Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-wq484 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-27T17:09:01.315Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

References

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 June 2017

Raymond W. Gibbs, Jr
Affiliation:
University of California, Santa Cruz
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
Metaphor Wars
Conceptual Metaphors in Human Life
, pp. 293 - 318
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2017

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Ackerman, J., Nocera, C., & Bargh, J. (2010). Incidental haptic sensations influence social judgments and decisions. Science, 328, 17121715.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alibali, M., Nathan, M., & Fujimori, Y. (2011). Gestures in the mathematics classroom: What’s the point? In Stein, N. & Raudenbush, S. (Eds.), Developmental cognitive science goes to school (pp. 219234). New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Allbritton, D., McKoon, G., & Gerrig, R. (1994). Metaphor-based schemas and text representations: Making connections through conceptual metaphors. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21, 114.Google Scholar
Alverson, H. (1994). Semantics and experience: Universal metaphors of time in English, Mandarin, Hindi, and Sesotho. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.Google Scholar
Al-Zahrani, A. (2007). Darwin’s metaphors revisited: Conceptual metaphors, conceptual blends, and idealized cognitive models in the theory of evolution. Metaphor and Symbol, 23, 5082.Google Scholar
Antle, A., Corness, G., & Droumeva, M. (2009). What the body knows: Exploring the benefits of embodied metaphors in hybrid physical digital environments. Interacting with Computers, 21, 6675.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Antović, M. (2009). Musical metaphors in Serbian and Romani children: An empirical study. Metaphor and Symbol, 24, 184202.Google Scholar
Antović, M., Bennett, A., & Turner, M. (2013). Running in circles or moving along lines: Conceptualization of musical elements in sighted and blind children. Musicae Scientiae, 17, 229245.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Auslander, J. (2001). Embodied mathematics. American Scientist, 89, 366367.Google Scholar
Aziz-Zadeh, L., & Damasio, A. (2008). Embodied semantics for actions: Findings from functional brain imaging. Journal of Physiology – Paris, 102, 3539.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Banerjee, P., Chatterjee, P., & Sinha, J. (2012). Is it light or dark? Recalling moral behavior changes perception of brightness. Psychological Science, 24, 407419.Google Scholar
Barcelona, A. (Ed.) (2000). Metaphor and metonymy at the crossroads. Berlin: Mouton.Google Scholar
Bardolph, M., & Coulson, S. (2014). How vertical hand movements impact brain activity elicited by literally and metaphorically related words: an ERP study of embodied metaphor. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 1031.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barnden, J. (2008). Metaphor and artificial intelligence: Why they matter to each other? In Gibbs, R. (Ed.), Cambridge handbook of metaphor and thought (pp. 311338). New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barnden, J. (2009). Metaphor and context: A perspective from artificial intelligence. In Musolff, A. & Zinken, J. (Eds.), Metaphor and discourse (pp. 7994). Basingstokes: Palgrave.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barnden, J. (2010). Metaphor and metonymy: Making their connections more slippery. Cognitive Linguistics, 21, 134.Google Scholar
Barry, C., Brescoll, V., Brownell, K., & Schlesinger, M. (2009). Obesity metaphors: How do beliefs about the causes of obesity affect support for public policy? Milbank Quarterly, 87, 747.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barsalou, L. (2008). Grounded cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 617645.Google Scholar
Becker, G. (1997). Disrupted lives: How people create meaning in a chaotic world. Berkeley: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Beger, A. (2011). Deliberate metaphors? An exploration of the choice and functions of metaphor in US-American college lectures. Metaphorik.de, 20, 3960.Google Scholar
Bergen, B. (2005). Mental simulation in literal and figurative language understanding. In Coulson, S. & Lewandowska-Tomaszczyk, B. (Eds.), The literal and nonliteral in language and thought (pp. 255280). Berlin: Lang.Google Scholar
Bergen, B., Lindsay, S., Matlock, T., & Narayanan, S. (2007). Spatial and linguistic aspects of visual imagery in sentence comprehension. Cognitive Science, 31, 733764.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bilz, K. (2012). Dirty hands or deterrence? An experimental examination of the exclusionary rule. Journal of Empirical Legal Studies, 9, 149171.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Black, M. (1954). Metaphor. Proceedings of the Aristotelian Society, 55, 273294.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bochaver, A., & Fenko, A. (2010). Metaphors in happy and unhappy life stories of Russian adults. Metaphor and Symbol, 25, 243262.Google Scholar
Boers, F. (2003). Applied linguistics perspectives on cross-cultural variation in conceptual metaphor. Metaphor and Symbol, 18, 231238.Google Scholar
Boers, F., & Littlemore, J. (2000). Cognitive style variables in participants’ explanations of conceptual metaphors. Metaphor and Symbol, 15, 177187.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boot, I., & Pecher, D. (2011). Representation of categories: Metaphorical use of the container schema. Experimental Psychology, 58, 162170.Google Scholar
Booth, W. (1978). Metaphor as rhetoric: The problem of evaluation. Critical Inquiry, 5, 4972.Google Scholar
Borbely, A. (2004). Toward a psychodynamic understanding of metaphor and metonymy: Their role in awareness and defense. Metaphor and Symbol, 19, 91114.Google Scholar
Boroditsky, L. (2000). Metaphoric Structuring: Understanding time through spatial metaphors. Cognition, 75, 128.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boroditsky, L. (2001). Does language shape thought? English and Mandarin speakers’ conceptions of time. Cognitive Psychology, 43, 122.Google Scholar
Boroditsky, L., & Ramscar, M. (2002). The roles of body and mind in abstract thought. Psychological Science, 13, 185189.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bortfeld, H., & McGlone, M. (2001). The continuum of metaphor processing. Metaphor and Symbol, 16, 7586.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bos, A., & Cienki, A. (2011). Inhibiting gesture reduces the amount of spatial metaphors in speech. Paper presented at the GESPIN Gesture and Speech in Interaction, Bielefeld, Germany.Google Scholar
Boulenger, V., Hauk, O., & Pulvermuller, F. (2009). Grasping ideas with the motor system. Semantic somatotopy in idiom comprehension. Cerebral Cortex, 19, 19051914.Google Scholar
Bowdle, B., & Gentner, D. (2005). The career of metaphor. Psychological Review, 112, 193216.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brower, C. (1999). A cognitive theory of musical meaning. Journal of Music Theory, 44, 323379.Google Scholar
Brown, T. (2003). Making truth: Metaphor in science. Champaign, IL: University of Illinois Press.Google Scholar
Caballero, R. (2003). Talking about space: Image metaphor in architectural discourse. Annual Review of Cognitive Linguistics, 1, 87105.Google Scholar
Caballero, R. (2006). Re-viewing space. Figurative language in architects’ assessment of built space. Berlin: Mouton.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caballero, R. (2014). Thinking, drawing and writing architecture through metaphor. Iberica, 28, 155180.Google Scholar
