Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-x4r87 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-28T00:31:10.768Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Outcome evaluation of fruits and vegetables distribution interventions in schools: a systematic review and meta-analysis

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  19 April 2021

Mariam R Ismail
Affiliation:
School of Health and Rehabilitation Sciences, Western University, London, ON, Canada
Jamie A Seabrook
Affiliation:
School of Food and Nutritional Sciences, Brescia University College; Department of Pediatrics, Department of Epidemiology & Biostatistics, Western University; Children’s Health Research Institute, Lawson Health Research Institute, London, ON, Canada
Jason A Gilliland*
Affiliation:
Department of Geography & Environment, School of Health Studies, Department of Pediatrics, Department of Epidemiology & Biostatistics, Western University; Children’s Health Research Institute, Lawson Health Research Institute, 1151 Richmond Street, London, ONN6A 3K7, Canada
*
*Corresponding author: Email jgillila@uwo.ca
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Objective:

Fruits and vegetables (FV) distribution interventions have been implemented as a public health strategy to increase children’s intake of FV at school settings. The purpose of this review was to examine whether snack-based FV distribution interventions can improve school-aged children’s consumption of FV.

Design:

Systematic review and meta-analysis of articles published in English, in a peer-reviewed journals, were identified by searching six databases up to August 2020. Standardised mean differences (SMD) and 95 % CI were calculated using a random effects model. Heterogeneity was quantified using I2 statistics.

Setting:

Population-based studies of interventions where the main focus was the effectiveness of distributed FV as snacks to schoolchildren in North America, Europe and Pacific were included.

Results:

Forty-seven studies, reporting on fifteen different interventions, were identified; ten studies were included in the meta-analysis. All interventions were effective in increasing children’s consumption of FV, with only one intervention demonstrating a null effect. Pooled results under all classifications showed effectiveness in improving children’s consumption of FV, particularly for multi-component interventions at post-intervention (SMD 0·20, 95 % CI 0·13, 0·27) and free distribution interventions at follow-up (SMD 0·19, 95 % CI 0·12, 0·27).

Conclusions:

Findings suggest that FV distribution interventions provide a promising avenue by which children’s consumption can be improved. Nonetheless, our results are based on a limited number of studies, and further studies should be performed to confirm these results. More consistent measurement protocols in terms of rigorous study methodologies, intervention duration and follow-up evaluation are needed to improve comparability across studies.

Type
Review Article
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of The Nutrition Society

Fruits and vegetables (FV) are important components of a healthy diet, and sufficient daily consumption is associated with nutritional adequacy(Reference Taylor, Evers and McKenna1,Reference Rolls, Ello-Martin and Carlton Tohill2) and the prevention of the majority of non-communicable chronic diseases(3). Recommended consumption of FV for children aged 4 to 13 years is five to six servings;(Reference Garriguet4) however, children are consuming less than the recommended amounts(Reference Dennison, Rockwell and Baker5Reference Polsky and Garriguet9). Low FV consumption in children is concerning, considering that dietary habits established in childhood tend to carry into later adulthood(Reference Dennison, Rockwell and Baker5,Reference Kelder, Perry and Klepp10,Reference Krebs-Smith, Heimendinger and Patterson11) , thus making childhood an opportune time for health promotion initiatives to instill healthy dietary behaviours.

Schools are the optimal setting for implementing health-promoting interventions because of the amount of time children spend in school, as well as the large percentage of food consumption that occurs during school hours(Reference Baxter, Milner and Hawkins12,Reference Budd and Volpe13) . It is reasonable to suggest then, that with significant time allotment, the school system has a responsibility to enhance the health and well-being of children. In addition, a large number of children can be reached through schools, regardless of their ethnicity, socio-economic background and/or nutritional status, thus reducing social inequalities(Reference Knai, Pomerleau and Lock14). Given that low FV intake is one of the lifestyle factors that may contribute to the health inequalities within society, providing/distributing FV to children within the school environment has the potential to reduce social inequalities(Reference Knai, Pomerleau and Lock14,Reference Hughes, Edwards and Clarke15) .

Numerous systematic reviews aimed at increasing children’s consumption of FV have been conducted; however, most have been conducted in only one region;(Reference Van Cauwenberghe, Maes and Spittaels16) using only one study design(Reference Delgado-Noguera, Tort and Martinez-Zapata17,Reference Evans, Christian and Cleghorn18) ; with children under 5 years of age(Reference Hodder, Stacey and O’Brien19); or using a broad scope of intervention strategies(Reference Hendrie, Lease and Bowen20Reference Triador, Farmer and Maximova26). None, to our knowledge, have focused on FV distribution-based interventions that address the strategies of availability and accessibility – two important environmental mediators that have been identified as consistent and positive predictors of children’s FV consumption(Reference de Sa and Lock21,Reference Blanchette and Brug27,Reference Rasmussen, Krolner and Klepp28) . While availability is defined as the presence of FV in the home or school environment, accessibility is defined as FV that are prepared, presented and/or maintained in a form that enables or motivates children to consume them (e.g. cutting up FV or designating time to eat FV)(Reference Blanchette and Brug27).

With the rapid influx of research on school food programming, a synthesis of the literature on this age group is needed. As such, the aim of this systematic review and meta-analysis was to evaluate the effectiveness of FV distribution interventions as a snack on school-aged children’s intake of FV. Primarily, the review focuses on studies that provided children with readily accessible and available nutritious FV during school hours as snacks (outside of breakfast or lunch time), as most of these programmes were conducted in a non-canteen system where no school-supplied or provided meals are offered. Additionally, results are pooled in a meta-analysis, which quantifies the evidence provided by the different studies, giving a precise estimate of the effect, and increasing the generalisability of the individual studies. Additionally, conducting such analyses would guide the design of future snack-based FV distribution-based interventions and would provide valuable findings to inform future research, practice and policy.

Methods

The authors followed the PRISMA (Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses) guidelines during all stages of design, implementation and reporting(Reference Moher, Liberati and Tetzlaff29).

Search strategy

Relevant studies were identified by searching PUBMED, ProQuest, EMBASE, CINAHL, Web of Science Core Collection and Scopus databases. The initial database searches were conducted in February 2019, with an updated search in August 2020. No date limit, language or geographic location restriction was applied; however, the search primarily yielded studies from the last 20 years. The search strategy was designed to be comprehensive by including different keywords selected from previously published literature in the area of school food programming. In consultation with an experienced librarian and informed by published literature in this area, searches were carried out combining four different search arms: (school* OR ‘school-based’) AND (intervention* OR program* OR scheme* OR campaign* OR initiative*OR project*) AND (fruit* OR vegetable*) AND (provision OR subsidised OR distribution OR free OR availability OR exposure OR accessibility). This method was adapted when Medical Subject Headings (MeSH) terms were not available. One reviewer screened the titles of the studies and imported all relevant titles into a citation manger (Mendeley v1.17.10). Duplicates were then removed and from the remaining studies, and abstract screening was completed independently by two reviewers. For any potentially relevant studies, full texts were assessed for eligibility independently by two reviewers. Once eligible studies were identified, a manual search of the reference lists of the included studies was conducted to identify any missed relevant studies. If consensus could not be achieved between the two independent reviewers, the senior corresponding author discussed, elucidated and resolved the adjudication process with the reviewers.

Study selection

To be included in the present review, studies needed to meet the following eligibility criteria: Population: school-aged children aged 4–14 years; Intervention: FV distribution as a snack solely or combined with another intervention approach (e.g. nutrition education, parental involvement) within the school environment; Comparator: no intervention (control) or an alternative intervention; and Outcome: FV consumption. All study designs were considered. Studies were excluded if they were reviews, conference proceedings/abstracts, design protocols or studies that reported on interventions that used other intervention approaches to increase children’s consumption of FV.

