Brandon E. Gavett; Email: bgavett@ucdavis.edu
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Objective:

Physical and recreational activities are behaviors that may modify risk of late-life cognitive decline. We sought to examine the role of retrospectively self-reported midlife (age 40) physical and recreational activity engagement – and self-reported change in these activities from age 40 to initial study visit – in predicting late-life cognition.

Method:

Data were obtained from 898 participants in a longitudinal study of cognitive aging in demographically and cognitively diverse older adults (Age: range = 49–93 years, M = 75, SD = 7.19). Self-reported physical and recreational activity participation at age 40 and at the initial study visit were quantified using the Life Experiences Assessment Form. Change in activities was modeled using latent change scores. Cognitive outcomes were obtained annually (range = 2–17 years) using the Spanish and English Neuropsychological Assessment Scales, which measure verbal episodic memory, semantic memory, visuospatial processing, and executive functioning.

Results:

Physical activity engagement at age 40 was strongly associated with cognitive performance in all four domains at the initial visit and with global cognitive slope. However, change in physical activities after age 40 was not associated with cognitive outcomes. In contrast, recreational activity engagement – both at age 40 and change after 40 – was predictive of cognitive intercepts and slope.

Conclusions:

Retrospectively self-reported midlife physical and recreational activity engagement were strongly associated with late-life cognition – both level of performance and rate of future decline. However, the data suggest that maintenance of recreational activity engagement (e.g., writing, taking classes, reading) after age 40 is more strongly associated with late-life cognition than continued maintenance of physical activity levels.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BYCreative Common License - NCCreative Common License - SA
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/4.0/), which permits non-commercial re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the same Creative Commons licence is included and the original work is properly cited. The written permission of Cambridge University Press must be obtained for commercial re-use.
Copyright
The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of International Neuropsychological Society

Alzheimer’s disease is characterized by the accumulation of abnormal levels of amyloid and tau pathology, which causes neurodegeneration and late-life cognitive decline that that unfolds over multiple decades (Sutphen et al., Reference Sutphen, Jasielec, Shah, Macy, Xiong, Vlassenko, Benzinger, Stoops, Vanderstichele, Brix, Darby, Vandijck, Ladenson, Morris, Holtzman and Fagan2015). Current evidence suggests that different risk and protective factors, encountered throughout the lifespan, have the potential to modify Alzheimer’s disease burden and associated clinical sequelae (Livingston et al., Reference Livingston, Huntley, Sommerlad, Ames, Ballard, Banerjee, Brayne, Burns, Cohen-Mansfield, Cooper, Costafreda, Dias, Fox, Gitlin, Howard, Kales, Kivimäki, Larson, Ogunniyi, Orgeta and Mukadam2020; Stern et al, Reference Stern, Arenaza‐Urquijo, Bartrés‐Faz, Belleville, Cantilon, Chetelat, Ewers, Franzmeier, Kempermann, Kremen, Okonkwo, Scarmeas, Soldan, Udeh‐Momoh, Valenzuela, Vemuri and Vuoksimaa2020). Broadly speaking, some protective factors are believed to confer their protection by enhancing resistance and resilience to Alzheimer’s disease pathology, where "resistance" refers to reduced development of brain pathology (e.g., less amyloid burden than expected based on one’s age and other risk factors) and "resilience" refers to the brain’s capacity to cope with pathology (e.g., slower cognitive decline than expected based on the degree of neurodegeneration; Arenaza-Urquijo & Vemuri, Reference Arenaza-Urquijo and Vemuri2020). Resistance and resilience are proposed to be manifestations of brain reserve, brain maintenance, and cognitive reserve (Stern et al., Reference Stern, Albert, Barnes, Cabeza, Pascual-Leone and Rapp2023), all of which may be enhanced by modifiable behavioral and health factors (de Rooij, Reference de Rooij2022). Of the many candidate protective factors, participation in physical and recreational activities are two of the most promising, given the robust evidence base in the literature to support their protective effects against late-life cognitive decline (Baumgart et al., Reference Baumgart, Snyder, Carrillo, Fazio, Kim and Johns2015; Fratiglioni et al., Reference Fratiglioni, Paillard-Borg and Winblad2004; Norton et al., Reference Norton, Matthews, Barnes, Yaffe and Brayne2014). For the purpose of this study, recreational activities are defined as leisure activities that provide intellectual stimulation, increase social contact, and/or support emotional well-being.

It has been hypothesized physical activity may initiate biological repair processes in organs including the brain, thus serving to preserve cognitive functioning via promoting brain maintenance (Lieberman et al., Reference Lieberman, Kistner, Richard, Lee and Baggish2021). For example, evidence has suggested that midlife physical activity is associated with larger late-life gray matter volumes (Rovio et al., Reference Rovio, Spulber, Nieminen, Niskanen, Winblad, Tuomilehto, Nissinen, Soininen and Kivipelto2010) and that late-life physical activity is associated with a number of positive late-life brain biomarkers, such as larger gray and white matter volumes, less amyloid and tau burden, and increased functional connectivity within the hippocampus (Benedict et al., Reference Benedict, Brooks, Kullberg, Nordenskjöld, Burgos, Le Grevès, Kilander, Larsson, Johansson, Ahlström, Lind and Schiöth2013; Brown et al., Reference Brown, Peiffer, Taddei, Lui, Laws, Gupta, Taddei, Ward, Rodrigues, Burnham, Rainey-Smith, Villemagne, Bush, Ellis, Masters, Ames, Macaulay, Szoeke, Rowe and Martins2013; Brown et al., Reference Brown, Rainey-Smith, Dore, Peiffer, Burnham, Laws, Taddei, Ames, Masters, Rowe, Martins, Villemagne and Cristina Polidori2018; Eisenstein et al., Reference Eisenstein, Giladi, Hendler, Havakuk and Lerner2021; Erickson et al., Reference Erickson, Raji, Lopez, Becker, Rosano, Newman, Gach, Thompson, Ho and Kuller2010; Steffener et al., Reference Steffener, Habeck, O’Shea, Razlighi, Bherer and Stern2016).

Past research has also shed some light on the association between mid- to late-life physical activity engagement and late-life cognition. Meta-analyses have found that, overall, higher baseline physical activity is associated with reduced risk of cognitive decline and dementia over follow-ups ranging from 1 to 27 years (Blondell et al., Reference Blondell, Hammersley-Mather and Veerman2014), and higher physical activity in people over 65 is associated with reduced risk of dementia due to Alzheimer’s disease (Beckett et al., Reference Beckett, Ardern and Rotondi2015). Similarly, recreational activity engagement may play a role in protecting against late-life cognitive decline. Mid- and late-life recreational activity participation has been associated with a reduced risk of late-life cognitive decline and dementia (e.g., Akbaraly et al., Reference Akbaraly, Portet, Fustinoni, Dartigues, Artero, Rouaud, Touchon, Ritchie and Berr2009; Andel et al., Reference Andel, Silverstein and Kåreholt2015; Carlson et al., Reference Carlson, Helms, Steffens, Burke, Potter and Plassman2008; Friedland et al., Reference Friedland, Fritsch, Smyth, Koss, Lerner, Chen, Petot and Debanne2001; Lindstrom et al., Reference Lindstrom, Fritsch, Petot, Smyth, Chen, Debanne, Lerner and Friedland2005; Scarmeas et al., Reference Scarmeas, Levy, Tang, Manly and Stern2001; Wilson et al., Reference Wilson, Mendes De Leon, Barnes, Schneider, Bienias, Evans and Bennett2002; Yates et al., Reference Yates, Ziser, Spector and Orrell2016), even after accounting for education and occupational complexity (Jonaitis et al., Reference Jonaitis, La Rue, Mueller, Koscik, Hermann and Sager2013), and late-life interventions to increase recreational activity engagement have positive effects on cognitive performance (e.g., Iizuka et al., Reference Iizuka, Suzuki, Ogawa, Kobayashi-Cuya, Kobayashi, Takebayashi and Fujiwara2019).