Caballero, R., & Ibarretxe-Antuñano, I. (2014). Ways of perceiving, moving, and thinking: Revindicating culture in conceptual metaphor research. Cognitive Semiotics, 5, 268290.Google Scholar
Cacciari, C., & Glucksberg, S. (1995). Understanding idioms: Do visual images reflect figurative meanings? European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 7, 283305.Google Scholar
Cacciari, C., & Pesciarelli, F. (2013). Motor activation in literal and non literal sentences: Does time matter? Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 17, doi:10.3389/fnhum.2013.00202.Google Scholar
Cacciari, C., Pellicciari, M. C., Bolognini, N., Senna, I., Miniussi, C., & Papagno, C. (2011). Literal, fictive and metaphorical motion sentences preserve the motion component of the verb. A TMS study. Brain & Language, 119, 149157.Google Scholar
Calbris, G. (1990). The semiotics of French gestures. Bloomington: Indiana University Press.Google Scholar
Cameron, L. (1999). Operationalising ‘metaphor’ for applied linguistic research. In Cameron, L. & Low, G. (Eds.), Researching and Applying Metaphor (pp. 328). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Cameron, L. (2003). Metaphor in educational discourse. London: Continuum.Google Scholar
Cameron, L. (2007). Patterns of metaphor in reconciliation talk. Discourse and Society, 18, 197222.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cameron, L. (2008). Metaphor and talk. In Gibbs, R. (Ed.), Cambridge handbook of metaphor and thought (pp. 197211). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Cameron, L. (2011). Metaphor and reconciliation: The discourse dynamics of empathy in post-conflict conversations. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Cameron, L. (2016). In Gibbs, R. (Ed.), Mixing metaphor. Amsterdam: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Cameron, L., Maslen, R., Todd, Z., Maule, J., Stratton, P., & Neil, S. (2009). The discourse dynamics approach to metaphor and metaphor-led discourse analysis. Metaphor and Symbol, 24, 6389.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Camus, J. (n.d.) Variation of cancer metaphors in scientific texts and press popularisaions. Retrieved from: ucrel.lancs.ac.uk/publications/cl2009/468_FullPaper.doc.Google Scholar
Can, H., & Can, N. (2010). The inner self desires a friendly chat: Chat metaphors in Turkish and English. Metaphor and Symbol, 25, 3455.Google Scholar
Cardillo, E., Watson, C., Schmidt, G., Kranjec, A., & Chatterjee, A. (2012). From novel to familiar: Tuning the brain for metaphors. Neuroimage, 59, 32123221.Google Scholar
Casasanto, D. (2008). Similarity and proximity: When does close in space mean close in mind? Memory & Cognition, 36, 10471056.Google Scholar
Casasanto, D. (2009). Embodiment of abstract concepts: Good and bad in right-and left-handers. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 138, 351367.Google Scholar
Casasanto, D., & Boroditsky, L. (2008). Time in the mind: Using space to think about time. Cognition, 106, 579593.Google Scholar
Casasanto, D., & Chrysikou, E. (2011). When left is “right”: Motor fluency shapes abstract concepts. Psychological Science, 22, 419422.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Casasanto, D., & Dijkstra, K. (2010). Motor action and emotional memory. Cognition, 115, 179185.Google Scholar
Casasanto, D., & Gijssels, T. (2015). What makes a metaphor an embodied metaphor? Linguistics Vanguard. doi:10.1080/0163853X.2015.10.Google Scholar
Casasanto, D., & Jasmin, K. (2010). Good and bad in the hands of politicians: Spontaneous gestures during positive and negative speech. PLoS ONE, 5, e11805.Google Scholar
Charlton, D. (Ed.) (1989). Hoffmann’s Musical Writings. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Charteris-Black, J. (2004). Corpus approaches to critical metaphor analysis. Basingstokes, UK: Palgrave–MacMillan.Google Scholar
Charteris-Black, J. (2012). Forensic deliberations on purposeful metaphor. Metaphor and the Social World, 2, 121.Google Scholar
Cherkasskiy, L., Song, H., Malahy, S., & Bargh, J. (2012). Soft on crime: Sitting in soft versus hard chairs produces more lenient recommended sentences. Hot topic talk at the embodiment preconference of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, San Diego, CA.Google Scholar
Chen, J-Y. (2007). Do Chinese and English speakers think about time differently? Failure of replicating Boroditsky (2001). Cognition, 104, 427436.Google Scholar
Ching, M. (1993). Games and play: Pervasive metaphors in American life. Metaphor and Symbolic Activity, 8, 4365.Google Scholar
Cienki, A. (1998). Metaphoric gestures and some of their relations to verbal metaphoric expressions. In Koenig, J-P. (Ed.), Discourse and cognition (pp. 189204). Stanford: CSLI Publications.Google Scholar
Cienki, A. (1998). STRAIGHT: An image schema and its metaphorical extensions. Cognitive Linguistics, 9, 107149.Google Scholar
Cienki, A. (2004). Bush’s and Gore’s language and gestures in the 2000 US presidential debates. Journal of Language and Politics, 3 (3), 409440.Google Scholar
Cienki, A., & Müller, C. (Eds.) (2008). Metaphor and gesture. Amsterdam: Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cienki, A., & Müller, C. (2008). Metaphor, Gesture, and Thought. In Gibbs, R. (Ed.), The Cambridge handbook of metaphor and thought (pp. 483501). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Citron, F., & Goldberg, A. (2014). Social context modulates the effect of hot temperature on perceived interpersonal warmth: A study of embodied metaphors. Language and Cognition, 6, 111.Google Scholar
Clark, A. (1997). Being there: Putting brain, body, and world together again. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Coegnarts, M., & Kravanja, P. (2012a). The visual and multimodal representation of time in film, or how time is metaphorically shaped in space. Image & Narrative, 13, 85100.Google Scholar
Coegnarts, M., & Kravanja, P. (2012b). From thought to modality: A theoretical framework for analysing structural–conceptual metaphors and image metaphors in film. Image & Narrative, 13, 96113.Google Scholar
Coegnarts, M., & Kravanja, P. (Eds.) (2015). Embodied cognition and cinema. Leuven: Leuven University Press.Google Scholar
Colston, H. (2015). Using figurative language. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coulson, S. (2001). Semantic Leaps: Frame-shifting and conceptual blending in meaning construction. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Coulson, S., & Pagán Cánovas, C. (2013). Understanding timelines: Conceptual metaphor and conceptual integration. Cognitive Semiotics, 5, 198219.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coulter, R., Zaltman, G., & Coulter, K. (2001). Interpreting consumer perceptions of advertising: An application of the Zaltman metaphor elicitation technique. Journal of Advertising, 30, 122.Google Scholar
Crawford, E. (2009). Conceptual metaphors of affect. Emotion Review, 1, 129139.Google Scholar
Crawford, L., Margolies, S., Drake, J., & Murphy, M. (2006). Affect biases memory of location: Evidence for the spatial representation of affect. Cognition and Emotion, 20, 11531169.Google Scholar
Crisp, P. (2002). Metaphorical propositions: A rational. Language and Literature, 11, 716.Google Scholar
Croft, W., & Cruse, A. (2004). Cognitive linguistics. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Cuyckens, H., & Zawada, B. (Eds.) (2001). Polysemy in cognitive linguistics. Amsterdam: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Dancygier, B. (2012). The language of stories: A cognitive approach. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Dancygier, B., & Sweetser, E. (2014). Figurative language. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Danesi, M. (2003). Conceptual metaphor theory and the teaching of mathematics: Findings of a pilot project. Semiotica, 145, 7183.Google Scholar