Data extraction and abstraction

The following information was extracted from each study: (1) basic identifying information about the study (authors, year of publication, programme name and country); (2) participants; (3) study design; (4) intervention group(s); (5) data collection methods and (6) findings. The Effective Public Health Practice Project (EPHPP) tool was used to assess the quality of each study on six criteria: selection bias, study design, confounding, blinding, data collection methods and withdrawals, and dropouts. Each criterion was rated as strong, moderate or weak, and these ratings were summed to obtain an overall score for each study. A ‘strong’ quality study had no weak rating, a ‘moderate’ quality study had one weak rating and a ‘weak’ quality study had two or more weak ratings(30). Each study was rated independently by two reviewers and disagreements were amended following discussion. In remaining cases of disagreement or uncertainty, the senior corresponding author discussed and resolved final scoring with the two independent reviewers.

Data synthesis

As FV consumption was assessed using multiple methods, the effect size for the meta-analysis was measured as a standardised mean difference (SMD) with a 95 % CI. We used SMD because the primary outcome was continuous, and we expected some variability in the way outcomes were measured. Heterogeneity was assessed using the I 2 statistic, which describes the proportion of total variation attributable to between-study heterogeneity(Reference Egger, Davey Smith and Altman31). I 2 values <30 % were considered to be low, values between 30 % and 50 % were considered to be low to moderate, values between 50 % and 75 % were considered to be moderate to high, and values >75 % were considered to be high(Reference Egger, Davey Smith and Altman31). I 2 values >50 % indicate that caution should be used when drawing conclusions from the data(Reference Higgins, Thompson and Deeks32). A random effects model was used to estimate the SMD in FV consumption because of its ability to statistically control for heterogeneity and to provide for wider 95 % CI than the fixed-effects model when significant heterogeneity is expected.

To be considered for inclusion in the meta-analysis, studies needed to provide the standard deviation (or sufficient information to calculate these) and the sample size. Where information on FV consumption in grams was missing, it was assumed that one portion of fruit and/or vegetable was equivalent to 100 g(3,Reference Bere, Veierod and Bjelland33,Reference Ovrebo, Stea and Te Velde34) . When standard error was reported in place of standard deviation, standard deviation were estimated using sd = √n x (upper limit – lower limit)/3·92 where n is the number of participants in each group(Reference Higgins and Deeks35). If interested outcomes were presented as interquartile range (IQR), standard deviation was calculated using IQR/1·35. This is generally only possible when the data are normally distributed. Given IQR is typically only reported in lieu of standard deviation when the data are non-normal, standard deviation was recorded in the data set for this study, assuming a normal distribution(Reference Hozo, Djulbegovic and Hozo36). Pooled standard deviations(Reference Higgins, Thompson and Deeks32) were estimated for two studies(Reference Tak, Te Velde and Brug37,Reference Tak, Te Velde and Brug38) , and studies with multiple intervention arms(Reference Bere, Hilsen and Klepp39,Reference Bere, Veierod and Klepp40) were combined to estimate the sample size, mean and standard deviation using the method described by Higgins(Reference Higgins and Deeks35). One study reported the total sample size but did not provide the sample size for each group. In this case, the sample size was estimated by assuming equal numbers of children in each group and the study was included(Reference Bere, Hilsen and Klepp39).

To evaluate the influence of each study on the overall effect size, sensitivity analysis was conducted using the leave-one-out method (i.e. removing a single study at a time and repeating the analysis)(Reference Sahebkar41,Reference Zhang, Zeng and Cao42) . All analyses were conducted using Review Manager 5.3 (The Cochrane Corp.).

Results

Literature search

Of the 5413 titles retrieved, 129 remained after title screening and removal of duplicates. Abstract screening left seventy-seven studies, as fifty-two did not meet the pre-specified eligibility criteria. Full-text screening left thirty-four studies, as forty-three did not meet the eligibility criteria. An additional thirteen studies were identified (nine from a reference list of the included studies; three from contacting the authors and one from a review paper) (Fig. 1). Following an update of the search (for articles published after February 2019), two additional studies from online database searches met the inclusion criteria. In total, this search identified forty-seven studies, all of which were included in the qualitative synthesis to give a comprehensive overview of published research in this study area. However, only ten studies met criteria to be included in the quantitative synthesis. The remaining studies (n 37) were not included in the meta-analysis because of various factors: necessary information could not be obtained (i.e. no control group(Reference Coyle, Potter and Schneider43Reference Jorgensen, Rasmussen and Aarestrup56); control is another intervention(Reference Horne, Hardman and Lowe57,Reference Horne, Tapper and Lowe58) ; unstandardised effect size(Reference Ransley, Taylor and Radwan49,Reference Reinaerts, Crutzen and Candel59,Reference Ransley, Greenwood and Cade60) ; not an actual consumption(Reference Hughes, Edwards and Clarke15,Reference Methner, Maschkowski and Hartmann61Reference Gates, Hanning and Gates68) and no sample size)(Reference Hector, Edwards and Gale69); tracking studies (i.e. dietary intervention initiated in childhood and tracked/followed up into adulthood)(Reference Ovrebo, Stea and Te Velde34,Reference Bere, te Velde and Smastuen70) and studies were rated as weak(Reference Ashfield-Watt, Stewart and Scheffer71Reference Bere, Veierod and Skare77). While seventeen out of the forty-seven studies provided enough information to be included in the quantitative synthesis, seven out of those seventeen studies were excluded due to being rated weak, leaving ten studies in the quantitative analyses to provide meaningful, rigorous conclusions.

Fig. 1 Flow diagram of search strategy and review process based on PRISMA statement. PRISMA, Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses

Study characteristics

The studies were predominantly conducted in Europe (n 30), North America (n 15) and Pacific (n 2), reporting on fifteen different interventions, published between 2003 and 2019. Study designs varied, and where reported, sample sizes ranged from 1 to 38 schools, 2–50 classes or <100 to >1000 children. The duration of the intervention ranged from <1 month (n 4) to > 1 month (n 43), while frequency of exposure ranged from <5x (n 12) to 5x a week (n 35) (see online supplemental Table 1). Follow-up of the studies ranged from 1 to 14 years, and four of the forty-seven studies had a follow-up of less than 1 year. In the forty-seven studies, twelve were classified as randomised controlled trials, twenty were classified as controlled clinical trials, eleven were classified as cohort trials (pre-post) and four were classified as cross-sectional studies. All intervention studies distributed free FV as a snack during breaktime within the school environment, with the exception of three studies(Reference Bere, Veierod and Bjelland33,Reference Bere, Hilsen and Klepp39,Reference Bere, Veierod and Klepp40) in which FV were provided at parental costs (subsidised). The majority of the studies distributed solely FV as a snack (i.e. stand-alone intervention), whereas some studies in addition to providing FV, integrated other supplementary/reinforcement components such as nutrition education(Reference Ovrebo, Stea and Te Velde34,Reference Tak, Te Velde and Brug38,Reference Ransley, Greenwood and Cade60,Reference Roccaldo, Censi and D’Addezio67,Reference Bere, Veierod and Bjelland78) , parental involvement(Reference Bere, Veierod and Bjelland33,Reference Jorgensen, Jorgensen and Aarestrup44,Reference Ransley, Taylor and Radwan49,Reference Jorgensen, Rasmussen and Aarestrup56,Reference Gates, Hanning and Gates68,Reference Te Velde, Brug and Wind79) , peer modelling and rewards(Reference Clarke, Ruxton and Hetherington46,Reference Lowe, Horne and Tapper50,Reference Horne, Hardman and Lowe57,Reference Horne, Tapper and Lowe58) . Most of the study interventions were guided by the constructs of social cognitive theory (SCT) (n 13)(Reference Bandura80), Intervention Mapping Protocol (IMP) (n 7)(Reference Bartholomew Eldrigde, Markham and Ruiter81) and utilisation-focused Participatory Approach(Reference Rossi, Lipsey and Freeman82).