While the literature broadly supports a protective effect of higher activity levels on late-life cognitive functioning, past research in this area has largely used cross-sectional ratings of activity engagement, either current or retrospective (Benedict et al., Reference Benedict, Brooks, Kullberg, Nordenskjöld, Burgos, Le Grevès, Kilander, Larsson, Johansson, Ahlström, Lind and Schiöth2013; Casaletto et al., Reference Casaletto, Rentería, Pa, Tom, Harrati, Armstrong, Rajan, Mungas, Walters, Kramer and Zahodne2020; Chang et al., Reference Chang, Jonsson, Snaedal, Bjornsson, Saczynski, Aspelund, Eiriksdottir, Jonsdottir, Lopez, Harris, Gudnason and Launer2010; Guiney et al., Reference Guiney, Lucas, Cotter and Machado2019), or interventions to experimentally induce short-term changes in activity levels. Although evidence suggests that activity engagement changes throughout the lifespan (e.g., Williams et al., Reference Williams, Pendleton and Chandola2020; van der Zee et al., Reference van der Zee, van der Mee, Bartels and de Geus2019), fewer studies have attempted to quantify the effect of naturalistic changes in activity engagement over the years or decades prior to the measurement of relevant cognitive outcomes (Tolppanen et al., Reference Tolppanen, Solomon, Kulmala, Kåreholt, Ngandu, Rusanen, Laatikainen, Soininen and Kivipelto2015; Wagner et al., Reference Wagner, Grodstein, Proust-Lima and Samieri2020). Similarly, previous studies on activity engagement have often measured cognitive outcomes cross-sectionally (Erickson et al., Reference Erickson, Prakash, Voss, Chaddock, Hu, Morris, White, Wójcicki, McAuley and Kramer2009; Gross et al., Reference Gross, Lu, Meoni, Gallo, Schrack and Sharrett2017) or have used relatively simple methods of capturing change, such as observed difference scores or dichotomous variables representing change vs. no change (e.g., Sofi et al., Reference Sofi, Valecchi, Bacci, Abbate, Gensini, Casini and Macchi2011), which do not account for measurement error. Therefore, to gain a more comprehensive understanding of how physical and recreational activities confer protection against late-life cognitive decline, it is important to determine whether changes in these activity levels over an extended time frame – e.g., from midlife to late life – offer any additional predictive value beyond static measures of activity engagement.

Using data from the current cohort, Brewster et al. (Reference Brewster, Melrose, Marquine, Johnson, Napoles, MacKay-Brandt, Farias, Reed and Mungas2014) found evidence to suggest that self-reported recreational activities at midlife (i.e., age 40) and current (i.e., late life) together formed a synergistic relationship with late-life cognitive decline, such that slower late-life cognitive decline was associated with high current recreational activities but low midlife recreational activity engagement. This pattern of results was interpreted to suggest that larger decreases in recreational activities after midlife were associated with a more rapid decline in global cognition, after controlling for numerous covariates. In addition, higher initial cognitive status in the domains of semantic memory and executive functioning were predicted by heavy physical activity engagement in midlife. However, the study by Brewster et al. (Reference Brewster, Melrose, Marquine, Johnson, Napoles, MacKay-Brandt, Farias, Reed and Mungas2014) was limited by its use of sum scores to quantify activity engagement without establishing whether those scores were equally valid at both age epochs.

Thus, the goal of the current study is to examine how retrospectively self-reported physical and recreational activity engagement in midlife, and changes in these activities from midlife to late life, influence late-life cognitive outcomes. Our study seeks to build upon previous research in several important ways. We will use latent-variable modeling to measure self-reported activity engagement, which can ensure that these constructs are measured with equal validity at both age epochs (measurement invariance; Liu et al., Reference Liu, Millsap, West, Tein, Tanaka and Grimm2017). This also allows for latent change score modeling, which provides a direct estimate of change in self-reported activity engagement over time, free from measurement error (McArdle, Reference McArdle2009). Of the few studies in this area that have used longitudinal cognitive outcomes, most have relied upon relatively coarse measures of cognitive status, such as the Mini-Mental State Examination (e.g., Schuit et al., Reference Schuit, Feskens, Launer and Kromhout2001), which may not be psychometrically well-suited for tracking cognitive change over time (e.g., due to ceiling effects). In the current study, we will use an established cognitive factor structure previously validated in this cohort (e.g., Fletcher et al., Reference Fletcher, Gavett, Harvey, Farias, Olichney, Beckett, DeCarli and Mungas2018; Gavett et al., Reference Gavett, Fletcher, Harvey, Farias, Olichney, Beckett, DeCarli and Mungas2018) to investigate cross-sectional and longitudinal cognitive outcomes, i.e., cognitive intercepts and slopes.

Drawing on the information presented above, we hypothesize that both cross-sectional (midlife: age 40) and longitudinal (change from midlife to late life) self-reported physical and recreational activity engagement will be positively associated with cognitive outcomes. Specifically, we predict that greater self-reported midlife engagement and less self-reported decline in engagement from midlife to late life will predict better cross-sectional cognitive performance (i.e., higher cognitive intercepts), and less rapid decline in cognitive performance (i.e., more positive cognitive slopes).

Method

Participants

Participants were 898 volunteers in the University of California (UC) Davis Aging and Diversity Cohort, which is a longitudinal sample that contains large proportions of Hispanic/Latinx, Black, and non-Hispanic White older adults. A more detailed description of the cohort, including recruitment, can be found in Hinton et al. (Reference Hinton, Carter, Reed, Beckett, Lara, DeCarli and Mungas2010). Participants in this study were evaluated and followed within the research program of the UC Davis Alzheimer’s Disease Center. Enrollment began in 2001 and a rolling enrollment design was used to build the cohort, with substantial enrollment continuing through 2010. Exclusion criteria included unstable major medical illness, major primary psychiatric disorder, and substance abuse or dependance in the last 5 years. All participants signed informed consent, all human subject involvement was overseen by institutional review boards at UC Davis, the Veterans Administration Northern California Health Care System, and San Joaquin General Hospital in Stockton, California, and the research was completed in accordance with the Helsinki Declaration. Study visits, which occur (approximately) annually, typically require several hours to complete and include comprehensive neuropsychological assessment, neurological examination, and completion of questionnaires. Due to rolling enrollment, there was variability in the total number of evaluations completed by each participant. The participants in the current study had at least two annual study visits where cognition was measured. Clinical diagnosis of participants was made by a consensus committee of experts using standard diagnostic criteria (e.g., Albert et al., Reference Albert, DeKosky, Dickson, Dubois, Feldman, Fox, Gamst, Holtzman, Jagust, Petersen, Snyder, Carrillo, Thies and Phelps2011; McKhann et al., Reference McKhann, Knopman, Chertkow, Hyman, Jack, Kawas, Klunk, Koroshetz, Manly, Mayeux, Mohs, Morris, Rossor, Scheltens, Carrillo, Thies, Weintraub and Phelps2011).

Materials

Self-reported physical and recreational activity participation was measured using the Life Experiences Assessment Form (LEAF; see Brewster et al., Reference Brewster, Melrose, Marquine, Johnson, Napoles, MacKay-Brandt, Farias, Reed and Mungas2014). Participants used the LEAF to answer questions about their current physical and recreational activity engagement as well as their retrospectively rated physical and recreational activity engagement at age 40. For some participants, “current” self-reported activity levels were queried at their baseline visit. For other individuals who had been enrolled prior to implementation of the LEAF, their “current” activity levels were based on the first study visit at which the LEAF was administered. Finally, some participants had discontinued or withdrawn from the parent study before the LEAF was administered; we retained data from these participants in the current study to ensure that our outcomes (cognitive intercepts and slopes) were estimated using as much data as possible. For all participants, self-reported ratings of activity engagement at age 40 were made retrospectively using the same set of questions. If the LEAF was administered more than once to any participant during follow-up study visit(s), only data from the initial administration was used in the current study. Physical activity items on the LEAF ask participants to use a 5-point Likert scale to rate their frequency of activity engagement, ranging from 1 (“Never”) to 5 (“Every day or almost every day”), on tasks described as light work-related tasks, heavy work-related tasks, light house/yard work, vigorous house/yard work, light exercise, and vigorous exercise. Recreational activity items on the LEAF ask about reading, complex cooking, writing, taking classes, performance arts, games or puzzles, cultural events, arts and crafts, socializing, and attendance at clubs or meetings, using the same 5-point Likert scale. Item response categories were collapsed when necessary to avoid sparse cells (i.e., response options that were endorsed by fewer than 10 participants).