Danesi, M. (2007). A conceptual metaphor framework for the teaching of mathematics. Studies in Philosophy and Education, 26, 225236.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deignan, A. (2003). Metaphorical expressions and culture: An indirect link. Metaphor and Symbol, 18, 255271.Google Scholar
Deignan, A. (2006). Metaphor in corpus linguistics. Amsterdam: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Desai, R., Binder, J., Conant, L., Mano, Q., & Seidenberg, M. (2011). The neural career of sensory-motor metaphors. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23, 23762386.Google Scholar
Devlin, K. (2005). The math instinct: Why you’re a mathematical genius (along with lobsters, birds, cats, and dogs). New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Dorst, A., Reijnierse, G., & Venhuize, G. (2013). One small step for MIP towards automated metaphor identification? Formulating general rules to determine basic meanings in large-scale approaches to metaphor. Metaphor and the Social World, 3, 7799.Google Scholar
Duffy, S. E. (2014). The role of cultural artifacts in the interpretation of metaphorical expressions about time. Metaphor and Symbol, 29, 94112.Google Scholar
Duffy, S. E., & Feist, M. (2014). Individual differences in the interpretation of ambiguous statements about time. Cognitive Linguistics, 25, 2954.Google Scholar
Duffy, S., Feist, M., & McCarthy, S. (2014). Moving through time: The role of personality in three real life contexts. Cognitive Science, 38(8), 1662–74.Google Scholar
Dunn, J. (2013). How linguistic structure influences and helps to predict metaphoric meaning. Cognitive Linguistics, 24, 3366.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Edwards, J. (1991). Categories are for talking: On the cognitive and discursive bases of categorization. Theory & Psychology, 1, 515542.Google Scholar
Elwood, W. (1995). Declaring war on the home front: Metaphor, presidents, and the war on drugs. Metaphor and Symbolic Activity, 10, 93114.Google Scholar
El Refaie, E. (2014). Appearances and dis/dys-appearances? A dynamic view of embodiment in conceptual metaphor theory. Metaphor and the Social World, 4, 109125.Google Scholar
Emanation, M. (1995). Metaphor and the expression of emotion: The value of cross-cultural perspectives. Metaphor and Symbolic Activity, 10, 163182.Google Scholar
Eubanks, P. (1999). Conceptual metaphor as rhetorical response: A reconsideration of metaphor. Written Communication, 16, 171199.Google Scholar
Eubanks, P. (2000). A war of words in the discourse of trade: The rhetorical constitution of metaphor. Carbondale: Southern Illinois University Press.Google Scholar
Eubanks, P. (2010). Metaphor and writing: Figurative language in the discourse of written communication. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Evans, V. (2003). The structure of time: Language, meaning, and temporal cognition. Amsterdam: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Fauconnier, G., & Lakoff, G. (2013). On metaphor and blending. Journal of Cognitive Semiotics, 5, 393399.Google Scholar
Fauconnier, G., & Turner, M. (2002). The way we think: Conceptual blending and the mind’s hidden complexities. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Fauconnier, G., & Turner, M. (2008). Rethinking metaphor. In Gibbs, R. (Ed.), The Cambridge handbook of metaphor and thought (pp. 5366). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Feldman, G. (2006). From molecules to metaphors. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Ferrara, K. (1994). Therapeutic ways with words. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Ferrari, G. (2006). Architectural space as metaphor in the Greek sanctuary. In Turner, M. (Ed.), The artful mind (pp. 225242). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Fesmire, S. (1994). Aerating the mind: The metaphor of mental functioning as bodily functioning. Metaphor and Symbolic Activity, 9, 3144.Google Scholar
Fludernik, M. (2011). The rise of cognitive metaphor theory and its literary repercussions. In Fludernik, M. (Ed.), Beyond conceptual metaphor theory: Perspectives on literary metaphor (pp. 19). London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Flusberg, S., Thibodeau, P., Sternberg, D., & Glick, J. (2010). A connectionist approach to embodied conceptual metaphors. Frontiers in Psychology, 1. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2010.00197.Google Scholar
Forceville, C. (2002). The identification of target and source in pictorial metaphors. Journal of Pragmatics, 3, 114.Google Scholar
Forceville, C. (2005). Visual representations of the idealized cognitive model of anger in the Asterix album La Zizanie. Journal of Pragmatics, 37, 6988.Google Scholar
Forceville, C. (2009). Non-verbal and multimodal metaphor in a cognitivist framework : Agendas for research. In Forceville, Urios-Aparisi C. & E. (Eds.), Multimodal metaphor (pp. 1942). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Forceville, C. (2009). Metonymy in visual and audiovisual discourse. In Ventola, E. & Guijjaro, A.J. Moya (Eds.), The world told and the world shown: multisemiotic issues (pp. 5674). Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan.Google Scholar
Forceville, C. (2011). The JOURNEY metaphor and the Source–Path–Goal schema in Agnès Varda’s autobiographical gleaning documentaries In Fludernik, M. (Ed.), Beyond cognitive metaphor theory: Perspectives on literary metaphor (pp. 281297). London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Forceville, C., & Jeulink, M. (2011). The flesh and blood of embodied understanding: The Source–Path–Goal schema in animation film. Pragmatics & Cognition, 19, 3759.Google Scholar
Freeman, D. (1995). Catch(ing) the nearest way: Macbeth and cognitive metaphor. Journal of Pragmatics, 24, 689708.Google Scholar
Freeman, M. (2011). The aesthetics of human experience: Minding, metaphor, and icon in poetic expression. Poetics Today, 32, 717752.Google Scholar
Gallese, V., & Lakoff, G. (2005). The brain’s concepts: The role of sensory-motor system in conceptual knowledge. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 22, 455479.Google Scholar
Geeraerts, D. (1997). Diachronic prototype semantics: A contribution to historical lexicology. Oxford: Clarendon.Google Scholar
Geeraerts, D., & Gevaert, C. (2008). Hearts and (angry) minds in Old English. In Sharifian, F., Yu, N., Dirven, R., & Niemeier, S. (Eds.), Culture, body, and language: Conceptualizations of internal body organs across cultures and languages (pp. 319348). Berlin: Mouton.Google Scholar
Geeraerts, D., & Grondelaers, S. (1995). Looking back at anger: Cultural traditions and metaphorical patterns. In Taylor, J. & MacLaury, R. (Eds.), Language and the cognitive construal of the world (pp. 153179). Berlin: Mouton.Google Scholar
Gentner, D., & Bowdle, B. (2008). Metaphor as structure-mapping. In Gibbs, R. (Ed.) The Cambridge handbook of metaphor and thought (pp. 109128). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Gentner, D., & Grudin, J. (1985). The evolution of mental metaphors in psychology: A 90-year retrospective. American Psychologist, 40, 181192.Google Scholar
Gentner, D., & Kurtz, K. (2006). Relations, objects, and the composition of analogies. Cognitive Science, 30, 609642.Google Scholar
Gentner, D., Imai, M., & Boroditsky, L. (2002). As time goes by: Understanding time as spatial metaphor. Language and Cognitive Processes, 17, 537565.Google Scholar