Assessment of study quality resulted in twelve studies that were rated as strong, twelve studies as moderate and twenty-three studies as having a weak quality. The primary reason for assigning a rating of ‘weak’ was that these studies lacked adequate information (i.e. under-reporting and/or lack of clarity) in the published manuscript to fulfil on all quality criteria (i.e. selection bias, blinding, confounders, withdrawals and dropouts) (Fig. 2).

Fig. 2 Summary of study quality assessment using the Effective Public Health Practice Project (EPHPP) quality assessment tool for quantitative studies. Criteria Scale: 1 – strong, 2 – moderate, 3 – weak, N/A – not applicable. Global Rating System: 1 – strong (no weak ratings), 2 – moderate (one weak rating), 3 – weak (two or more weak ratings). QA Tool accessible through http://www.ephpp.ca/PDF/Quality%20Assessment%20Tool_2010_2.pdf

Effects of interventions

Even though the measurement of FV consumption was often combined, making it difficult to determine the effectiveness with respect to each, all studies noted that fruits were served more frequently than vegetables. As a result, this review defined a positive outcome as having a measurable effect on children’s consumption of FV at either post-intervention (i.e. immediately following the end of an intervention), follow-up (i.e. after a period of time from the end of an intervention) or both time points. Conversely, a negative or null outcome indicates no effect on children’s consumption of FV. Given the considerable heterogeneity/variability across the studies in terms of intervention characteristics (i.e. study design, intervention duration, follow-up length, distribution frequency, geographical location, type of FV served and diet assessment methodology), synthesis of the results was challenging. Thus, we used SMD to account for heterogeneity and therefore, evidence synthesis was established a priori subgroups by stratifying/classifying studies on three principal outcome summary measures: intervention sustainability (i.e. post-intervention or follow-up), approach of intervention (i.e. stand-alone, or multi-component) and type of distribution (free or subsidised).

Intervention sustainability

Twenty-six out of forty-seven studies measured FV consumption among children, reported an increase in consumption at post-intervention(Reference Hughes, Edwards and Clarke15,Reference Bere, Hilsen and Klepp39,Reference Bere, Veierod and Klepp40,Reference Coyle, Potter and Schneider43,Reference Yeo and Edwards45Reference Lowe, Horne and Tapper50,Reference White53,Reference Ovrum and Bere54,Reference Jorgensen, Rasmussen and Aarestrup56,Reference Horne, Hardman and Lowe57,Reference Ransley, Greenwood and Cade60Reference Hass, Lischetzke and Hartmann63,Reference Jamelske, Bica and McCarty66Reference Hector, Edwards and Gale69,Reference Eriksen, Haraldsdottir and Pederson72,Reference He, Beynon and Sangster Bouck74,Reference Olsho, Klerman and Ritchie83,Reference Jean Naylor84) , while only three studies reported an increase in FV consumption at follow-up(Reference Jorgensen, Jorgensen and Aarestrup44,Reference Bica and Jamelske64,Reference Jamelske and Bica65) . The remaining sixteen studies reported an increase in FV consumption at post-intervention, with a loss of effectiveness at follow-up (i.e. not sustainable)(Reference Ovrebo, Stea and Te Velde34,Reference Tak, Te Velde and Brug37,Reference Tak, Te Velde and Brug38,Reference Gates, Hanning and Gates51,Reference Gates, Hanning and Gates52,Reference Horne, Tapper and Lowe58,Reference Reinaerts, Crutzen and Candel59,Reference Bere, te Velde and Smastuen70,Reference Ashfield-Watt, Stewart and Scheffer71,Reference Fogarty, Antoniak and Venn73,Reference Skinner, Hanning and Metatawabin75Reference Bere, Veierod and Bjelland78) , except for two studies(Reference Bere, Veierod and Bjelland33,Reference Te Velde, Brug and Wind79) in which there were null effects at both time points(Reference Bere, Veierod and Bjelland33), and an increase in FV at both time points (sustainable)(Reference Te Velde, Brug and Wind79).

Pooled analysis was performed with nine studies (11 322 participants)(Reference Bere, Veierod and Bjelland33,Reference Tak, Te Velde and Brug38Reference Bere, Veierod and Klepp40,Reference Bere, Veierod and Bjelland78,Reference Te Velde, Brug and Wind79,Reference Olsho, Klerman and Ritchie83Reference Reinaerts, de Nooijer and Candel85) . Significant differences were found between intervention and control groups at post-intervention (SMD 0·17, 95 %, CI 0·07, 0·26; I 2 = 81 %, P = 0·0006). Pooled analysis was performed with four studies (3085 participants)(Reference Bere, Veierod and Bjelland33,Reference Tak, Te Velde and Brug37,Reference Bere, Veierod and Bjelland78,Reference Te Velde, Brug and Wind79) . Significant differences were found between intervention and control groups at follow-up (SMD 0·14, 95 % CI 0·04, 0·25, I 2 = 50 %, P = 0·008) (Fig. 3).

Fig. 3 School-based interventions to promote fruit and vegetable consumption. Meta-analysis of intervention sustainability at post-intervention and at follow-up. (Standardised mean differences and 95 % CI). 1Measurement at post-intervention time point; 2measurement at follow-up time point

Intervention approach

Among the forty-seven studies, twenty-seven distributed FV solely (referred to as stand-alone interventions), while the remaining twenty supplied FV along with another supplementary component (referred to as multi-component interventions). Nine studies distributed FV with nutrition education alone(Reference Ovrebo, Stea and Te Velde34,Reference Tak, Te Velde and Brug37,Reference Tak, Te Velde and Brug38,Reference Tussing-Humphreys, Thomson and McCabe-Sellers48,Reference Woodruff55,Reference Ransley, Greenwood and Cade60,Reference Roccaldo, Censi and D’Addezio67,Reference He, Beynon and Sangster Bouck74,Reference Bere, Veierod and Bjelland78) , while five studies included FV distribution, nutrition education and another supplementary component(Reference Ransley, Taylor and Radwan49,Reference Jorgensen, Rasmussen and Aarestrup56,Reference Gold, Larson and Tucker62,Reference Gates, Hanning and Gates68,Reference Te Velde, Brug and Wind79) . The remaining five multi-component studies distributed FV in combination with peer modelling and rewards(Reference Clarke, Ruxton and Hetherington46,Reference Lowe, Horne and Tapper50,Reference Horne, Hardman and Lowe57,Reference Horne, Tapper and Lowe58) , and parental involvement(Reference Jorgensen, Jorgensen and Aarestrup44). All studies reported a positive effect on children’s FV consumption, except for one multi-component intervention(Reference Bere, Veierod and Bjelland33), where a null effect was reported, despite including FV distribution, nutrition education and parental involvement.

Pooled analysis was performed with five studies (8028 participants)(Reference Bere, Hilsen and Klepp39,Reference Bere, Veierod and Klepp40,Reference Olsho, Klerman and Ritchie83Reference Reinaerts, de Nooijer and Candel85) . Significant differences were found between stand-alone interventions and control groups at post-intervention (SMD 0·18, 95 % CI 0·02, 0·34, I 2 = 89 %, P = 0·03). As for stand-alone intervention at follow-up, no summary estimate was found due to the absence of studies reported under this classification.

With respect to multi-component interventions, pooled analysis was performed with four studies (3294 participants)(Reference Bere, Veierod and Bjelland33,Reference Tak, Te Velde and Brug38,Reference Bere, Veierod and Bjelland78,Reference Te Velde, Brug and Wind79) and four studies (3085 participants)(Reference Bere, Veierod and Bjelland33,Reference Tak, Te Velde and Brug37,Reference Bere, Veierod and Bjelland78,Reference Te Velde, Brug and Wind79) at both post-intervention and follow-up. Significant differences were found between multi-component interventions and control group at post-intervention (SMD 0·20, 95 % CI 0·13, 0·27, I 2 = 0 %, P < 0·00001) and at follow-up (SMD 0·14, 95 % CI 0·04, 0·25, I 2 = 50 %, P = 0·008) (Fig. 4).