Using the current sample data, we estimated the internal consistency reliability of the LEAF scores for both activity types (physical and recreational) and both age epochs (age 40 and current) using the item.omega function from the misty (version 0.4.10; Yanagida, Reference Yanagida2023) package in R version 4.2.3 (R Core Team, 2023). This function calculates McDonald’s omega reliability coefficient (McDonald, Reference McDonald1999) for categorical data and can account for residual covariances between items (Green & Yang, Reference Green and Yang2009). The sample reliability statistics are as follows: Age 40 physical activities, ω = .66, 95% CI [.61, .70]; current physical activities, ω = .63, 95% CI [.59, .68]; age 40 recreational activities, ω = .67, 95% CI [.63, .71]; and current recreational activities, ω = .68, 95% CI [.64, .72].

Cognitive functioning was measured using the Spanish and English Neuropsychological Assessment Scales (SENAS). The SENAS has undergone extensive development as a battery of cognitive tests relevant to cognitive aging that allow for valid comparisons across racial, ethnic, and language groups (Mungas et al., Reference Mungas, Reed, Crane, Haan and González2004, Reference Mungas, Reed, Haan and González2005, Reference Mungas, Reed, Marshall and González2000). Development and validation of the SENAS used modern psychometric methods to ensure highly reliable measurement across a diverse range of abilities and ages, and across both Spanish and English speakers. The SENAS yields four psychometrically matched composite scores – verbal episodic memory, semantic memory, spatial, and executive functioning – that are normally distributed and free from floor and ceiling effects. The episodic memory composite score is created using a multi-trial word-list-learning task (Mungas et al., Reference Mungas, Reed, Crane, Haan and González2004). Semantic memory is assessed using object-naming and picture association tasks. Executive functioning scores represent a combination of verbal fluency (semantic, phonemic) and working memory (backward digit span and visual span, list sorting) tasks. The SENAS Spatial Localization scale was used to derive the Spatial composite score; this task measures perception and reproduction of increasingly complex two-dimensional spatial relationships. The SENAS measures were administered at all evaluations. Administration procedures, measure development, and psychometric characteristics of the SENAS battery are described in more detail elsewhere (Mungas et al., Reference Mungas, Reed, Crane, Haan and González2004).

Data analysis

Physical and recreational activities

For each activity type, two latent variables were derived from the LEAF data: (1) activity engagement at age 40 and (2) change in activity engagement from age 40 to current. Latent change score models (McArdle, Reference McArdle2009) were used to estimate these age 40 and change factors for physical (Fig. 1) and recreational (Fig. 2) activities. To account for differences in time elapsed between age 40 and current, participant age (centered at 70) was included as a covariate in these models. Because physical and recreational activity engagement were measured using self-report, which is susceptible to bias introduced by the presence of cognitive impairment (i.e., cognitively impaired individuals may be less capable of reliably reporting on their personal history), we sought to determine whether the models used to estimate these activities were invariant to cognitive status. Before running the latent change score models, we also established that physical and recreational activities could be modeled with equal validity at both age epochs (age 40 and current). A description of our approach to establishing the time invariance of these models is provided in an electronic supplement.

Figure 1. Latent change score model for physical activities. Arrows labeled with λ indicate that the factor loadings for the corresponding indicators were constrained to be equal across age epochs. Asterisks indicate freely estimated parameters. The residual autocorrelations between the same indicator variables (e.g., light work) at age 40 and current (C) were modeled as one latent variable per item pair, but only shown once (in dashed lines) for simplicity.

Figure 2. Latent change score model for recreational activities. Arrows labeled with λ indicate that the factor loadings for the corresponding indicators were constrained to be equal across age epochs. Asterisks indicate freely estimated parameters. The residual autocorrelations between the same indicator variables (e.g., reading) at age 40 and current (C) were modeled as one latent variable per item pair, but only shown once (in dashed lines) for simplicity.

Cognition

Cognitive performance outcomes were modeled using separate but correlated latent intercepts for four constructs – verbal episodic memory, semantic memory, spatial ability, and executive functioning – along with a single latent slope factor representing rate of change in global cognitive functioning. This factor structure has provided the best fit to our longitudinal cognitive data in numerous other studies (e.g., Fletcher et al., Reference Fletcher, Gavett, Harvey, Farias, Olichney, Beckett, DeCarli and Mungas2018; Gavett et al., Reference Gavett, Fletcher, Harvey, Farias, Olichney, Beckett, DeCarli and Mungas2018); in particular, our use of a global cognitive slope is consistent with literature suggesting that much of the variance in late-life cognitive decline can be attributed to a general change factor (e.g., Tucker-Drob et al., Reference Tucker-Drob, Brandmaier and Lindenberger2019).

Hypothesis testing

We created two integrated structural models – one for physical activities and the other for recreational activities – to test our hypotheses about the degree to which late-life cognition is influenced by midlife activity participation and changes in activity participation from midlife to late-life. This model was analyzed using the Bayes estimator (with default priors) within the multilevel modeling framework of Mplus version 8.6 (Muthén & Muthén, Reference Muthén and Muthén2021). In the within part of the model (Fig. 3A), we estimated intercepts and slopes for each of the four cognitive domains as a function of time. The cognitive test scores were regressed on a dichotomous indicator of whether participants had participated in neuropsychological assessment at a prior study visit to account for practice effects. The cognitive scores were also regressed on a previous evaluation by Spanish language interaction term because past research in this cohort has shown that the magnitude of previous evaluation effects differs between Spanish and English speakers (Brewster et al., Reference Brewster, Melrose, Marquine, Johnson, Napoles, MacKay-Brandt, Farias, Reed and Mungas2014; Early et al., Reference Early, Widaman, Harvey, Beckett, Park, Farias, Reed, DeCarli and Mungas2013; Melrose et al., Reference Melrose, Brewster, Marquine, MacKay-Brandt, Reed, Farias and Mungas2015). In the between part of the model (Fig. 3B), a global cognitive slope factor was incorporated, and the five primary outcome variables (4 cognitive intercepts and 1 cognitive slope) were regressed on latent activity participation at age 40, latent change in activity participation from age 40 to the current visit, and a number of observed covariates. These covariates included age (years centered at 70), education (years centered at 12), and dichotomous indicator variables representing sex (reference = female), referral source (0 = community, 1 = clinic), Black/African American racial identification (0 = no, 1 = yes), Latinx or Hispanic ethnic identification (0 = no, 1 = yes), other ethnic/racial identification (0 = no, 1 = yes), and primary language (0 = English, 1 = Spanish). See the Supplemental material for more details about the multilevel analyses.

Figure 3. Multilevel model path diagram showing illustrating our approach to hypothesis testing. In the within part of the model (panel A), domain-specific random intercepts and slopes are defined as a function of time. In the between part of the model (panel B), domain-specific cognitive intercepts and the global cognitive slope are regressed on activity factors (age 40 and change) and covariates.

Results

Ages in this sample ranged from 49 to 93 years, education ranged from 0 to 20 years, and the number of study visits ranged from 2 to 17. Additional participant demographics are provided in Table 1. We also show descriptive data in Table 1 stratified by whether or not participants ever completed the LEAF questionnaire. The results show that those who never completed the LEAF were older, less educated, more cognitively impaired (both in terms of cognitive performance and in terms of frequency with which a diagnosis of MCI or Dementia was made), and more likely to be a clinical – rather than community – referral into the parent study. Therefore, by retaining participants without LEAF data in the primary analyses, our cognitive outcomes (intercepts and slope) are more representative of the full range of diversity in the population from which this cohort was recruited.