Gentner, D., Bowdle, B., Wolff, P., & Boronat, C. (2001). Metaphor is like analogy. In Gentner, D., Holyoak, K., & Kokinov, B. (Eds.), The analogical mind: Perspectives from cognitive science (pp. 199253). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Gibbs, R. (1990). Psycholinguistic studies on the conceptual basis of idiomaticity. Cognitive Linguistics, 1, 417451.Google Scholar
Gibbs, R. (1991). Semantic analyzability in children’s understanding of idioms. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 34, 613620.Google Scholar
Gibbs, R. (1992). What do idioms really mean? Journal of Memory and Language, 31, 485506.Google Scholar
Gibbs, R. (1994). The poetics of mind: Figurative thought, language, and understanding. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Gibbs, R. (2003). Embodied meanings in performing, interpreting and talking about dance improvisation. In Albright, C. & Gere, D. (Eds.), Taken by surprise: A dance improvisation reader (pp. 185196). Middletown, CT: Wesleyan University Press.Google Scholar
Gibbs, R. (2005). The psychological status of image schemas. In Hampe, B. (Ed.), From perception to meaning: Image schemas in cognitive linguistics. Berlin: Mouton.Google Scholar
Gibbs, R. (2006a). Introspection and cognitive linguistics: Should we trust our intuitions? Annual Review of Cognitive Linguistics, 4, 135152.Google Scholar
Gibbs, R. (2006b). Embodiment and cognitive science. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Gibbs, R. (2006c). Metaphor interpretation as embodied simulation. Mind & Language, 21, 434458.Google Scholar
Gibbs, R. (Ed.) (2008). The Cambridge handbook of metaphor and thought. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gibbs, R. (2011a). Evaluating conceptual metaphor theory. Discourse Processes, 48, 529562.Google Scholar
Gibbs, R. (2011b). Are deliberate metaphors really deliberate? A question of human consciousness and action. Metaphor and the Social World, 1, 2652.Google Scholar
Gibbs, R. (2013). Metaphoric cognition as social activity: Dissolving the divide between metaphor in thought and communication. Metaphor and the Social World, 3, 5476.Google Scholar
Gibbs, R. (2015a). Do pragmatic signals affect conventional metaphor understanding? A failed test of deliberate metaphor theory. Journal of Pragmatics, 90, 7787.Google Scholar
Gibbs, R. (2015b). Does deliberate metaphor theory have a future? Journal of Pragmatic, 90, 7376.Google Scholar
Gibbs, R. (Ed.) (2016). Mixing metaphor. Amsterdam: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Gibbs, R., & Beitel, D. (1995). What proverb understanding reveals about how people think? Psychological Bulletin, 118, 133154.Google Scholar
Gibbs, R., & Bogdonovich, J. (1999). Mental imagery in interpreting poetic metaphor. Metaphor and Symbol, 14, 3744.Google Scholar
Gibbs, R., & Cameron, L. (2008). The socio-cognitive dynamics of metaphor performance. Cognitive Systems Research, 9, 6475.Google Scholar
Gibbs, R., & Colston, H. (2012). Interpreting figurative meaning. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Gibbs, R., & Ferriera, L. (2011). Do people infer the entailments of conceptual metaphors during verbal metaphor understanding? In Brdar, M. & Fuchs, M. (Eds.), Converging and diverging tendencies in cognitive linguistics (pp. 7492). Amsterdam: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Gibbs, R., & Franks, H. (2002). Embodied metaphors in women’s narratives about their experiences with cancer. Health Communication, 14, 139165.Google Scholar
Gibbs, R., & Nascimento, S. (1996). How we talk when we talk about love: Metaphorical concepts and understanding love poetry. In Kreuz, R. & MacNealy, M. (Eds.), Empirical approaches to literature and aesthetics (pp. 221240). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Gibbs, R., & O’Brien, J. (1990). Idioms and mental imagery: The metaphorical motivation for idiomatic meaning. Cognition, 36, 3568.Google Scholar
Gibbs, R., Beitel, D., Harrington, M., & Sanders, P. (1994). Taking a stand on the meanings of stand: Bodily experience as motivation for polysemy. Journal of Semantics, 11, 231251.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gibbs, R., Bogdonovich, J., Sykes, J., & Barr, D. (1997). Metaphor in idiom comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language, 37, 141154.Google Scholar
Gibbs, R., Buchalter, D., Moise, J., & Farrar, W. (1993). Literal meaning and figurative language. Discourse Processes, 16, 387403.Google Scholar
Giessner, S. R., & Schubert, T. W. (2007). High in the hierarchy: How vertical location and judgments of leaders’ power are interrelated. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 104, 3044.Google Scholar
Gineste, M-D., Indurkhya, B., & Scart, V. (2000). Emergence of features in metaphor comprehension. Metaphor and Symbol, 15, 117135.Google Scholar
Glucksberg, S. (2001). Understanding figurative language: From metaphors to idioms. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Glucksberg, S. (2008). Metaphors create categories. In Gibbs, R. (Ed.), Cambridge handbook of metaphor and thought (pp. 6783). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Glucksberg, S., & Haught, C. (2006). On the relation between metaphor and simile: When comparison fails. Mind and Language, 21, 360378.Google Scholar
Glucksberg, S., & Keysar, B. (1990). Understanding metaphorical comparisons: Beyond similarity. Psychological Review, 97, 318.Google Scholar
Glucksberg, S., Brown, M., & McGlone, M. (1993). Conceptual metaphors are not automatically accessed during idiom comprehension. Memory & Cognition, 21, 711715.Google Scholar
Glucksberg, S., Keysar, B., & McGlone, M. (1992). Metaphor understanding and accessing conceptual schema: Reply to Gibbs (1992). Psychological Review, 99, 578581.Google Scholar
Goatly, A. (1997). The language of metaphors. New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Goatly, A. (2002). Conflicting metaphors in the Hong Kong special administrative region educational reform proposals. Metaphor and Symbol, 17, 263294.Google Scholar
Goatly, A. (2007). Washing the brain: Metaphor and hidden ideology. Amsterdam: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Goldwasser, O. (2005). Where is metaphor? Conceptual metaphor and alternative classification in the hieroglyphic script. Metaphor and Symbol, 20, 95113.Google Scholar
Golshaie, R., & Golfam, A. (2014). Processing conventional conceptual metaphors in Persian: A corpus-based psycholinguistic study. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 44, 496518.Google Scholar
Gong, S-P., & Ahrens, K. (2007). Processing conceptual metaphors in ongoing discourse. Metaphor and Symbol, 22, 313330.Google Scholar
Grady, J. (1997). Theories are buildings revisited. Cognitive Linguistics, 8, 267290.Google Scholar
Grady, J. (1999). A typology of motivation for metaphor: Correlations vs. resemblances. In Gibbs, R. & Steen, G. (Eds.), Metaphor in cognitive linguistics (pp. 79100). Amsterdam: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Grady, J., Oakley, T., &. Coulson, S. (1999). Conceptual blending and metaphor. In Gibbs, R. & Steen, G. (Eds.), Metaphor in cognitive linguistics (pp. 101124). Amsterdam: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Graf, E-M. (2011). Adolescents’ use of spatial TIME metaphors: A matter of cognition or sociocommunicative practice? Journal of Pragmatics, 43, 723734.Google Scholar
Gries, S. (2006). Corpus-based methods and cognitive semantics: The many senses of to run. In Gries, S. & Stefanowitsch, A. (Eds.), Corpora in cognitive linguistics: corpus-based approaches to syntax and lexis (pp. 5799). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Gries, S. & Stefanowitsch, A. (Eds.) (2006). Corpora in cognitive linguistics: corpus-based approaches to syntax and lexis. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Hampe, B. (Ed.) (2005). From perception to meaning: Image schemas in cognitive linguistics. Berlin: Mouton.Google Scholar
