Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-wg55d Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-19T16:08:15.476Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

3 - A Career Emerging from an Unnecessary Analysis

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  29 September 2022

Saul Kassin
Affiliation:
John Jay College of Criminal Justice, City University of New York
Get access

Summary

I retired a couple of years ago as a kid in my mid-80s. But I hadn’t stopped working. We have a yearlong seminar at UC Riverside, a statistical consulting course. Graduate students can enroll for credit, and we have six or seven faculty members who attend throughout the entire academic year. On Tuesdays from noon to 1 p.m., we would bring data analytic or other methodological questions to that group.

Type
Chapter
Information
Pillars of Social Psychology
Stories and Retrospectives
, pp. 15 - 23
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2022

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Suggested Reading

Ambady, N., & Rosenthal, R. (1992). Thin slices of expressive behavior as predictors of interpersonal consequences: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 111(2), 256274.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Glass, G. V. (1976). Primary, secondary, and meta-analysis of research. Educational Researcher, 5(10), 38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Milgram, S. (1963). Behavioral study of obedience. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 67(4), 371378.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morgan, W. (2002). Origin and history of the earliest thematic apperception test. Journal of Personality Assessment, 79(3), 422445.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pearson, K. (1904). Report on certain enteric fever inoculation statistics. British Medical Journal, 3, 1243-1246.Google Scholar
Rosenthal, R. (1956). An attempt at the experimental induction of the defense mechanism of projection. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of California at Los Angeles.Google Scholar
Rosenthal, R. (1963). On the social psychology of the psychological experiment: The experimenter’s hypothesis as unintended determinant of experimental results. American Scientist, 51(2), 268283.Google ScholarPubMed
Rosenthal, R. (1966). Experimenter Effects in Behavioral Research. New York: Appleton.Google Scholar
Rosenthal, R. (1978). Combining results of independent studies. Psychological Bulletin, 85(1), 185193.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosenthal, R. (1994). Interpersonal expectancy effects: A 30-year perspective. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 3(6), 176179.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosenthal, R. (2003). Covert communication in laboratories, classrooms, and the truly real world. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 12(5), 151154.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosenthal, R., & Fode, K. L. (1963). The effect of experimenter bias on the performance of the albino rat. Behavioral Science, 8(3), 183189.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosenthal, R., & Jacobson, L. (1968). Pygmalion in the Classroom: Teacher Expectation and Pupils’ Intellectual Development. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosenthal, R., & Lawson, R. (1964). A longitudinal study of the effects of experimenter bias on the operant learning of laboratory rats. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 2(2), 6172.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosenthal, R., & Rosnow, R. L. (2008). Essentials of Behavioral Research: Methods and Data Analysis (3rd ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Rosenthal, R., & Rubin, D. B. (1978). Interpersonal expectancy effects: The first 345 studies. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1(3), 377415.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosenthal, R., & Rubin, D. B. (1986). Meta-analytic procedures for combining studies with multiple effect sizes. Psychological Bulletin, 99(3), 400406.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×