Table 1. Participant demographics at initial visit

LEAF = life experiences assessment form; MCI = mild cognitive impairment.

Difference column indicates whether the ‘With LEAF’ and ‘Without LEAF’ subgroups differ significantly (α = .05) on the chosen variable.

The fits of the latent change score models were satisfactory, especially for the physical activities model (Fig. 1), χ 2 (df = 58) = 143.97, p < .01, CFI = 0.955, TLI = 0.940, RMSEA = 0.049 (95% CI [0.039, 0.059]). Fit was more modest, but acceptable, for the recreational activities model (Fig. 2), χ 2 (df = 193) = 472.40, p < .01 CFI = 0.904, TLI = 0.895, RMSEA = 0.049 (95% CI [0.044, 0.055]). Measurement invariance testing was performed to determine whether physical and recreational activity engagement could be modeled equally well in cognitively impaired (i.e., MCI and dementia) and cognitively intact participants. Full results are shown in the supplementary material. To summarize here, there was mixed evidence to support the scalar invariance of the physical and recreational activities models across groups defined by cognitive impairment status. For both activity factors, because the scalar invariance models had reasonably good absolute fit, we proceeded with our planned analysis. Because we used a Bayesian estimator to fit the full structural model shown in Figure 3, traditional model fit statistics were not available. However, the models reached convergence as determined by a potential scale reduction factor (Gelman & Rubin, Reference Gelman and Rubin1992) that reached a point of stability (i.e., < 1.1) for the second half of all sampling iterations.

The multilevel modeling results are presented in Tables 2 (physical activities) and 3 (recreational activities). Retrospectively self-reported physical activity engagement at age 40 was a strong predictor of all cognitive outcomes (the four domain-specific intercepts and the global slope); in contrast, change in self-reported physical activity engagement from age 40 to current (adjusted for amount of time elapsed) was not a salient predictor of any cognitive outcome. On the other hand, not only was retrospectively self-reported age 40 recreational activity engagement a predictor of all cognitive outcomes, but change in self-reported recreational activities over time was a strong predictor of all cognitive outcomes as well. A visual depiction of these results is shown in Figure 4, which is based on a hypothetical reference participant, representing a 70-year-old non-Hispanic White English-speaking woman with 12 years of education who was a community referral.

Figure 4. Model-predicted verbal episodic memory scores plotted as a function of time (x-axis), activity level at age 40 (colored lines), change in activity level over time (vertical facets), and activity type (horizontal facets). Data are based on a hypothetical 70-year-old non-Hispanic White English-speaking woman with 12 years of education who was a community referral (reference participant).

Table 2. Physical activities model results

CrI = credible intervals; EM = episodic memory; EF = executive functioning; PA = physical activities; Δ PA = change in physical activities; SM = semantic memory; VS = visuospatial.

*95% credible intervals of the posterior distribution do not include 0.

Table 3. Recreational activities model results

CrI = credible intervals; EM = episodic memory; EF = executive functioning; PA = physical activities; Δ PA = change in physical activities; SM = semantic memory; VS = visuospatial.

*95% credible intervals of the estimate do not include 0.

Discussion

In recent decades, it has become increasingly well-established that Alzheimer’s disease pathology – namely, the accumulation of amyloid and tau pathologies – begins to unfold decades before clinical signs and symptoms emerge (Sutphen et al., Reference Sutphen, Jasielec, Shah, Macy, Xiong, Vlassenko, Benzinger, Stoops, Vanderstichele, Brix, Darby, Vandijck, Ladenson, Morris, Holtzman and Fagan2015). Therefore, it is important to identify modifiable protective factors that can be intervention targets earlier in life. Previous research has established the importance of several lifestyle variables that are associated with greater age-related resilience against neurodegeneration and cognitive decline, including engagement in physical and recreational activities (Baumgart et al., Reference Baumgart, Snyder, Carrillo, Fazio, Kim and Johns2015; Fratiglioni et al., Reference Fratiglioni, Paillard-Borg and Winblad2004).

Past research in this area has tended to examine cross-sectional activity ratings or short-term experimental interventions, whereas few studies have attempted to examine naturalistic changes in activity engagement over the years or decades prior to the measurement of late-life cognition (Tolppanen et al., Reference Tolppanen, Solomon, Kulmala, Kåreholt, Ngandu, Rusanen, Laatikainen, Soininen and Kivipelto2015; Wagner et al., Reference Wagner, Grodstein, Proust-Lima and Samieri2020). Similarly, past research has tended to use outcome variables like cross-sectional cognitive performance, dichotomous indicators of cognitive decline, or conversion to dementia; as such, less is known about how prior activity engagement influences the rate of change in late-life cognition (Schuit et al., Reference Schuit, Feskens, Launer and Kromhout2001; Sofi et al., Reference Sofi, Valecchi, Bacci, Abbate, Gensini, Casini and Macchi2011; Wagner et al., Reference Wagner, Grodstein, Proust-Lima and Samieri2020). We addressed these gaps in the literature by using rigorous methods for measuring not only static self-reported physical and recreational activity engagement – but change in these self-reported behaviors over time – along with high-quality longitudinal cognitive outcome data. We hypothesized that both cross-sectional (midlife: age 40) and longitudinal (change from midlife to late-life) self-reported physical and recreational activity engagement would be positively associated with cognitive outcomes, i.e., cognitive intercepts and slopes.

Our hypothesis was partially supported: more retroactively self-reported activity engagement at midlife – both physical and recreational – was predictive of better late-life cognitive intercepts and slopes. However, we found a marked difference when examining change in self-reported activity engagement as a predictor of late-life cognition. After controlling for retroactively self-reported midlife activity levels, maintaining or even increasing one’s self-reported engagement in recreational activities (e.g., intellectually stimulating activities) over time was associated with higher baseline cognition and less rapid cognitive decline in later life. In contrast, change in self-reported physical activity after midlife was not a positive predictor of late-life cognitive decline. These findings are consistent with those of Brewster et al. (Reference Brewster, Melrose, Marquine, Johnson, Napoles, MacKay-Brandt, Farias, Reed and Mungas2014), though we expanded upon their study by using sophisticated latent-variable methods, a larger sample size, a longer period of follow-up, and more comprehensive cognitive outcome data. Our findings are also consistent with previous studies that have found a positive association between mid- and late-life activity participation and late-life cognition (e.g., Akbaraly et al., Reference Akbaraly, Portet, Fustinoni, Dartigues, Artero, Rouaud, Touchon, Ritchie and Berr2009; Andel et al., Reference Andel, Silverstein and Kåreholt2015; Carlson et al., Reference Carlson, Helms, Steffens, Burke, Potter and Plassman2008, Friedland et al., Reference Friedland, Fritsch, Smyth, Koss, Lerner, Chen, Petot and Debanne2001; Lindstrom et al., Reference Lindstrom, Fritsch, Petot, Smyth, Chen, Debanne, Lerner and Friedland2005; Scarmeas et al., Reference Scarmeas, Levy, Tang, Manly and Stern2001; Wilson et al., Reference Wilson, Mendes De Leon, Barnes, Schneider, Bienias, Evans and Bennett2002; Yates et al., Reference Yates, Ziser, Spector and Orrell2016).

Importantly, mid- and late-life participation in cognitively stimulating activities has been shown to be associated with independently derived and validated measures of late-life cognitive reserve (Reed et al., Reference Reed, Dowling, Tomaszewski Farias, Sonnen, Strauss, Schneider, Bennett and Mungas2011). Reserve in that study was operationalized as cognitive function not explained by comprehensive measures of neuropathology, and thus, these results suggest that cognitively stimulating activities promote resilience to negative effects of brain pathology on cognition. Although the current study does not address whether the protective effect of recreational activity engagement on cognition is mediated via increased cognitive reserve, our findings contribute more evidence to the extant literature suggesting that higher levels of recreational activity engagement are associated with better late-life cognition and slower cognitive decline. We also note that a novel conclusion arising from the current study is that change in recreational activities over a period of decades may be a very important target for interventions designed at promoting late-life cognition, thus building on similar evidence obtained from shorter-term intervention studies (Iizuka et al., Reference Iizuka, Suzuki, Ogawa, Kobayashi-Cuya, Kobayashi, Takebayashi and Fujiwara2019). These findings suggest that community-based interventions designed to increase access to recreational activities – especially in middle age and later – could make an important contribution to public health.