Hart, C. (2008). Critical discourse analysis and metaphor: Toward a theoretical framework. Critical Discourse Studies, 5, 91106.Google Scholar
Haser, V. (2005). Metaphor, metonymy, and experientialist philosophy: Challenging cognitive semantics. Mouton: Berlin.Google Scholar
Haught, C. (2013). A tale of two tropes: How metaphor and simile differ. Metaphor and Symbol, 28 (4), 254274.Google Scholar
Hellmann, J. H., Thoben, D. F., & Echterhoff, G. (2013). The sweet taste of revenge: Gustatory experience induces metaphor-consistent judgments of a harmful act. Social Cognition, 31, 531542.Google Scholar
Hellsten, I. (2000). Dolly: Scientific breakthrough or Frankenstein’s monster? Journalistic and scientific metaphors of cloning. Metaphor and Symbol, 15, 215221.Google Scholar
Hellsten, I., & Nerlich, B. (2011). Synthetic biology: Building the language for a new science brick by metaphorical brick. New Genetics and Society, 30, 375397.Google Scholar
Herrera-Soler, H. (2006). Metaphors in press headlines of globalization. Annual Review of Cognitive Linguistics, 4, 120.Google Scholar
Hogan, P. (2002). A minimal, lexicalist/constituent transfer account of metaphor. Style, 36, 484502.Google Scholar
Horchak, O., Giger, J-C., Pochwatko, G. (2014). Simulation of metaphorical actions and discourse comprehension. Metaphor and Symbol, 29, 122.Google Scholar
Hornsby, N. (1995). High fidelity. London: Riverhead Books.Google Scholar
Horstmann, G., & Ansorge, U. (2011). Compatibility between tones, head movements, and facial expressions. Emotion, 11, 975980.Google Scholar
Howe, J. (2007). Argument is argument: An essay on conceptual metaphor and verbal dispute. Metaphor and Symbol, 23, 123.Google Scholar
Howe, N. (1988). Metaphor in contemporary American political discourse. Metaphor & Symbolic Activity, 3, 87104.Google Scholar
Hulrburt, R., & Schwitzgebel, E. (2007). Describing inner experience? Proponent meets skeptic. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Hutchins, E. (1995). Cognition in the wild. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Ibarretxe-Antuñano, I. (2008). Vision metaphors for the intellect: Are they really cross-linguistic? Atlantis, 6, 1533.Google Scholar
Ibarretxe-Antuñano, I. (2013). The relationship between conceptual metaphor and culture. Intercultural Pragmatics, 10, 315339.Google Scholar
Jackendoff, R. (1983). Semantics and cognition. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Jackendoff, R., & Aaron, D. (1991). Review of Lakoff and Turner (1989). Language, 87, 320338.Google Scholar
Jäkel, O. (2002). Hypothesis revisited: The cognitive theory of metaphor applied to religious texts. Metaphorik.de, No. 02, Online: www.metaphorik.de/sites/www.metaphorik.de/files/journal-pdf/02_2002_jaekel.pdf.Google Scholar
Jeppsson, F., Haglund, J., & Amin, T. (2015).Varying use of conceptual metaphors across levels of expertise in thermodynamics International Journal of Science Education, 37, 780805.Google Scholar
Johansen, M. (2009). Embodied strategies in mathematical cognition. Unpublished manuscript.Google Scholar
Johansson-Falck, M. (2010). Are metaphorical paths and roads ever paved? Corpus analysis of real and imagined journeys. Review of Cognitive Linguistics, 8, 93122.Google Scholar
Johansson-Falck, M., & Gibbs, R. (2012). Embodied motivations for metaphoric meaning. Cognitive Linguistics, 23, 251272.Google Scholar
Johnson, M. (1981). Introduction: Metaphor in the philosophical tradition. In Johnson, M. (Ed.), Philosophical perspectives on metaphor (pp. 347). Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota Press.Google Scholar
Johnson, M. (1987). The body in mind: The bodily basis of meaning, imagination and reason. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Johnson, M. (1991). Knowing through the body. Philosophical Psychology, 4, 318.Google Scholar
Johnson, M. (1993). Moral imagination: Implications of cognitive science for ethics. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Johnson, M. (2007). The meaning of the body: Aesthetics of human understanding. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Johnson, M., & Larsen, S. (2003). Something in the way she moves. Metaphor and Symbol, 18, 6384.Google Scholar
Jing-Schmidt, Z. (2008). Much mouth much tongue: Chinese metonymies and metaphors of verbal behavior. Cognitive Linguistics, 19, 241282.Google Scholar
Kang, Y., Williams, L., Clark, M., Gray, J., & Bargh, J. (2011). Physical temperature effects on trust behavior: the role of insula. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 6, 507515.Google Scholar
Kappelhoff, H., & Müller, C. (2011). Embodied meaning construction. Multimodal metaphor and expressive movement in speech, gesture, and feature film. Metaphor and the Social World, 1, 121153.Google Scholar
Katz, A., & Ferratti, T. (2001). Moment-by-moment comprehension of proverbs in discourse. Metaphor and Symbol, 11, 1737.Google Scholar
Katz, A., & Law, A. (2010). Experimental support for conceptual metaphors with an episodic memory task. Metaphor and Symbol, 25, 263270.Google Scholar
Katz, A., & Taylor, T. (2008). The journeys of life: Examining a conceptual metaphor with semantic and episodic memory recall. Metaphor and Symbol, 23, 148173.Google Scholar
Keefer, L., Landau, M., Sullivan, D., & Rothschild, Z. (2014). Embodied metaphor and abstract problem solving: Testing a metaphoric fit hypothesis in the health domain. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 55, 1220.Google Scholar
Kennedy, J. (1982). Metaphor in pictures. Perception, 11, 589605.Google Scholar
Kertész, A., & Rákosi, C. (2009). Cyclic vs. circular argumentation in conceptual metaphor theory. Cognitive Linguistics, 20, 703732.Google Scholar
Keysar, B., & Bly, B. (1995). Intuitions of the transparency of idioms: Can one keep a secret by spilling the beans? Journal of Memory and Language, 34, 89109.Google Scholar
Keysar, B., Shen, Y., Glucksberg, S., & Horton, W. (2000). Conventional language: How metaphoric is it? Journal of Memory and Language, 43, 576593.Google Scholar
Kimmel, M. (2013). The conceptual metaphors of narrative structure: Gestural evidence for spatialized form in storytelling. Public Journal of Semiotics, 5, 75123.Google Scholar
Kintsch, W. (2008). Symbol systems and perceptual representations. In De Vega, M., Glenberg, A., & Graesser, A. (Eds.), Symbols and Embodiment (pp. 145164). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Kittay, E. (1987). Metaphor: Its cognitive force and linguistic structure. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Koller, V. (2002). “A shotgun wedding”: co-occurrence of war and marriage metaphors in mergers and acquisitions discourse. Metaphor and Symbol, 17 (3),179203.Google Scholar
Koller, V. (2004). Metaphor and gender in business media discourse: A critical cognitive study. Basingstoke: Palgrave.Google Scholar
Koller, V. (2009). Brand images: Multimodal metaphor in corporate branding messages. In Forceville, C. & Urios-Aparisi, E. (Eds.), Multimodal metaphor (pp. 4572). Berlin: Mouton De Gruyter.Google Scholar
Kövecses, Z. (1991). Happiness: A definitional effort. Metaphor and Symbolic Activity, 6(1), 2946.Google Scholar
Kövecses, Z. (1993). Tocqueville’s passionate “beast”: A linguistic analysis of the concept of American Democracy. Metaphor and Symbolic Activity, 9, 113133.Google Scholar
Kövecses, Z. (1995). American friendship and the scope of metaphor. Cognitive Linguistics, 6, 315346.Google Scholar
Kövecses, Z. (1998). Are there any emotion-specific metaphors? In Athanasiadou, A. & Tabakowska, E. (Eds.), Speaking of emotions: Conceptualization and expression (pp. 127151). Berlin: Mouton.Google Scholar