Fig. 4 School-based interventions to promote fruit and vegetable consumption. Meta-analysis of intervention approach (stand-alone or multi-component) at post-intervention and at follow-up. (Standardised mean differences and 95 % CI). 1Measurement at post-intervention time point; 2measurement at follow-up time point

Type of distribution

A total of forty-one out of forty-seven studies distributed FV at no parental cost (free), while the remaining six studies distributed FV either at a parental cost (subsidised)(Reference de Sa and Lock21,Reference Bere, Veierod and Bjelland33) or a combination(Reference Bere, Hilsen and Klepp39,Reference Bere, Veierod and Klepp40,Reference Bere, Veierod and Bjelland78,Reference Te Velde, Brug and Wind79) . All studies demonstrated a positive effect on school-aged children’s FV consumption, except for one study(Reference Bere, Veierod and Bjelland33) in which FV were provided at a subsidised cost.

Pooled analyses were performed with eight studies (10 363 participants)(Reference Tak, Te Velde and Brug38Reference Bere, Veierod and Klepp40,Reference Bere, Veierod and Bjelland78,Reference Te Velde, Brug and Wind79,Reference Olsho, Klerman and Ritchie83Reference Reinaerts, de Nooijer and Candel85) and three studies (2716 participants)(Reference Tak, Te Velde and Brug37,Reference Bere, Veierod and Bjelland78,Reference Te Velde, Brug and Wind79) both at post-intervention and follow-up. Significant differences were found between free intervention and control groups at both post-intervention (SMD 0·20, 95 % CI 0·09, 0·30, I 2 = 83 %, P = 0·0003) and follow-up (SMD 0·19, 95 %, CI 0·12, 0·27, I 2 = 0 %, P < 0·00001) (Fig. 5).

Fig. 5 School-based interventions to promote fruit and vegetable consumption. Meta-analysis of intervention type of distribution (free and subsidised) at post-intervention and at follow-up. (Standardised mean differences and 95 % CI). 1Measurement at post-intervention time point; 2measurement at follow-up time point

As for subsidised interventions, pooled analyses were performed with three studies (1798 participants)(Reference Bere, Veierod and Bjelland33,Reference Bere, Hilsen and Klepp39,Reference Bere, Veierod and Klepp40) at post-intervention only. This is because one study was indicated at follow-up measurement and, as a result, pooled analysis cannot be conducted(Reference Bere, Veierod and Bjelland33). No significant differences were found between subsidised intervention and control groups at post-intervention (SMD 0·02, 95 % CI -0·12, 0·16, I 2 = 47 %, P = 0·75) (Fig. 5).

Exploration of heterogeneity

Heterogeneity among the included studies was significantly reduced when sensitivity analysis was applied for all principal outcome summary, apart from classifications reported under stand-alone and multi-component post-intervention and free distribution at follow-up. In particular, the statistically significant effect size for the impact of FV distribution interventions was found to be sensitive to the studies eliminated, except for studies pooled under the classification of stand-alone at post-intervention in which elimination of the studies did not influence the robustness of the calculated effect size (I 2 = 90 %, P = 0·001). This indicates the significant heterogeneity among the limited number of studies included under this classification. As for multi-component interventions at post-intervention and free distribution interventions at follow-up, sensitivity analysis could not be applied as the heterogeneity among studies were null. This was evident by the calculated effect size of (I 2 = 0 %, P = 0·52) for studies under the multi-component at post-intervention classification and (I 2 = 0 %, P = 0·98) for studies under the classification of free distribution at follow-up. As for the classifications of subsidised and stand-alone interventions at follow-up, sensitivity analysis could not be applied because of the absence of studies reported under these classifications. Notwithstanding the application of a random effects approach, the overall rate of heterogeneity was high, and the majority of the studies contributed to this heterogeneity (see online supplemental Table 2).

Discussion

This was the first systematic review and meta-analysis to explore the effectiveness of snack-based FV distribution interventions to promote school-aged children’s consumption of FV. The findings demonstrated the positive effects that distributing FV as a snack within the school environment can have on children’s consumption of FV, particularly fruit consumption. Nonetheless, this outcome may not be surprising given that children have more access, exposure and repeated opportunities to try new FV, which are all factors that have been shown to improve children’s consumption of FV(Reference Cooke86Reference Laureati, Bergamaschi and Pagliarini90).

The preference for fruit is consistent with studies that demonstrate an increased consumption for fruit in school-aged children(Reference Brug, Tak and te Velde87,Reference Mennella and Bobowski88,Reference Laureati, Bergamaschi and Pagliarini90) . For example, among the included studies, there appears to be a greater impact on children’s fruit than vegetable consumption. There are several reasons why this could be the case. First, most of the studies were of European origin, where it is the social norm to consume fruit as a snack and vegetables at main meals(Reference Bere, Veierod and Bjelland33,Reference Ransley, Taylor and Radwan49,Reference Ashfield-Watt, Stewart and Scheffer71) . Second, fruit was more frequently served to children to avoid waste and maintain their interests(Reference Coyle, Potter and Schneider43,Reference Jamelske and Bica47,Reference Potter, Schneider and Coyle91) , contributing to unequal exposure opportunities for behavioural change to occur with respect to each(Reference Ransley, Greenwood and Cade60). Third, the most frequently served vegetables were celery sticks, carrots or cherry tomatoes because of ease of preparation and distribution, which might induce feelings of boredom as children were exposed to the same stimuli, which can lead to lower preference and consumption of vegetables(Reference Bere, Veierod and Klepp40,Reference Olsen, Ritz and Kraaij92) . Finally, most of these programmes were of short-term duration that could easily impact the dietary behaviour of fruit consumption compared to vegetables, which usually take a long time to influence(Reference Coyle, Potter and Schneider43,Reference Gold, Larson and Tucker62,Reference Bica and Jamelske64) . Taken together, these findings indicate that changing children’s dietary habits of vegetables consumption is a difficult proposition, and future studies should consider adequate level of exposure to a variety of vegetables to maintain long-lasting effects on changing the dietary behaviours of vegetable consumption.

Our meta-analysis shows effectiveness at increasing children’s consumption of FV when pooling studies according to two time points (i.e. post-intervention and follow-up). However, our analyses were not successful at determining whether children’s consumption of FV is sustainable (i.e. successful at increasing children’s consumption of FV at both post-intervention and follow-up), as the majority of the studies failed to measure effectiveness at both time points (i.e. immediately following an intervention and after a period of time at follow-up). However, a rudimentary comparison among the studies that measured FV intake at both time points(Reference Ovrebo, Stea and Te Velde34,Reference Tak, Te Velde and Brug37,Reference Tak, Te Velde and Brug38,Reference Gates, Hanning and Gates51,Reference Gates, Hanning and Gates52,Reference Horne, Tapper and Lowe58,Reference Reinaerts, Crutzen and Candel59,Reference Bere, te Velde and Smastuen70,Reference Ashfield-Watt, Stewart and Scheffer71,Reference Fogarty, Antoniak and Venn73,Reference Skinner, Hanning and Metatawabin75Reference Bere, Veierod and Bjelland78) shows that distributing FV to school-aged children was not ultimately sustainable at increasing children’s consumption of FV at follow-up, with the exception of one intervention in which a significant effect was noted at 1-year follow-up in Norway(Reference Te Velde, Brug and Wind79). Previous studies have shown an increased consumption of fruit in the intervention group while the intervention was operating(Reference Bere, Veierod and Klepp40), 1(Reference Bere, Veierod and Bjelland78), 3 years(Reference Bere, Veierod and Skare77) and 14 years(Reference Ovrebo, Stea and Te Velde34) but not at 7 years(Reference Bere, te Velde and Smastuen70) after the intervention ended. This indicates that dietary interventions initiated in childhood tend to maintain to a significant extent into adulthood; however, the strength of dietary tracking is often underestimated due to several methodological difficulties including, but not limited to, differences in study design, methods of dietary assessment, use of statistical methods, the duration of an intervention and follow-up, which consequently limits the opportunity to quantify the habitual dietary behaviour trajectories over time(Reference Hovdenak, Stea and Twisk93).