This study has several limitations. First, self-reported ratings of activity engagement at age 40 were made retrospectively. Retrospective ratings of past behaviors can be unreliable and subject to bias (e.g., Fransson et al., Reference Fransson, Knutsson, Westerholm and Alfredsson2008). In particular, individuals with lower cognitive functioning – an outcome variable used in the current study – may be more likely to make mistakes when answering questions about their personal history (Schneider et al., Reference Schneider, Junghaenel, Zelinski, Meijer, Stone, Langa and Kapteyn2021). Even contemporaneous self-report of current activity levels may be susceptible to reporting bias (Adams et al., Reference Adams, Matthews, Ebbeling, Moore, Cunningham, Fulton and Hebert2005). We sought to investigate whether our models used to measure self-reported physical and recreational activity engagement were invariant to cognitive status. Those results, reported in Supplemental material, were supportive of measurement invariance for recreational activities, but mixed for physical activities. Thus, it is possible that the results pertaining to physical activities could be influenced by the degree to which cognitively impaired individuals can make valid self-reports about their activity engagement. Similarly, the fit of the recreational activities measurement model to the data was modest. Finding more optimal methods to measure lifespan changes in these activities is an area for continued improvement.

Future research can build upon these results by measuring activity engagement directly and prospectively (e.g., using accelerometers or similar technology). In this study, change was modeled using latent change scores derived from two-time points and, therefore, cannot account for nonlinear changes – which may provide a better fit to the data (van der Zee et al., Reference van der Zee, van der Mee, Bartels and de Geus2019; Wagner et al., Reference Wagner, Grodstein, Proust-Lima and Samieri2020) – in activity engagement over time. Finally, our results were derived from two separate models for physical and recreational activity engagement, which essentially treats these two types of activities as statistically independent. However, it is likely that they share some variance with one another, although the degree to which they share variance may depend on population-level characteristics (e.g., Casaletto et al., Reference Casaletto, Rentería, Pa, Tom, Harrati, Armstrong, Rajan, Mungas, Walters, Kramer and Zahodne2020). Because we did not jointly estimate the influence of both physical and recreational activities together in the same model, it is possible that our results would have changed if all predictors were used in a single model. However, this was statistically untenable given the complexity of the models.

The current results were derived from observational data and are limited with respect to causal inferences. Participants were not randomly selected, nor were they randomly assigned to different levels of activity engagement; in other words, participants self-selected their own physical and recreational activity levels. It is possible that one or more unmeasured variable(s), such as early-life intellectual functioning (Kumpulainen et al., Reference Kumpulainen, Heinonen, Pesonen, Salonen, Andersson, Lano, Wolke, Kajantie, Eriksson and Raikkonen2017) or socioeconomic status (Hua & Brown, Reference Hua and Brown2022), may have predisposed some individuals to be more physically and recreationally active throughout the lifespan, and also predisposed them toward lower risk of cognitive decline through pathways that were (largely) independent from physical and recreational activity participation (e.g., better nutrition, better access to health care, less exposure to environmental toxins). In addition, we cannot exclude reverse causality as a possible explanation for some of our results, as activity engagement may decline as a result of early declines in cognitive functioning (e.g., Sabia et al., Reference Sabia, Dugravot, Dartigues, Abell, Elbaz, Kivimäki and Singh-Manoux2017). Therefore, it is possible that some of our participants may have experienced cognitive decline prior to study enrollment, causing them to become less physically and/or recreationally active. However, because many known modifiable risk factors for dementia can be positively influenced by physical and recreational activity participation – such as depression, diabetes, hypertension, obesity, and social isolation (Livingston et al., Reference Livingston, Huntley, Sommerlad, Ames, Ballard, Banerjee, Brayne, Burns, Cohen-Mansfield, Cooper, Costafreda, Dias, Fox, Gitlin, Howard, Kales, Kivimäki, Larson, Ogunniyi, Orgeta and Mukadam2020) – there are several plausible mechanisms by which mid- and late-life activity engagement could reasonably be assumed to exert a causal influence over late-life cognition (Huai et al., Reference Huai, Xun, Reilly, Wang, Ma and Xi2013; Schuch et al., Reference Schuch, Vancampfort, Richards, Rosenbaum, Ward and Stubbs2016; Teh & Tey, Reference Teh and Tey2019; Wu et al., Reference Wu, Gao, Chen and van Dam2009; Zhu et al., Reference Zhu, Zhang, Chen, Zhao, Ba, Lin, Lu, Yu, Cai, Zhang and Ji2022). Randomized controlled trials may be needed to fully elucidate these causal pathways.

Despite these limitations, the current study has many strengths. We took care to ensure that physical and recreational activity engagement – and their changes – could be measured with equal validity over both self-reported time epochs. We used a well-established latent change score approach to measuring change in activity levels, which ensures that our predictor variables are minimally influenced by measurement error (McArdle, Reference McArdle2009). Extensive, well-validated neuropsychological outcome data were available for participants across many study visits, thus providing excellent characterization of cognitive aging trajectories. The sample was also quite diverse; the ethnoracial composition of our sample was approximately 25% Black and 24% Hispanic/Latinx. Our sample was also diverse in terms of cognitive functioning, clinic vs. community referral source, and primary language spoken. These elements of diversity allow for generalization of our results to a broad segment of the older adult population and allow inferences to be made about a broad spectrum of cognitive functioning, ranging from cognitively healthy to dementia.

We did not conduct separate analyses stratified by ethnic, racial, or language groups. It is possible, however, that physical and recreational activity participation – or one’s self-report of such – could be influenced by cultural or linguistic factors. Future research may wish to explore whether any such differences exist, and if they are relevant to late-life cognition. This same comment could also be made about sex differences. The vast majority of women will experience menopause between 40 (the age used as the first indicator of activity engagement in this study) and 75 (the average baseline participant age in the current study) years of age, thus highlighting the importance of considering sex differences when studying lifestyle and behavior changes across the second half of the lifespan.

In this study, we provide evidence to suggest that self-reported midlife (age 40) engagement in physical and recreational activities is predictive of late-life cognitive functioning. Further, self-reported change in cognitively stimulating recreational activities – more so than change in physical activities – was also highly predictive of future cognitive decline. Our study adds to this large and well-developed body of literature, using a rigorous longitudinal, latent-variable modeling approach to measuring cognitive outcomes and the predictor variables of interest to address how life-course trajectories of recreational and physical activity are associated with late-life cognitive decline. These results, in the context of the broader literature, suggest that both types of activity engagement may contribute to late-life cognitive health when implemented at midlife or perhaps even earlier. Some researchers in this area have theorized that the protective effects of recreational activity engagement on late-life cognition are mediated by cognitive reserve (e.g., Shatenstein et al., Reference Shatenstein, Barberger-Gateau and Mecocci2015). Testing this hypothesized mediation pathway would be an important next step to determine whether midlife recreational activity participation – and change in recreational activity participation beyond age 40 – can in fact build cognitive reserve and, in turn, offer resilience against late-life brain changes.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617723000553.

Funding statement

This work was supported by grants from the National Institute of Aging (P30 AG072972; R01 AG066748; R01 AG031563).

Competing interests

None.