Kövecses, Z. (2000). Metaphor and emotion. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Kövecses, Z. (2003). Language, figurative thought, and cross-cultural comparison. Metaphor and Symbol, 18, 311320.Google Scholar
Kövecses, Z. (2005). Metaphor in culture: Universality and variation. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Kövecses, Z. (2006). Language, mind, and culture: A practical introduction. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Kövecses, Z. (2008). Conceptual metaphor theory: Some criticisms and alternative proposals. Annual Review of Cognitive Linguistics, 6, 168184.Google Scholar
Kövecses, Z. (2010). Metaphor and culture. Acta Universistatis Sapientiae, Philologica, 2, 197220.Google Scholar
Kövecses, Z. (2010). Metaphor: A practical introduction. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Kövecses, Z. (2012). The biblical story retold: A cognitive linguistic perspective. In Brdar, M., Gries, S., & Fuchs, M. (Eds.), Cognitive linguistic: Convergence and expansion (pp. 325354). Amsterdam: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Kövecses, Z. (2015). Where metaphors come from: Reconsidering context in metaphor. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Krennmayr, T. (2011). Metaphor in newspapers. Utrecht: LOT dissertation series, 276.Google Scholar
Lacey, S., Stilla, R., & Sathian, K. (2012). Metaphorically feeling: comprehending textural metaphors activates somatosensory cortex. Brain and Language, 120, 416421.Google Scholar
Lachaud, C. M. (2013). Conceptual metaphors and embodied cognition: EEG coherence reveals brain activity differences between primary and complex conceptual metaphors during comprehension. Cognitive Systems Research, 22–23, 1226.Google Scholar
Lackie, B. (1991). Catch a falling kingdom: A meditation on King Lear. Metaphor and Symbol, 6, 259270.Google Scholar
Lakens, D., Semin, G., & Foroni, F. (2011). Why your highness needs the people: Comparing the absolute and relative representation of power in vertical space. Social Psychology, 42 (3), 205213.Google Scholar
Lakoff, G. (1987). Women, fire, and dangerous things: What categories reveal about the mind. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Lakoff, G. (1990). The invariance hypothesis: Is abstract reason based on image-schemas? Cognitive Linguistics, 1, 3974.Google Scholar
Lakoff, G. (1993). The contemporary theory of metaphor. In Ortony, A. (Ed.), Metaphor and thought (Vol. 2). (pp. 202251). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Lakoff, G. (1996). Moral politics. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Lakoff, G. (2008). The neural theory of metaphor. In Gibbs, R. (Ed.), Cambridge handbook of metaphor and thought. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Lakoff, G. (2009). The political mind: A cognitive science guide to your brain and its politics. New York: Penguin.Google Scholar
Lakoff, G. (2014). Mapping the brain’s metaphor circuitry: Metaphorical thought in everyday reason. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8. http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00958.Google Scholar
Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1980). Metaphors we live by. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1999). Philosophy in the flesh. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (2002). Why cognitive linguistics requires embodied realism. Cognitive Linguistics, 13, 245263.Google Scholar
Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (2003). Metaphors we live by (2nd edition). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Lakoff, G., & Kövecses, Z. (1987). The cognitive model of anger inherent in American English. In Holland, D. & Quinn, N. (Eds.), Cultural models in language and thought (pp. 195221). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Lakoff, G., & Núñez, R. (2002). Where mathematics comes from: How the embodied mind brings mathematics into being. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Lakoff, G., & Turner, M. (1989). More than cool reason: A field guide to poetic metaphor. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Lakoff, G., Espenson, J., & Schwartz, A. (1991). The master metaphor list. Technical report, University of California, Berkeley.Google Scholar
Langacker, R. (2006). On the continuous debate about discreteness. Cognitive Linguistics, 17, 107151.Google Scholar
Langston, W. (2002). Violating orientational metaphors slows reading. Discourse Processes, 34, 281310.Google Scholar
Larson, B., Nerlich, B., & Wallis, P. ( 2006). Metaphor and biorisks. Science Communication, 26, 243268.Google Scholar
Larson, S. (2013). Musical forces: Motion, metaphor and meaning in music. Bloomington, IN: Indiana University Press.Google Scholar
Leddy, T. (1995). Metaphor and metaphysics. Metaphor and Symbol, 10: 205222.Google Scholar
Lee, S., & Schwarz, N. (2010). Washing away dissonance. Science, 329, 709.Google Scholar
Lee, S., & Schwarz, N. (2012). Bidirectionality, mediation, and moderation of metaphorical effects: The embodiment of social suspicion and fishy smells. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 103, 737749.Google Scholar
Leezenberg, M. (2001). Contexts of metaphor. Amsterdam: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Leung, A., Kim, S., Polman, E., Ong, L., Qiu, L., Goncalo, J., & Sanchez-Burks, J. (2011). Embodied metaphors and creative acts. Psychological Science, 23, 502509.Google Scholar
Levin, L., Mitamura, T., MacWhinney, B., Fromm, D., Carbonell, J., Feely, W., Frederking, R., Gershman, A., & Ramirez, C. (2014) “Resources for the Detection of Conventionalized Metaphors in Four Languages”, in Proceedings of the ninth international conference on language resources and evaluation (LREC’14) (pp. 498–501).Google Scholar
Lewandowska-Tomaszczyk, B. (2007). Polysemy, prototypes and radial categories. In Geeraerts, D. & Cuyckens, H. (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of cognitive linguistics (pp. 139169). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Li, J. (2010). Verticality, horizontality, and states of the self: Cognitive metaphors for the “spatial self” in Chinese autobiographical writings. Metaphor and Symbol, 26, 6895.Google Scholar
Liljenquist, K., Zhong, C-B., & Galinsky, A. (2010). The smell of virtue: Clean scents promote reciprocity and charity. Psychological Science, 21, 381383.Google Scholar
Linn, R. (1991). Holocaust metaphors and symbols in the moral dilemmas of contemporary Israeli soldiers. Metaphor and Symbolic Activity, 6, 6186.Google Scholar
Lizardo, O. (2012). The conceptual bases of metaphors of dirt and cleanliness in moral and non-moral reasoning. Cognitive Linguistics, 23, 367393.Google Scholar
Loenneker-Rodman, B. (2008). The Hamburg metaphor database project: Issues in resource creation. Language Resources and Evaluation, 42, 293318.Google Scholar
Loenneker-Rodman, B., & Narayanan, S. (2012). Computational approaches to figurative language. In Spivey, M., McRae, K., & Joanisse, M. (Eds.), Cambridge handbook of psycholinguistics (pp. 485504). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Lonergan, J., & Gibbs, R. (2016). Tackling mixed metaphor in discourse: Corpus and psychological studies. In Gibbs, R. (Ed.), Mixing metaphor (pp. 5774). Amsterdam: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Louwerse, M. (2008). Embodied relations are encoded in language. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15 (4), 838844.Google Scholar
Low, G. & Todd, Z. (2010) Good practice in metaphor analysis: Guidelines and pitfalls. In Metaphor analysis - research practice in applied linguistics, social sciences and the humanities (pp. 217229). United Kingdom: Equinox eBooks Publishing.Google Scholar