Several valuable recommendations for successful school food programming have been proposed. These include increased availability and accessibility of FV, education directed at behavioural change, an appropriate theoretical framework, parental involvement, peer and teacher role modelling, messages specifically targeting FV intake as opposed to general healthy eating messages, and adequate time and duration(Reference Knai, Pomerleau and Lock14,Reference Hoelscher, Evans and Parcel94) . Although the reviewed studies possessed a number of these features, differences related to study design, intervention duration, follow-up length, distribution frequency, geographic location, type of FV served, diet assessment methodology, and implementation processes and practices are likely to be the main reasons for the significant heterogeneity among the included studies. For instance, lack of curricular activity implementation in studies based in the Netherlands and Spain is the result of the workload placed on teachers implementing the programme(Reference Wind, Bjelland and Perez-Rodrigo95). This, in turn, resulted in a null intervention effect on children’s consumption of FV in the Netherlands and Spain compared to Norway at 1-year follow-up(Reference Te Velde, Brug and Wind79). In a recent systematic review identifying the conditions and resources under which snack-based FV distribution interventions are most likely to be effective and sustainable(Reference Ismail, Seabrook and Gilliland96), it was shown that distributing FV to school-aged children as a snack can increase consumption, but only with proper implementation. These include participation of the whole school community, school staff training, involving parents within the school and home environment, and adapting the programme to meet school needs and resources. In addition to the successes, the review also highlighted barriers to implementation which included limited funding, insufficient teachers’ time, poor awareness, coordination and communication between key stakeholders (e.g. teachers, school staff, suppliers)(Reference Ismail, Seabrook and Gilliland96). The authors also suggest future recommendations regarding aspects of the intervention that could be adapted or modified to increase the likelihood of success of future snack-based school food programming(Reference Ismail, Seabrook and Gilliland96).

In addition, effectiveness was shown in studies that were conducted for greater than 1 month (n 43), offered FV five times/week (n 35) and employed a theoretical framework (n 21) to those that did not, with the exception of one study(Reference Bere, Veierod and Bjelland33) in which a null effect was observed despite the fact that the intervention lasted a year, offered FV five times a week and was based on theoretical framework (SCT). This indicates that further research is required to determine what is the effective element/component, that if found in an intervention, will be associated with a positive and sustainable FV consumption among children. Therefore, our findings should also be interpreted with caution given the considerable heterogeneity existing between studies grouped under these classifications.

A comparison of FV distribution interventions that employed a stand-alone approach to those that employed a multi-component approach failed to demonstrate more positive effects on children’s FV consumption. For example, both approaches were effective at increasing FV, given that children have more access, exposure and repeated opportunities to try new FV(Reference Blanchette and Brug27). However, our meta-analysis shows that multi-component interventions were more effective in increasing the consumption of FV at post-intervention and follow-up. This was evident particularly in interventions that employed a nutrition education in addition to FV distribution(Reference Bere, Veierod and Bjelland33,Reference Tak, Te Velde and Brug38,Reference He, Beynon and Sangster Bouck74,Reference Bere, Veierod and Bjelland78,Reference Te Velde, Brug and Wind79) or interventions that employed parental involvement as well as nutrition education and FV distribution(Reference Bere, Veierod and Bjelland33,Reference Te Velde, Brug and Wind79) . This is because children spend most of their time at school(Reference Baxter, Milner and Hawkins12) and most of their education about healthy dietary behaviours occurs while at school(Reference Knai, Pomerleau and Lock14). This indicates that simply providing FV to children is not enough to make dietary behaviour change, as children’s consumption of FV will decline as soon as they become ineligible for the programme. As a result, incorporating other strategies such as nutrition education that goes along with providing FV may provide children with the skills and knowledge needed to ensure long-lasting improvement in their dietary choices, particularly in terms of FV consumption. Therefore, significant consideration should be given to integrating nutrition and health topics permanently in the regular curriculum and/or integrating parental involvement into the design of an intervention as positive associations with children’s consumption of FV were noted when both nutrition education and parental involvement were incorporated into an intervention(Reference Jorgensen, Jorgensen and Aarestrup44). Studies have long recognised the positive effects of associating exposure with another reinforcement on children’s intake of FV(Reference Libman25,Reference Triador, Farmer and Maximova26,Reference Cooke, Chambers and Anez97,Reference Wardle, Herrera and Cooke98) . Nonetheless, our results are only based on five studies(Reference Bere, Veierod and Bjelland33,Reference Tak, Te Velde and Brug37,Reference Tak, Te Velde and Brug38,Reference Bere, Veierod and Bjelland78,Reference Te Velde, Brug and Wind79) , and consequently, our findings should be treated with caution.

Most of the reviewed studies (41 of 47) that distributed FV at no cost to parents were effective at increasing FV consumption in school-aged children. Our meta-analysis shows that pooling studies under this classification demonstrates a positive effect on children’s consumption of FV with free compared to subsidised FV distribution interventions. This was also evident when a rudimentary comparison of the four studies that provided children with FV for free and subsidised costs, all positively increased children’s consumption, with a larger impact from the free distribution.(Reference Bere, Hilsen and Klepp39,Reference Bere, Veierod and Klepp40,Reference Bere, Veierod and Bjelland78,Reference Te Velde, Brug and Wind79) The authors suggest that the difference in effectiveness between free and subsidised FV distribution may be because free distribution addresses both availability and accessibility(Reference Cullen, Baranowski and Owens99), whereas subsidised distribution only increases the accessibility but not the availability of FV(Reference Bere, Veierod and Bjelland33). This indicates that free distribution of FV may be the most effective strategy to increase children’s FV consumption because it addresses and reduces existing social inequalities(Reference Knai, Pomerleau and Lock14,Reference Rasmussen, Pedersen and Johnsen100,Reference Ball, Lamb and Costa101) .

The I 2 value indicated a high-level of between-study heterogeneity. This was evident particularly for studies grouped under the classification of post-intervention (I 2 = 81 %), stand-alone (89 %) and free distribution (I 2 = 83 %) at post-intervention. While we cannot rule out publication bias or small study effects (e.g. negative or reverse results might not have published) as an explanation for our findings, sensitivity analyses were therefore conducted. This is to prevent making definite conclusions when included studies had a lot of publication bias. However, this statistical approach has its own limitations as significant findings were reported under different exploratory assumptions(Reference Egger, Davey Smith and Altman31,Reference Higgins and Deeks35) . This was evident when heterogeneity was significantly reduced to less than 50 % when studies were excluded on all principal outcome summary except for stand-alone intervention at post-intervention classification (I 2 = 90 %, P = 0·001). This indicates that elimination of the studies under this classification did not influence the robustness of the calculated effect size which is due to the significant heterogeneity of the studies included under this classification.

This systematic review has several strengths. First, the search was comprehensive, including searches of six electronic databases with no restriction on publication date, country or study design. Second, quality assessments were conducted for each study which allowed for a more rigorous assessment of the validity and weight of the evidence included in the review. This was evident by solely including studies with ‘moderate’ and ‘strong’ ratings in the quantitative synthesis which took adequate measures to avoid selection bias and control confounding factors. Third, despite the high variability observed in measurement of FV consumption across studies, the meta-analysis was conducted using the SMD as the effect measure, by accounting for the high heterogeneity observed among studies, giving a more precise estimate of the effect.