References

Adams, S. A., Matthews, C. E., Ebbeling, C. B., Moore, C. G., Cunningham, J. E., Fulton, J., & Hebert, J. R. (2005). The effect of social desirability and social approval on self-reports of physical activity. American Journal of Epidemiology, 161(4), 389398. https://doi.org/10.1093/aje/kwi054 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Akbaraly, T. N., Portet, F., Fustinoni, S., Dartigues, J-F., Artero, S., Rouaud, O., Touchon, J., Ritchie, K., Berr, C. (2009). Leisure activities and the risk of dementia in the elderly: Results from the three-city study. Neurology, 73(11), 854861. https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181b7849b CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Albert, M. S., DeKosky, S. T., Dickson, D., Dubois, B., Feldman, H. H., Fox, N. C., Gamst, A., Holtzman, D. M., Jagust, W. J., Petersen, R. C., Snyder, P. J., Carrillo, M. C., Thies, B., Phelps, C. H. (2011). The diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment due to alzheimer’s disease: Recommendations from the national institute on aging-alzheimer’s association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimer’s & Dementia, 7(3), 270279. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jalz.2011.03.008 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Andel, R., Silverstein, M., & Kåreholt, I. (2015). The role of midlife occupational complexity and leisure activity in late-life cognition. Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 70(2), 314321.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arenaza-Urquijo, E. M., & Vemuri, P. (2020). Improving the resistance and resilience framework for aging and dementia studies. Alzheimer’s Research & Therapy, 12(1), 41. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13195-020-00609-2 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baumgart, M., Snyder, H. M., Carrillo, M. C., Fazio, S., Kim, H., & Johns, H. (2015). Summary of the evidence on modifiable risk factors for cognitive decline and dementia: A population-based perspective. Alzheimer’s & Dementia, 11(6), 718726. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jalz.2015.05.016 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beckett, M. W., Ardern, C. I., & Rotondi, M. A. (2015). A meta-analysis of prospective studies on the role of physical activity and the prevention of Alzheimer’s disease in older adults. BMC Geriatrics, 15, 9. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12877-015-0007-2 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benedict, C., Brooks, S. J., Kullberg, J., Nordenskjöld, R., Burgos, J., Le Grevès, M., Kilander, L., Larsson, E-M., Johansson, L., Ahlström, Håkan, Lind, L., Schiöth, H. B. (2013). Association between physical activity and brain health in older adults. Neurobiology of Aging, 34(1), 8390. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2012.04.013 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blondell, S. J., Hammersley-Mather, R., & Veerman, J. L. (2014). Does physical activity prevent cognitive decline and dementia?: A systematic review and meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. BMC Public Health, 14(1), 112. https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2458-14-510 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brewster, P. W. H., Melrose, R. J., Marquine, M. D., Johnson, J. K., Napoles, A., MacKay-Brandt, A., Farias, S., Reed, B., Mungas, D. (2014). Life experience and demographic influences on cognitive function in older adults. Neuropsychology, 28(6), 846858. https://doi.org/10.1037/neu0000098 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, B. M., Peiffer, J. J., Taddei, K., Lui, J. K., Laws, S. M., Gupta, V. B., Taddei, T., Ward, V. K., Rodrigues, M. A., Burnham, S., Rainey-Smith, S. R., Villemagne, V. L., Bush, A., Ellis, K. A., Masters, C. L., Ames, D., Macaulay, S. L., Szoeke, C., Rowe, C. C., Martins, R. N., for the AIBL Research Group (2013). Physical activity and amyloid-β plasma and brain levels: Results from the Australian imaging, biomarkers and lifestyle study of ageing. Molecular Psychiatry, 18(8), 875881. https://doi.org/10.1038/mp.2012.107 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, B. M., Rainey-Smith, S. R., Dore, V., Peiffer, J. J., Burnham, S. C., Laws, S. M., Taddei, K., Ames, D., Masters, C. L., Rowe, C. C., Martins, R. N., Villemagne, V. L., Cristina Polidori, M. (2018). Self-reported physical activity is associated with tau burden measured by positron emission tomography. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease, 63(4), 12991305. https://doi.org/10.3233/JAD-170998 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carlson, M. C., Helms, M. J., Steffens, D. C., Burke, J. R., Potter, G. G., & Plassman, B. L. (2008). Midlife activity predicts risk of dementia in older male twin pairs. Alzheimer’s & Dementia, 4(5), 324331. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jalz.2008.07.002 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Casaletto, K. B., Rentería, M. A., Pa, J., Tom, S. E., Harrati, A., Armstrong, N. M., Rajan, K. B., Mungas, D., Walters, S., Kramer, J., Zahodne, L. B. (2020). Late-life physical and cognitive activities independently contribute to brain and cognitive resilience. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease, 74(1), 363376. https://doi.org/10.3233/JAD-191114 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chang, M., Jonsson, P. V., Snaedal, J., Bjornsson, S., Saczynski, J. S., Aspelund, T., Eiriksdottir, G., Jonsdottir, M. K., Lopez, O. L., Harris, T. B., Gudnason, V., Launer, L. J. (2010). The effect of midlife physical activity on cognitive function among older adults: AGES--RReykjavik study. The Journals of Gerontology. Series A, Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences, 65(12), 13691374. https://doi.org/10.1093/gerona/glq152 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Rooij, S. R. (2022). Are brain and cognitive reserve shaped by early life circumstances? Frontiers in Neuroscience, 16, 825811. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2022.825811 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Early, Dé R., Widaman, K. F., Harvey, D., Beckett, L., Park, L. Q., Farias, S. T., Reed, B. R., DeCarli, C., Mungas, D. (2013). Demographic predictors of cognitive change in ethnically diverse older persons. Psychology and Aging, 28(3), 633645. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0031645 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eisenstein, T., Giladi, N., Hendler, T., Havakuk, O., & Lerner, Y. (2021). Physically active lifestyle is associated with attenuation of hippocampal dysfunction in cognitively intact older adults. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, 13, 720990. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2021.720990 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Erickson, K. I., Prakash, R. S., Voss, M. W., Chaddock, L., Hu, L., Morris, K. S., White, S. M., Wójcicki, T. R., McAuley, E., Kramer, A. F. (2009). Aerobic fitness is associated with hippocampal volume in elderly humans. Hippocampus, 19(10), 10301039. https://doi.org/10.1002/hipo.20547 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Erickson, K. I., Raji, C. A., Lopez, O. L., Becker, J. T., Rosano, C., Newman, A. B., Gach, H. M., Thompson, P. M., Ho, A. J., Kuller, L. H. (2010). Physical activity predicts gray matter volume in late adulthood: The cardiovascular health study. Neurology, 75(16), 14151422. https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181f88359 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fletcher, E., Gavett, B., Harvey, D., Farias, S. T., Olichney, J., Beckett, L., DeCarli, C., Mungas, D. (2018). Brain volume change and cognitive trajectories in aging. Neuropsychology, 32(4), 436449. https://doi.org/10.1037/neu0000447 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fransson, E., Knutsson, A., Westerholm, P., & Alfredsson, L. (2008). Indications of recall bias found in a retrospective study of physical activity and myocardial infarction. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology, 61(8), 840847. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jclinepi.2007.09.004 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fratiglioni, L., Paillard-Borg, S., & Winblad, B. (2004). An active and socially integrated lifestyle in late life might protect against dementia. Lancet Neurology, 3(6), 343353. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1474-4422(04)00767-7 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Friedland, R. P., Fritsch, T., Smyth, K. A., Koss, E., Lerner, A. J., Chen, C. H., Petot, G. J., Debanne, S. M. (2001). Patients with Alzheimer’s disease have reduced activities in midlife compared with healthy control-group members. Proceedings of The National Academy of Sciences, 98(6), 34403445. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.061002998 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gavett, B. E., Fletcher, E., Harvey, D., Farias, S. T., Olichney, J., Beckett, L., DeCarli, C., Mungas, D. (2018). Ethnoracial differences in brain structure change and cognitive change. Neuropsychology, 32(5), 529540. https://doi.org/10.1037/neu0000452 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gelman, A., & Rubin, D. B. (1992). Inference from iterative simulation using multiple sequences. Statistical Science, 7(4), 457511. https://doi.org/10.1214/ss/1177011136 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Green, S. B., & Yang, Y. (2009). Reliability of summed item scores using structural equation modeling: An alternative to coefficient alpha. Psychometrika, 74(1), 155167. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11336-008-9099-3 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gross, A. L., Lu, H., Meoni, L., Gallo, J. J., Schrack, J. A., & Sharrett, A. R. (2017). Physical activity in midlife is not associated with cognitive health in later life among ognitively normal older adults. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease, 59(4), 13491358. https://doi.org/10.3233/JAD-170290 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Guiney, H., Lucas, S. J. E., Cotter, J. D., & Machado, L. (2019). Investigating links between habitual physical activity, cerebrovascular function, and cognitive control in healthy older adults. Neuropsychologia, 125, 6269. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.NEUROPSYCHOLOGIA.2019.01.011 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hinton, L., Carter, K., Reed, B. R., Beckett, L., Lara, E., DeCarli, C., & Mungas, D. (2010). Recruitment of a community-based cohort for research on diversity and risk of dementia. Alzheimer Disease and Associated Disorders, 24(3), 234241. https://doi.org/10.1097/wad.0b013e3181c1ee01 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hua, C. L., & Brown, J. S. (2022). Childhood socioeconomic status and physical activity in later life: The role of perceived neighborhood cohesion and wealth in adulthood. Journal of Applied Gerontology, 41(2), 506514. https://doi.org/10.1177/0733464820969312 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huai, P., Xun, H., Reilly, K. H., Wang, Y., Ma, W., & Xi, B. (2013). Physical activity and risk of hypertension: A meta-analysis of prospective cohort studies. Hypertension, 62(6), 10211026. https://doi.org/10.1161/HYPERTENSIONAHA.113.01965 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Iizuka, A., Suzuki, H., Ogawa, S., Kobayashi-Cuya, K. E., Kobayashi, M., Takebayashi, T., & Fujiwara, Y. (2019). Can cognitive leisure activity prevent cognitive decline in older adults? A systematic review of intervention studies. Geriatrics & Gerontology International, 19(6), 469482. https://doi.org/10.1111/ggi.13671 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jonaitis, E., La Rue, A., Mueller, K. D., Koscik, R. L., Hermann, B., & Sager, M. A. (2013). Cognitive activities and cognitive performance in middle-aged adults at risk for Alzheimer’s disease. Psychology and Aging, 28(4), 10041014. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0034838 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kumpulainen, S. M., Heinonen, K., Pesonen, A-K., Salonen, M. K., Andersson, S., Lano, A., Wolke, D., Kajantie, E., Eriksson, J. G., Raikkonen, K. (2017). Childhood cognitive ability and physical activity in young adulthood. Health Psychology, 36(6), 587597. https://doi.org/10.1037/hea0000493 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lieberman, D. E., Kistner, T. M., Richard, D., Lee, I. M., & Baggish, A. L. (2021). The active grandparent hypothesis: Physical activity and the evolution of extended human healthspans and lifespans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 118(50), e2107621118. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2107621118 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lindstrom, H. A., Fritsch, T., Petot, G., Smyth, K. A., Chen, C. H., Debanne, S. M., Lerner, A. J., Friedland, R. P. (2005). The relationships between television viewing in midlife and the development of Alzheimer’s disease in a case-control study. Brain and Cognition, 58(2), 157165. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2004.09.020 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liu, Y., Millsap, R. E., West, S. G., Tein, J. Y., Tanaka, R., & Grimm, K. J. (2017). Testing measurement invariance in longitudinal data with ordered-categorical measures. Psychological Methods, 22(3), 486506. https://doi.org/10.1037/met0000075 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Livingston, G., Huntley, J., Sommerlad, A., Ames, D., Ballard, C., Banerjee, S., Brayne, C., Burns, A., Cohen-Mansfield, J., Cooper, C., Costafreda, S. G., Dias, A., Fox, N., Gitlin, L. N., Howard, R., Kales, H. C., Kivimäki, M., Larson, E. B., Ogunniyi, A., Orgeta, V., …, Mukadam, N. (2020). Dementia prevention, intervention, and care: 2020 report of the lancet commission. Lancet, 396(10248), 413446. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(20)30367-6 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McArdle, J. J. (2009). Latent variable modeling of differences and changes with longitudinal data. Annual Review of Psychology, 60(1), 577605. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.60.110707.163612 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McDonald, R. P. (1999). Test theory: A unified approach. Erlbaum.Google Scholar