Lowenberg, I. (1975). Identifying metaphors. Foundations of Language, 12, 315338.Google Scholar
Lu, W., & Chiang, W. (2007) Emptiness we live by: Metaphors and paradoxes in Buddhism’s Heart Sutra. Metaphor and Symbol, 22, 331355.Google Scholar
Maalej, Z. (2004). Figurative language in anger expressions in Tunisian Arabic: An extended view of embodiment. Metaphor and Symbol, 19, 5175.Google Scholar
MacArthur, F. (2005). The competent horseman in a horseless world: Observations on a conventional metaphor in Spanish and English. Metaphor and Symbol, 20, 7194.Google Scholar
MacArthur, F. (2015). On using a dictionary to identify the basic senses of words Metaphor and the Social World, 5, 124136.Google Scholar
Maglio, P. P., & Matlock, T. (1999). The conceptual structure of information space. In Munro, A., Höök, K., & Benyon, D. (Eds.), Social navigation of information space (pp. 155173). Berlin: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
Mahon, B., & Caramazza, A. (2008). A critical look at the embodied cognition hypothesis & a new proposal for grounding conceptual content. Journal of Physiology – Paris, 102, 5970.Google Scholar
Manuel Urena, J., & Faber, P. (2010). Reviewing imagery in resemblance and non-resemblance metaphors. Cognitive Linguistics, 21, 123149.Google Scholar
Markgraf, S., & Pavlik, L. (1998). “Reel” metaphors for teaching. Metaphor and Symbol, 13, 275285.Google Scholar
Marmaridou, S. (2006). On the conceptual, cultural and discursive motivation of Greek pain lexicalizations. Cognitive Linguistics, 17, 393434.Google Scholar
Marshak, R (2009). Organizational change: Views from the edge. Bethel, ME: The Lewin Center.Google Scholar
Martin, J. (1990). A computational model of metaphor interpretation. San Diego: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Mason, Z. (2004). CorMet: A computational, corpus-based conventional metaphor extraction system. Computational Linguistics, 30, 2344.Google Scholar
Matlock, T., Ramscar, M., & Boroditsky, L. (2005). The experiential link between spatial and temporal language. Cognitive Science, 29, 655664.Google Scholar
Matlock, T., Castro, S.C., Fleming, M., Gann, T., & Maglio, P.P. (2014). Spatial metaphors in web use. Spatial Cognition and Computation, 14, 306320.Google Scholar
McGlone, M. (1996). Conceptual metaphors and figurative language interpretation: Food for thought? Journal of Memory and Language, 35, 544565.Google Scholar
McGlone, M. (2001). Concepts as metaphors. In Glucksberg, S. (Ed.), Understanding figurative language (pp. 90107). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
McGlone, M. (2007). What is the explanatory value of a conceptual metaphor? Language & Communication, 27, 109126.Google Scholar
McGlone, M. (2011). Hyperbole, homunculi, and hindsight bias: An alternative evaluation of conceptual metaphor theory. Discourse Processes, 48, 563574.Google Scholar
McGlone, M., & Harding, J. (1998). Back (or forward?) to the future: The role of perspective in temporal language comprehension. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 24, 12111223.Google Scholar
McMullen, L. (1989). Use of figurative language in successful and unsuccessful cases of psychotherapy: Three comparisons. Metaphor and Symbolic Activity, 4, 203225.Google Scholar
McNeill, D. (1992). Hand in mind. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Meier, B., & Robinson, M. (2004). Why the sunny side is up. Psychological Science, 15, 243247.Google Scholar
Meier, B., & Robinson, M. (2006). Does “feeling down” mean seeing down? Depressive symptoms and vertical selective attention. Journal of Research in Personality, 40, 451461.Google Scholar
Meier, B., Moeller, S., Riemer-Peltz, M., & Robinson, M. (2012). Sweet taste preferences and experiences predict pro-social inferences, personalities, and behaviors. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 102, 163174.Google Scholar
Meier, B., Robinson, M., & Clore, G. (2004). Why good guys wear white: Automatic inferences about stimulus valence based on brightness. Psychological Science, 15, 8287.Google Scholar
Meier, B., Robinson, M., Crawford, L., & Ahlvers, W. (2007). When ‘light’ and ‘dark’ thoughts become light and dark responses: Affect biases brightness judgments. Emotion, 7, 366376.Google Scholar
Miles, L., Nind, L., Macrae, C. (2010). Moving through time. Psychological Science, 21, 222223.Google Scholar
Mittelberg, I. (2010). Geometric and image-schematic patterns in gesture space. In Evans, V. & Chilton, P. (Eds.), Language, cognition and space: The state of the art and new directions (pp. 351385). London: Equinox.Google Scholar
Mittelberg, I., & Waugh, L. (2009). Metonymy first, metaphor second: A cognitive-semiotic approach to multimodal figures of speech in co-speech gesture. In Forceville, C. & Urios-Aparisi, E. (Eds.), Multimodal metaphor (pp. 328356). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Moore, K. (2006). Space-to-time mappings and temporal concepts. Cognitive Linguistics, 17, 199244.Google Scholar
Morgan, G. (1997). Images in organizations (2nd edition). Thousand Oaks, London: Sage Publications.Google Scholar
Murphy, G. (1996). On metaphoric representations. Cognition, 60, 173204.Google Scholar
Musolff, A. (2000). Political imagery of Europe: A house without exit doors. Journal of Multiingual and Multocultual Development, 21 (3), 216229.Google Scholar
Musolff, A. (2004). Metaphor and political discourse: Analogical reasoning in debates about Europe. London: Palgrave–Macmillan.Google Scholar
Musolff, A. (2006). Metaphorical scenarios in public discourse. Metaphor and Symbol, 21, 2338.Google Scholar
Musolff, A. (2012). The study of metaphor as part of critical discourse analysis. Critical Discourse Studies, 9, 301310.Google Scholar
Müller, C. (2007). Metaphors dead and alive, sleeping and waking: A dynamics view. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Müller, C. & Schmitt, C. (2015). Audio-visual metaphors of the financial crisis: Meaning making and the flow of experience. Revista Brasileira de Linguística Aplicada, 15, 311341.Google Scholar
Naciscione, A. (2015). Rethinking basic terminology in proverb research: A cognitive study. Proverbium: Yearbook of International Proverb Scholarship, 32, 345357.Google Scholar
Narayanan, S. (1997). Moving right along: A computational model of metaphoric reasoning about events. Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, International Computer Science Institute, University of California, Berkeley.Google Scholar
Nayak, N., & Gibbs, R. (1990). Conceptual knowledge in the interpretation of idioms. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 119, 315330.Google Scholar
Nerlich, B., & Jaspal, R. (2012). Metaphors we die by? Geoengineering, metaphors, and the argument from catastrophe. Metaphor and Symbol, 27, 131147.Google Scholar
Neuman, Y., Assaf, D., Cohen, Y., Last, M., Argamon, S., & Newton, H. (2013). Metaphor identification in large texts corpora. PLoS ONE, 8, e62343. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0062343.Google Scholar
Niebert, K., & Gropengiesser, H. (2015). Understanding starts in the mesocosm: Conceptual metaphor as a framework for external representations in science teaching. International Journal of Science Education, 37, 903933.Google Scholar
Núñez, R. (2005). Creating mathematical infinities: The beauty of transfinite cardinals. Journal of Pragmatics, 37, 17171741.Google Scholar
Núñez, R. (2008). Proto-numerosities and concepts of number: Biologically plausible and culturally mediated top-down mathematical schemas. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 31(6), 665666.Google Scholar