Like all studies, the present review is not without limitations. First, our review is limited by the number of studies included in the meta-analysis, which often resulted in less-rigorous study design; therefore, definite conclusions regarding intervention effectiveness remain unknown. Second, all studies were at risk of bias because they relied largely on questionnaires or recall to record dietary consumption rather than objective measures (e.g. weighing). Third, given that all interventions were focused on FV consumption, it is possible that dietary questionnaires were biased (e.g. being over-estimated in the intervention group) or a poor means (insufficiently sensitive) to detect the relatively small changes in FV consumption, reflected in the wide CI. In addition, subgroup analyses had to be undertaken due to heterogeneity, which reduced our statistical power, and as such, concrete conclusions could not be drawn. Finally, external validity of the evidence was also limited because all the reviewed studies were conducted in Europe and North America, potentially limiting the review generalisability to other developed and developing countries.

Conclusions

The findings of this review demonstrate that snack-based FV distribution interventions within the school environment represent a promising avenue to enhance children’s consumption of FV. Given the greater success at increasing the consumption of fruit, more emphasis is needed on developing novel interventions to achieve greater effectiveness in terms of vegetable consumption. All interventions were effective in increasing the consumption of FV among elementary school-aged children, except for subsidised interventions. Further research is needed to improve the quality of evidence, including studies with more rigorous study designs, sufficient sample sizes, consistent measures and reporting of FV consumption, and follow-up evaluations to confirm these findings. Overall, to inform appropriate policy-making decisions, it is important to develop adequate interventions within the school environment to improve the physical school food environment, as school-aged children spend a large portion of their day in school.

Acknowledgements

Acknowledgements: The authors would like to thank Karen Stewart-Kirk and Bahar Entezari for their assistance with the screening and quality assessment process. The authors would also like to thank librarian Roxanne Isard for advising on the search strategy. Financial support: This research did not receive any specific grant from funding agencies in the public, commercial or not-for-profit sectors. Conflicts of interest: There are no conflicts of interest. Authorship: M.R.I. was involved in all aspects of the research from the development of research questions, data analysis to manuscript preparation. J.A.S. was involved in all aspects of the research from the development of research questions and data analysis to manuscript preparation. J.A.G. was involved in all aspects of the research from the development of research questions and data analysis to manuscript preparation. Ethics of human subject participation: Ethical approval was not required as this study did not collect original data.

Supplementary material

For supplementary material accompanying this paper, visit https://doi.org/10.1017/S1368980021001683