McKhann, G. M., Knopman, D. S., Chertkow, H., Hyman, B. T., Jack, C. R. Jr., Kawas, C. H., Klunk, W. E., Koroshetz, W. J., Manly, J. J., Mayeux, R., Mohs, R. C., Morris, J. C., Rossor, M. N., Scheltens, P., Carrillo, M. C., Thies, B., Weintraub, S., Phelps, C. H. (2011). The diagnosis of dementia due to Alzheimer’s disease: Recommendations from the national institute on aging-Alzheimer’s association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimer’s & Dementia, 7(3), 263269. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jalz.2011.03.005 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Melrose, R. J., Brewster, P., Marquine, M. J., MacKay-Brandt, A., Reed, B., Farias, S. T., & Mungas, D. (2015). Early life development in a multiethnic sample and the relation to late life cognition. The Journals of Gerontology. Series B, Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 70(4), 519531. https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbt126 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mungas, D., Reed, B. R., Crane, P. K., Haan, M. N., & González, H. (2004). Spanish and English neuropsychological assessment scales (SENAS): Further development and psychometric characteristics. Psychological Assessment, 16(4), 347359. https://doi.org/10.1037/1040-3590.16.4.347 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mungas, D., Reed, B. R., Haan, M. N., & González, H. (2005). Spanish and English neuropsychological assessment scales: Relationship to demographics, language, cognition, and independent function. Neuropsychology, 19(4), 466475. https://doi.org/10.1037/0894-4105.19.4.466 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mungas, D., Reed, B. R., Marshall, S. C., & González, H. M. (2000). Development of psychometrically matched English and Spanish language neuropsychological tests for older persons. Neuropsychology, 14(2), 209223. https://doi.org/10.1037//0894-4105.14.2.209 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mungas, D., Reed, B. R., Tomaszewski Farias, S., & DeCarli, C. (2005). Criterion-referenced validity of a neuropsychological test battery: Equivalent performance in elderly Hispanics and non-Hispanic whites. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 11(5), 620630. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617705050745 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mungas, D., Widaman, K. F., Reed, B. R., & Tomaszewski Farias, S. (2011). Measurement invariance of neuropsychological tests in diverse older persons. Neuropsychology, 25(2), 260269. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0021090 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (2021). Mplus user’s guide (8th edn). Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén.Google Scholar
Norton, S., Matthews, F. E., Barnes, D. E., Yaffe, K., & Brayne, C. (2014). Potential for primary prevention of Alzheimer’s disease: An analysis of population-based data. Lancet Neurology, 13(8), 788794. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1474-4422(14)70136-X CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
R Core Team. (2023). R: A language and environment for statistical computing [Manual]. R Foundation for Statistical Computing. https://www.R-project.org/ Google Scholar
Reed, B. R., Dowling, M., Tomaszewski Farias, S., Sonnen, J., Strauss, M., Schneider, J. A., Bennett, D. A., Mungas, D. (2011). Cognitive activities during adulthood are more important than education in building reserve. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 17(4), 615624. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617711000014 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rovio, S., Spulber, G., Nieminen, L. J., Niskanen, E., Winblad, B., Tuomilehto, J., Nissinen, A., Soininen, H., & Kivipelto, M. (2010). The effect of midlife physical activity on structural brain changes in the elderly. Neurobiology of aging, 31(11), 19271936. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2008.10.007 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sabia, S., Dugravot, A., Dartigues, J. F., Abell, J., Elbaz, A., Kivimäki, M., & Singh-Manoux, A. (2017). Physical activity, cognitive decline, and risk of dementia: 28 year follow-up of whitehall II cohort study. BMJ (Online), 357, j2709. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.j2709 Google ScholarPubMed
Scarmeas, N., Levy, G., Tang, M. X., Manly, J., & Stern, Y. (2001). Influence of leisure activity on the incidence of Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology, 57(12), 22362242. https://doi.org/10.1212/wnl.57.12.2236 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schneider, S., Junghaenel, D. U., Zelinski, E. M., Meijer, E., Stone, A. A., Langa, K. M., & Kapteyn, A. (2021). Subtle mistakes in self-report surveys predict future transition to dementia. Alzheimer’s & Dementia, 13(1), e12252. https://doi.org/10.1002/dad2.12252 Google ScholarPubMed
Schuch, F. B., Vancampfort, D., Richards, J., Rosenbaum, S., Ward, P. B., & Stubbs, B. (2016). Exercise as a treatment for depression: A meta-analysis adjusting for publication bias. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 77, 4251. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2016.02.023 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schuit, A. J., Feskens, E. J., Launer, L. J., & Kromhout, D. (2001). Physical activity and cognitive decline, the role of the apolipoprotein e4 allele. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise, 33(5), 772777. https://doi.org/10.1097/00005768-200105000-00015 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shatenstein, B., Barberger-Gateau, P., & Mecocci, P. (2015). Prevention of age-related cognitive decline: Which strategies, when, and for whom? Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease, 48(1), 3553. https://doi.org/10.3233/JAD-150256 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sofi, F., Valecchi, D., Bacci, D., Abbate, R., Gensini, G. F., Casini, A., & Macchi, C. (2011). Physical activity and risk of cognitive decline: A meta-analysis of prospective studies. Journal of Internal Medicine, 269(1), 107117. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2796.2010.02281.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Steffener, J., Habeck, C., O’Shea, D., Razlighi, Q., Bherer, L., & Stern, Y. (2016). Differences between chronological and brain age are related to education and self-reported physical activity. Neurobiology of Aging, 40, 138144. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2016.01.014 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stern, Y., Albert, M., Barnes, C. A., Cabeza, R., Pascual-Leone, A., & Rapp, P. R. (2023). A framework for concepts of reserve and resilience in aging. Neurobiology of Aging, 124, 100103. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2022.10.015 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stern, Y., Arenaza‐Urquijo, E. M., Bartrés‐Faz, D., Belleville, S., Cantilon, M., Chetelat, G., Ewers, M., Franzmeier, N., Kempermann, G., Kremen, W. S., Okonkwo, O., Scarmeas, N., Soldan, A., Udeh‐Momoh, C., Valenzuela, M., Vemuri, P., Vuoksimaa, E., and the Reserve, Resilience and Protective Factors PIA Empirical Definitions and Conceptual Frameworks Workgroup (2020). Whitepaper: Defining and investigating cognitive reserve, brain reserve, and brain maintenance. Alzheimer’s & Dementia, 16(9), 13051311. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jalz.2018.07.219 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sutphen, C. L., Jasielec, M. S., Shah, A. R., Macy, E. M., Xiong, C., Vlassenko, A. G., Benzinger, T. L. S., Stoops, E. E. J., Vanderstichele, H. M. J., Brix, B., Darby, H. D., Vandijck, M. L. J., Ladenson, J. H., Morris, J. C., Holtzman, D. M., Fagan, A. M. (2015). Longitudinal cerebrospinal fluid biomarker changes in preclinical Alzheimer disease during middle age. JAMA Neurology, 72(9), 10291042. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamaneurol.2015.1285 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Teh, J., & Tey, N. P. (2019). Effects of selected leisure activities on preventing loneliness among older Chinese. SSM - Population Health, 9, 100479. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ssmph.2019.100479 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tolppanen, A‐Maija, Solomon, A., Kulmala, J., Kåreholt, I., Ngandu, T., Rusanen, M., Laatikainen, T., Soininen, H., Kivipelto, M. (2015). Leisure-time physical activity from mid- to late life, body mass index, and risk of dementia. Alzheimer’s & Dementia, 11(4), 434443.e6. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jalz.2014.01.008 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tucker-Drob, E. M., Brandmaier, A. M., & Lindenberger, U. (2019). Coupled cognitive changes in adulthood: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 145(3), 273301. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000179 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van der Zee, M. D., van der Mee, D., Bartels, M., & de Geus, E. (2019). Tracking of voluntary exercise behaviour over the lifespan. The International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity, 16(1), 17. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12966-019-0779-4 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wagner, M., Grodstein, F., Proust-Lima, C., & Samieri, C. (2020). Long-term trajectories of body weight, diet, and physical activity from midlife through late life and subsequent cognitive decline in women. American Journal of Epidemiology, 189(4), 305313. https://doi.org/10.1093/aje/kwz262 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Williams, B. D., Pendleton, N., & Chandola, T. (2020). Cognitively stimulating activities and risk of probable dementia or cognitive impairment in the English longitudinal study of ageing. SSM Population Health, 12, 100656. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ssmph.2020.100656 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilson, R. S., Mendes De Leon, C. F., Barnes, L. L., Schneider, J. A., Bienias, J. L., Evans, D. A., & Bennett, D. A. (2002). Participation in cognitively stimulating activities and risk of incident Alzheimer disease. Journal of the American Medical Association, 287(6), 742748. https://doi.org/10.1001/jama.287.6.742 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wu, T., Gao, X., Chen, M., & van Dam, R. M. (2009). Long-term effectiveness of diet-plus-exercise interventions vs. diet-only interventions for weight loss: A meta-analysis. Obesity Reviews, 10(3), 313323. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-789X.2008.00547.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yanagida, T. (2023). misty: Miscellaneous functions “T. Yanagida” [Manual]. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=misty Google Scholar
Yates, L. A., Ziser, S., Spector, A., & Orrell, M. (2016). Cognitive leisure activities and future risk of cognitive impairment and dementia: Systematic review and meta-analysis. International Psychogeriatrics, 28(11), 17911806.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhu, X., Zhang, F., Chen, J., Zhao, Y., Ba, T., Lin, C., Lu, Y., Yu, T., Cai, X., Zhang, L., Ji, L. (2022). The effects of supervised exercise training on weight control and other metabolic outcomes in patients with type 2 diabetes: A meta-analysis. International Journal of Sport Nutrition and Exercise Metabolism, 32(3), 186194. https://doi.org/10.1123/ijsnem.2021-0168 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Figure 1. Latent change score model for physical activities. Arrows labeled with λ indicate that the factor loadings for the corresponding indicators were constrained to be equal across age epochs. Asterisks indicate freely estimated parameters. The residual autocorrelations between the same indicator variables (e.g., light work) at age 40 and current (C) were modeled as one latent variable per item pair, but only shown once (in dashed lines) for simplicity.

Figure 1

Figure 2. Latent change score model for recreational activities. Arrows labeled with λ indicate that the factor loadings for the corresponding indicators were constrained to be equal across age epochs. Asterisks indicate freely estimated parameters. The residual autocorrelations between the same indicator variables (e.g., reading) at age 40 and current (C) were modeled as one latent variable per item pair, but only shown once (in dashed lines) for simplicity.

Figure 2

Figure 3. Multilevel model path diagram showing illustrating our approach to hypothesis testing. In the within part of the model (panel A), domain-specific random intercepts and slopes are defined as a function of time. In the between part of the model (panel B), domain-specific cognitive intercepts and the global cognitive slope are regressed on activity factors (age 40 and change) and covariates.

Figure 3

Table 1. Participant demographics at initial visit

Figure 4

Figure 4. Model-predicted verbal episodic memory scores plotted as a function of time (x-axis), activity level at age 40 (colored lines), change in activity level over time (vertical facets), and activity type (horizontal facets). Data are based on a hypothetical 70-year-old non-Hispanic White English-speaking woman with 12 years of education who was a community referral (reference participant).

Figure 5

Table 2. Physical activities model results

Figure 6

Table 3. Recreational activities model results

Supplementary material: PDF

Gavett et al. supplementary material

Gavett et al. supplementary material

Download Gavett et al. supplementary material(PDF)
PDF 212.1 KB