References

Taylor, JP, Evers, S & McKenna, M (2005) Determinants of healthy eating in children and youth. Can J Public Health 96, S20S26.Google ScholarPubMed
Rolls, BJ, Ello-Martin, JA & Carlton Tohill, B (2004) What can intervention studies tell us about the relationship between fruit and vegetable consumption and weight management? Nutr Rev 62, 117.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
World Health Organization (WHO) (2003) Fruit and Vegetable Promotion Initiative: A Meeting Report, /25–27/08/03. Geneva, Switzerland: World Health Organization.Google Scholar
Garriguet, D (2007) Canadian’s eating habits. Health Rep 18, 1732.Google Scholar
Dennison, B, Rockwell, H & Baker, S (1998) Fruit and vegetable intake in young children. J Am Coll Nutr 17, 371378.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kim, S, Moore, L, Galuska, D et al. (2014) Vital signs: fruit and vegetable intake among children-United States, 2003–2010. Morb Mortal Weekly Rep 63, 671676.Google ScholarPubMed
Minaker, L & Hammond, D (2016) Low frequency of fruit and vegetable consumption among Canadian youth: findings from the 2012/2013 Youth Smoking Survey. J School Health 86, 135142.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Colapinto, C, Graham, J & St-Pierre, S (2018) Trends and correlates of frequency of fruit and vegetable consumption, 2007–2014. Health Rep 29, 914.Google Scholar
Polsky, J & Garriguet, D (2020) Changes in vegetable and fruit consumption in Canada between 2004 and 2015. Health Rep 31, 312.Google ScholarPubMed
Kelder, S, Perry, C, Klepp, K et al. (1994) Longitudinal tracking of adolescents smoking, physical activity and food choices behaviors. Am J Public Health 84, 11211126.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krebs-Smith, S, Heimendinger, J, Patterson, B et al. (1995) Psychosocial factors associated with fruit and vegetable consumption. Am J Health Promot 10, 98104.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baxter, AP, Milner, PC, Hawkins, S et al. (1997) The impact of heart health promotion on coronary heart disease lifestyle risk factors in schoolchildren. Public Health 111, 231237.Google ScholarPubMed
Budd, G & Volpe, S (2006) School-based obesity prevention: research, challenges, and recommendations. J School Health 76, 485495.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Knai, C, Pomerleau, J, Lock, K et al. (2006) Getting children to eat more fruit and vegetables: a systematic review. Prev Med 42, 8595.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hughes, RJ, Edwards, KL, Clarke, GP et al. (2012) Childhood consumption of fruit and vegetables across England: a study of 2306 6–7-year-olds in 2007. Br J Nutr 108, 733742.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van Cauwenberghe, E, Maes, L, Spittaels, H et al. (2010) Effectiveness of school-based interventions in Europe to promote healthy nutrition in children and adolescents: systematic review of published and ‘grey’ literature. Br J Nutr 103, 781797.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Delgado-Noguera, M, Tort, S, Martinez-Zapata, MJ et al. (2011) Primary school interventions to promote fruit and vegetable consumption: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Prev Med 53, 39.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Evans, CE, Christian, MS, Cleghorn, CL et al. (2012) Systematic review and meta-analysis of school-based interventions to improve daily fruit and vegetable intake in children aged 5 to 12 years. Am J Clin Nutr 96, 889901.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hodder, RK, Stacey, FG, O’Brien, KM et al. (2018) Interventions for increasing fruit and vegetable consumption in children aged 5 years and under. Cochrane Database Syst Rev 1, 1284.Google Scholar
Hendrie, GA, Lease, HJ, Bowen, J et al. (2017) Strategies to increase children’s vegetable intake in home and community settings: a systematic review of literature. Matern Child Nutr 13, 122.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Sa, J & Lock, K (2008) Will European agricultural policy for school fruit and vegetables improve public health? A review of school fruit and vegetable programmes. Eur J Public Health 18, 558568.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
An, R (2013) Effectiveness of subsidies in promoting healthy food purchases and consumption: a review of field experiments. Public Health Nutr 16, 12151228.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DeCosta, P, Moller, P, Frost, MB et al. (2017) Changing children’s eating behaviour - a review of experimental research. Appetite 113, 327357.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Margolin, A, Goto, K, Wolff, C et al. (2018) Let’s talk food: elementary school students’ perceptions of school and home food environment and the impact of the Harvest of the Month Program on their dietary attitudes and behaviors. Int J Child Youth Fam Stud 8, 154167.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Libman, K (2007) Growing youth growing food: how vegetable gardening influences young people’s food consciousness and eating habits. Appl Env Edu Comm 6, 8795.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Triador, L, Farmer, A, Maximova, K et al. (2015) A school gardening and healthy snack program increased Aboriginal First Nations children’s preferences toward vegetables and fruit. J Nutr Educ Behav 47, 176180.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blanchette, L & Brug, J (2005) Determinants of fruit and vegetable consumption among 6–12-year-old children and effective interventions to increase consumption. J Hum Nutr Diet 18, 431443.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rasmussen, M, Krolner, R, Klepp, KI et al. (2006) Determinants of fruit and vegetable consumption among children and adolescents: a review of the literature. Part I: quantitative studies. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 3, 119.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moher, D, Liberati, A, Tetzlaff, J et al. (2009) Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: the PRISMA Statement. PLoS Med 6, e1000097.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Effective Public Health Practice Project (EPHPP) (2008) Quality Assessment Tool for Quantitative Studies. http://www.ephpp.ca/PDF/Quality%20Assessment%20Tool_2010_2.pdf (accessed September 2020).Google Scholar
Egger, M, Davey Smith, G & Altman, DG (2008) Systematic Reviews in Healthcare: Meta-Analysis in Context, 2nd ed. London, UK: BMJ Books.Google Scholar
Higgins, J, Thompson, S, Deeks, J et al. (2003) Measuring inconsistency in meta-analyses. Biomed J 327, 557560.Google ScholarPubMed
Bere, E, Veierod, MB, Bjelland, M et al. (2006a) Outcome and process evaluation of a Norwegian school-randomized fruit and vegetable intervention: fruits and Vegetables Make the Marks (FVMM). Health Educ Res 21, 258267.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ovrebo, B, Stea, TH, Te Velde, SJ et al. (2019) A comprehensive multicomponent school-based educational intervention did not affect fruit and vegetable intake at the 14-year follow-up. Prev Med 121, 7985.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Higgins, J & Deeks, J (2008) Chapter 7: Selecting sSudies and Collecting Data. Chichester, UK: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Hozo, SP, Djulbegovic, B & Hozo, I (2005) Estimating the mean and variance from the median, range, and the size of a sample. BMC Med Res Methodol 5, 13.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tak, NI, Te Velde, SJ & Brug, J (2008) Long-term effects of the Dutch Schoolgruiten Project – promoting fruit and vegetable consumption among primary-school children. Public Health Nutr 12, 12131223.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tak, NI, Te Velde, SJ & Brug, J (2007) Ethnic differences in 1-year follow-up effect of the Dutch Schoolgruiten Project - promoting fruit and vegetable consumption among primary-school children. Public Health Nutr 10, 14971507.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bere, E, Hilsen, M & Klepp, KI (2010) Effect of the nationwide free school fruit scheme in Norway. Br J Nutr 104, 589594.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bere, E, Veierod, MB & Klepp, KI (2005) The Norwegian School Fruit Programme: evaluating paid v. no-cost subscriptions. Prev Med 41, 463470.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sahebkar, A (2014) Are curcuminoids effective C-reactive protein-lowering agents in clinical practice? Evidence from a meta-analysis. Phytother Res 28, 633642.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhang, Y, Zeng, Z, Cao, Y et al. (2014) Effect of urinary protease inhibitor (ulinastatin) on cardiopulmonary bypass: a meta-analysis for China and Japan. PLoS One 9, e113973.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coyle, K, Potter, S, Schneider, D et al. (2009) Distributing free fresh fruit and vegetables at school: results of a pilot outcome evaluation. Public Health Rep 124, 660669.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jorgensen, S, Jorgensen, T, Aarestrup, A et al. (2016) Parental involvement and association with adolescents’ fruit and vegetable intake at follow-up: process evaluation results from the multi-component school-based Boost intervention. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 13, 116.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yeo, ST & Edwards, RT (2006) Encouraging fruit consumption in primary schoolchildren: a pilot study in North Wales. J Hum Nutr Diet 19, 299302.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clarke, AM, Ruxton, CHS, Hetherington, L et al. (2009) School intervention to improve preferences for fruit and vegetables. Nutr Food Sci 39, 118127.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jamelske, E & Bica, L (2014) The USDA fresh fruit and vegetable program: a case study of implementation and consumption in Wisconsin. J Child Nutr Manag 38, 18.Google Scholar
Tussing-Humphreys, L, Thomson, J, McCabe-Sellers, B et al. (2012) A school-based fruit and vegetable snacking pilot intervention for lower Mississippi delta children. Infant Child Adol Nutr 4, 340347.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ransley, JK, Taylor, EF, Radwan, Y et al. (2010) Does nutrition education in primary schools make a difference to children’s fruit and vegetable consumption? Public Health Nutr 13, 18981904.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lowe, CF, Horne, PJ, Tapper, K et al. (2004) Effects of a peer modelling and rewards-based intervention to increase fruit and vegetable consumption in children. Eur J Clin Nutr 58, 510522.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gates, A, Hanning, RM, Gates, M et al. (2012) Inadequate nutrient intakes in youth of a remote first nation community: challenges and the need for sustainable changes in program and policy. ISRN Public Health 2012, 15.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gates, A, Hanning, RM, Gates, M et al. (2016) Four-year evaluation of a healthy school snack program in a remote first nations community. Health Behav Policy Rev 3, 226237.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
White, G (2006) Evaluation of the school fruit and vegetable pilot scheme. Educ Health 24, 6264.Google Scholar
Ovrum, A & Bere, E (2014) Evaluating free school fruit: results from a natural experiment in Norway with representative data. Public Health Nutr 17, 12241231.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Woodruff, SJ (2019) Fruit and vegetable intake and preferences associated with the northern fruit and vegetable program (2014–2016). Can J Diet Pract Res 80, 7278.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jorgensen, TS, Rasmussen, M, Aarestrup, AK et al. (2015) The role of curriculum dose for the promotion of fruit and vegetable intake among adolescents: results from the Boost intervention. BMC Public Health 15, 536.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Horne, P, Hardman, C, Lowe, C et al. (2009) Increasing parental provision and children’s consumption of lunchbox fruit and vegetables in Ireland: the Food Dudes intervention. Eur J Clin Nutr 63, 613618.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Horne, P, Tapper, K, Lowe, C et al. (2004) Increasing children’s fruit and vegetable consumption: a peer-modelling and rewards-based intervention. Eur J Clin Nutr 58, 16491660.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reinaerts, E, Crutzen, R, Candel, M et al. (2008) Increasing fruit and vegetable intake among children: comparing long-term effects of a free distribution and a multicomponent program. Health Educ Res 23, 987996.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ransley, JK, Greenwood, DC, Cade, JE et al. (2007) Does the school fruit and vegetable scheme improve children’s diet? A non-randomised controlled trial. J Epidemiol Community Health 61, 699703.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Methner, S, Maschkowski, G & Hartmann, M (2017) The European School Fruit Scheme: impact on children’s fruit and vegetable consumption in North Rhine-Westphalia, Germany. Public Health Nutr 20, 542548.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gold, A, Larson, M, Tucker, J et al. (2017) Classroom nutrition education combined with fruit and vegetable taste testing improves children’s dietary intake. J School Health 87, 106113.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hass, J, Lischetzke, T & Hartmann, M (2018) Does the distribution frequency matter? A subgroup specific analysis of the effectiveness of the EU School Fruit and Vegetable Scheme in Germany comparing twice and thrice weekly deliveries. Public Health Nutr 21, 13751387.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bica, L & Jamelske, E (2012) USDA Fresh fruit and vegetable program creates postive change in children’s consumption and other behaviours related to eating fruit and vegetables. J Child Nutr Manag 36, 18.Google Scholar
Jamelske, E & Bica, L (2012) Impact of the USDA Fresh Fruit and Vegetable Program on children’s consumption. J Child Nutr Manag 36, 110.Google Scholar
Jamelske, E, Bica, L, McCarty, D et al. (2008) Preliminary findings from an evaluation of the USDA Fresh Fruit and Vegetable Program in Wisconsin schools. Wis Med J 107, 225230.Google ScholarPubMed
Roccaldo, R, Censi, L, D’Addezio, L et al. (2017) A teachers’ training program accompanying the “School Fruit Scheme” fruit distribution improves children’s adherence to the Mediterranean diet: an Italian trial. Int J Food Sci Nutr 68, 887900.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gates, A, Hanning, RM, Gates, M et al. (2011) A School nutrition program improves vegetable and fruit knowledge, preferences, and exposure in First Nation youth. Open Nutr J 5, 16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hector, D, Edwards, S, Gale, J et al. (2017) Achieving equity in Crunch&Sip((R)): a pilot intervention of supplementary free fruit and vegetables in NSW classrooms. Health Promot J Austr 28, 238242.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bere, E, te Velde, SJ, Smastuen, MC et al. (2015) One year of free school fruit in Norway--7 years of follow-up. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 12, 17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ashfield-Watt, PA, Stewart, EA & Scheffer, JA (2008) A pilot study of the effect of providing daily free fruit to primary-school children in Auckland, New Zealand. Public Health Nutr 12, 693701.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eriksen, K, Haraldsdottir, J, Pederson, R et al. (2003) Effect of a fruit and vegetable subscription in Danish schools. Public Health Nutr 6, 5763.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fogarty, AW, Antoniak, M, Venn, AJ et al. (2007) Does participation in a population-based dietary intervention scheme have a lasting impact on fruit intake in young children? Int J Epidemiol 36, 10801085.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
He, M, Beynon, C, Sangster Bouck, M et al. (2009) Impact evaluation of the Northern Fruit and Vegetable Pilot Programme - a cluster-randomised controlled trial. Public Health Nutr 12, 21992208.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Skinner, K, Hanning, RM, Metatawabin, J et al. (2012) Impact of a school snack program on the dietary intake of grade six to ten First Nation students living in a remote community in northern Ontario, Canada. Rural Remote Health 12, 117.Google Scholar
Wells, L & Nelson, M (2005) The National School Fruit Scheme produces short-term but not longer-term increases in fruit consumption in primary school children. Br J Nutr 93, 537542.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bere, E, Veierod, M, Skare, Q et al. (2007) Free school fruit-sustained effect 3 years later. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 4, 16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bere, E, Veierod, MB, Bjelland, M et al. (2006b) Free school fruit-sustained effect 1 year later. Health Educ Res 21, 268275.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Te Velde, SJ, Brug, J, Wind, M et al. (2008) Effects of a comprehensive fruit- and vegetable-promoting school-based intervention in three European countries: the Pro Children Study. Br J Nutr 99, 893903.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bandura, A (2004) Health promotion by social cognitive means. Health Educ Behav 31, 143164.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bartholomew Eldrigde, K, Markham, C, Ruiter, R et al. (2016) Planning Health Promotion Programs: An Intervention Mapping Approach, 4th ed. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.Google Scholar
Rossi, P, Lipsey, M & Freeman, H (2004) Evaluation: A systematic Approach, 7th ed. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.Google Scholar
Olsho, LE, Klerman, JA, Ritchie, L et al. (2015) Increasing child fruit and vegetable intake: findings from the US Department of Agriculture Fresh Fruit and Vegetable Program. J Acad Nutr Diet 115, 12831290.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jean Naylor, P (2014) Efficacy of a minimal dose school fruit and vegetable snack intervention. J Food Nutr Disord 3, 111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reinaerts, E, de Nooijer, J, Candel, M et al. (2007) Increasing children’s fruit and vegetable consumption: distribution or a multicomponent programme? Public Health Nutr 10, 939947.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cooke, L (2007) The importance of exposure for healthy eating in childhood: a review. J Hum Nutr Diet 20, 294301.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brug, J, Tak, NI, te Velde, SJ et al. (2008) Taste preferences, liking and other factors related to fruit and vegetable intakes among schoolchildren: results from observational studies. Br J Nutr 99, Suppl 1, S7S14.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mennella, JA & Bobowski, NK (2015) The sweetness and bitterness of childhood: insights from basic research on taste preferences. Physiol Behav 152, 502507.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schindler, JM, Corbett, D & Forestell, CA (2013) Assessing the effect of food exposure on children’s identification and acceptance of fruit and vegetables. Eat Behav 14, 5356.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Laureati, M, Bergamaschi, V & Pagliarini, E (2014) School-based intervention with children. Peer-modelling, reward, and repeated exposure reduce food neophobia and increase liking of fruits and vegetables. Appetite 83, 2632.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Potter, S, Schneider, D, Coyle, K et al. (2011) What works? Process evaluation of a school-based fruit and vegetable distribution program In Mississippi. J School Health 81, 202211.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olsen, A, Ritz, C, Kraaij, L et al. (2012) Children’s liking and intake of vegetables: a school-based intervention study. Food Qual Prefer 23, 9098.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hovdenak, IM, Stea, TH, Twisk, J et al. (2019) Tracking of fruit, vegetables and unhealthy snacks consumption from childhood to adulthood (15 year period): does exposure to a free school fruit programme modify the observed tracking? Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 16, 22.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoelscher, D, Evans, A, Parcel, G et al. (2002) Designing effective nutrition interventions for adolescents. J Am Diet Assoc 102, S52S63.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wind, M, Bjelland, M, Perez-Rodrigo, C et al. (2008) Appreciation and implementation of a school-based intervention are associated with changes in fruit and vegetable intake in 10- to 13-year old schoolchildren-the Pro Children study. Health Educ Res 23, 9971007.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ismail, MR, Seabrook, JA & Gilliland, JA (2021) Process evaluation of fruit and vegetables distribution interventions in school-based settings: a systematic review. Prev Med Rep 21, 110.Google ScholarPubMed
Cooke, LJ, Chambers, LC, Anez, EV et al. (2011) Eating for pleasure or profit: the effect of incentives on children’s enjoyment of vegetables. Psychol Sci 22, 190196.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wardle, J, Herrera, M-L, Cooke, L et al. (2003) Modifying children’s food preferences: the effects of exposure and reward on acceptance of an unfamiliar vegetable. Eur J Clin Nutr 57, 341348.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cullen, KW, Baranowski, T, Owens, E et al. (2003) Availability, accessibility, and preferences for fruit, 100 % fruit juice, and vegetables influence children’s dietary behavior. Health Educ Behav 30, 615626.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rasmussen, M, Pedersen, TP, Johnsen, NF et al. (2018) Persistent social inequality in low intake of vegetables among adolescents, 2002–2014. Public Health Nutr 21, 16491653.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ball, K, Lamb, KE, Costa, C et al. (2015) Neighbourhood socioeconomic disadvantage and fruit and vegetable consumption: a seven countries comparison. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 12, 68.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Fig. 1 Flow diagram of search strategy and review process based on PRISMA statement. PRISMA, Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses

Figure 1

Fig. 2 Summary of study quality assessment using the Effective Public Health Practice Project (EPHPP) quality assessment tool for quantitative studies. Criteria Scale: 1 – strong, 2 – moderate, 3 – weak, N/A – not applicable. Global Rating System: 1 – strong (no weak ratings), 2 – moderate (one weak rating), 3 – weak (two or more weak ratings). QA Tool accessible through http://www.ephpp.ca/PDF/Quality%20Assessment%20Tool_2010_2.pdf

Figure 2

Fig. 3 School-based interventions to promote fruit and vegetable consumption. Meta-analysis of intervention sustainability at post-intervention and at follow-up. (Standardised mean differences and 95 % CI). 1Measurement at post-intervention time point; 2measurement at follow-up time point

Figure 3

Fig. 4 School-based interventions to promote fruit and vegetable consumption. Meta-analysis of intervention approach (stand-alone or multi-component) at post-intervention and at follow-up. (Standardised mean differences and 95 % CI). 1Measurement at post-intervention time point; 2measurement at follow-up time point

Figure 4

Fig. 5 School-based interventions to promote fruit and vegetable consumption. Meta-analysis of intervention type of distribution (free and subsidised) at post-intervention and at follow-up. (Standardised mean differences and 95 % CI). 1Measurement at post-intervention time point; 2measurement at follow-up time point

Supplementary material: File

Ismail et al. supplementary material

Ismail et al. supplementary material 1

Download Ismail et al. supplementary material(File)
File 44.7 KB
Supplementary material: PDF

Ismail et al. supplementary material

Ismail et al. supplementary material 2

Download Ismail et al. supplementary material(PDF)
PDF 10.3 MB
Supplementary material: File

Ismail et al. supplementary material

Ismail et al. supplementary material 3

Download Ismail et al. supplementary material(File)
File 59 KB