Núñez, R., & Sweetser, E. (2006). With the future behind them: Convergent evidence from Aymara language and gesture in the crosslinguistic comparison of spatial construals of time. Cognitive Science, 30, 401450.Google Scholar
Núñez, R., Motz, B., & Teuscher, U. (2006). Time after time: The psychological reality of the Ego- and Time-Reference-Point distinction in metaphorical construals of time. Metaphor and Symbol, 21, 133146.Google Scholar
Oakley, T., & Hougaard, A. (Eds.) (2008). Mental spaces in discourse and interaction. Amsterdam: Benjamins.Google Scholar
O’Brien, G. (2003). Indigestible food, conquering hordes, and waste materials: Metaphors of immigrants and the early immigration restriction debate in the United States. Metaphor and Symbol, 18, 3347.Google Scholar
O’Connor, K. (1998). Money and finance as solid, liquid and gas in Spanish. Metaphor and Symbol, 13, 141157.Google Scholar
Odebunmi, A. (2010). Ideology and body part metaphors in Nigerian English. Review of Cognitive Linguistics, 6, 272299.Google Scholar
Ortiz, M. (2014). Visual manifestations of primary metaphors through Mise-en-scène techniques. Image & Narrative, 15, 116.Google Scholar
Orton, S. (2004). Conceptual metaphor in the archaeological record: Methods and an example from the American Southwest. American Antiquity, 65, 613645.Google Scholar
Ortony, A. (1975). Why metaphors are necessary and not just nice. Educational Theory, 25, 4553.Google Scholar
Ortony, A. (1988). Are emotion metaphors conceptual or lexical? Cognition & Emotion, 2, 95103.Google Scholar
Otman, S. (2000). Conceptual metaphor in the archaeological record: Methods and examples from the American Southwest. American Antiquity, 65, 214265.Google Scholar
Ozcaliskan, S. (2003a). Metaphorical motion in crosslinguistic perspective: A comparison of English and Turkish. Metaphor and Symbol, 18, 189228.Google Scholar
Ozcaliskan, S. (2003b). In a caravanserai with two doors I am walking day and night: Metaphors of death and life in Turkish. Cognitive Linguistics, 14, 281320.Google Scholar
Pancake, A. (1993). Taken by storm: The exploitation of metaphor in the Persian Gulf Metaphor and Symbolic Activity, 8, 281295.Google Scholar
Panther, K-U., & Thornburg, L. (Eds.) (2003). Metonymy and pragmatic inferencing. Amsterdam: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Panther, K-U., & Thornburg, L. (2007). Metonymy. In Geeraerts, D. & Cuyckens, H. (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of cognitive linguistics (pp. 236263). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Parker, G. (1998). Philosophy of metaphor: Science or poetry? Minds and Machines, 8, 423431.Google Scholar
Pauwels, E. (2013). Communication: Mind the metaphor. Nature, 500, 523524.Google Scholar
Pelosi, A-C. (2007). A psycholinguistic analysis of the metaphor ‘difficulties are weights.’ Linguagem em (Dis)curso, 7, 289404.Google Scholar
Peña Cervel, S. (2010). Macbeth revisited: A cognitive analysis. Metaphor and Symbol 26 (1), 122.Google Scholar
Pérez-Sobrino, P., & Julich, N. (2014). Let’s talk about music. A corpus-based account of musical motion. Metaphor and Symbol, 29, 298315.Google Scholar
Perlman, M., Clark, N., & Johansson Falck, M. (2015). Iconic prosody in story reading. Cognitive Science, 6, 13481368.Google Scholar
Pexman, P., Ferratti, T., & Katz, A. (2000). Discourse factors that influence the online reading of metaphor and irony. Discourse Processes, 29, 201222.Google Scholar
Pfaff, K., Gibbs, R., & Johnson, M. (1997). Metaphor in using and understanding euphemism and dysphemism. Applied Psycholinguistics, 18, 5983.Google Scholar
Phillips, B., & McQuarrie, E. (2007). Road map or secret weapon: The role of conceptual metaphor in shaping marketing communications about exercise. In Lowrey, T. (Ed.), Psycholinguistic phenomena in marketing communication (pp. 135154). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Pinker, S. (2007). The stuff of thought. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Pragglejaz Group. (2007). MIP: A method for identifying metaphorically-used words in discourse. Metaphor and Symbol, 22, 140.Google Scholar
Pritzker, S. (2007). Thinking hearts, feeling brains: Metaphor, culture, and the self in Chinese narratives of depression. Metaphor and Symbol, 22, 251274.Google Scholar
Psomadakis, C. (2007). Mapping metaphors in Modern Greek: Life is a journey. CamLing, 221–228. (paper presented at The Fifth Cambridge Postgraduate Conference in Language Research, www.ling.cam.ac.uk/camling/Manuscripts/CamLing2007_Psomadakis.pdf.Google Scholar
Puschmann, C., & Burgess, J. (2014). Metaphors of big data. International Journal of Communication, 8, 16901708.Google Scholar
Ptzemyslaw, J., van der Lans, J., & Hermans, C. (1998). Metaphor theories and religious language understanding. Metaphor and Symbol, 13, 287292.Google Scholar
Quinn, N. (1992). The cultural basis of metaphor. In Fernandez, J. (Ed.), Beyond metaphor: The theory of tropes in anthropology (pp. 5693). Stanford, California: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Radden, G. (2002). How metonymic are metaphors? In Dirven, R. & Pörings, R. (Eds.), Metaphor and metonymy in comparison and contrast (pp. 407434). Berlin, New York: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Radden, G., & Panther, K-U. (Eds.) (1999). Metonymy in thought and language. Amsterdam: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Rakova, M. (2002). The philosophy of embodied realism: A high price to pay? Cognitive Linguistics, 13, 215244.Google Scholar
Rakova, M. (2003). The extent of the literal: Metaphor, polysemy, and theories of concepts. New York: Palgrave Macmillan.Google Scholar
Read, S., Cesa, I., Jones, D., & Collins, N. (1990). When is the federal budget like a baby? Metaphor in political rhetoric. Metaphor and Symbol, 5, 125149.Google Scholar
Reddy, M. (1979). The conduit metaphor. In Ortony, A. (Ed.), Metaphor and thought (pp. 284310). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Reisfield, G., & Wilson, G. (2004). Use of metaphor in the discourse on cancer. Journal of Clinical Oncology, 22, 40244027.Google Scholar
Richardson, D., & Matlock, T. (2007). The integration of figurative language and static depictions: An eye-movement study of fictive motion. Cognition, 102, 129138.Google Scholar
Ritchie, D. (2003). Categories and similarities: A note on circularity. Metaphor and Symbol, 18, 4953.Google Scholar
Ritchie, D. (2006). Context and communication in metaphor. Basingstoke, UK: Palgrave.Google Scholar
Ritchie, D. (2008). X IS A JOURNEY: Embodied simulation in metaphor interpretation. Metaphor and Symbol, 23, 174199.Google Scholar
Ritchie, D. (2009). Relevance and simulation in metaphor. Metaphor and Symbol, 24, 249272.Google Scholar
Rizzolatti, G., & Craighero, L. (2004). The mirror neuron system. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 27, 169192.Google Scholar
Robins, S., & Mayer, R. (2000). The metaphor framing effect: Metaphorical reasoning about text-based dilemmas. Discourse Processes, 30, 5786.Google Scholar
Rohrer, T. (1991). To plow the sea: Metaphors for regional peace in Latin America. Metaphor and Symbolic Activity, 6, 163181.Google Scholar
Rohrer, T. (1995). The metaphorical logic of (political) rape: The new wor(l)d order. Metaphor and Symbol, 10, 115137.Google Scholar
Romaine, S. (1996). War and peace in the global greenhouse: Metaphors we die by. Metaphor and Symbolic Activity, 11, 175194.Google Scholar
Rose-Brooks, C. (1958). A grammar of metaphor. London: Secker & Warburg.Google Scholar
Ruiz de Mendoza, F., & Mairal, U. (2007). High-level metaphor and metonymy in meaning construction. In Radden, G., Kopolke, K., Berg, M., & Siemund, P. (Eds.), Aspects of meaning construction (pp. 3351). Amsterdam: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Ruiz de Mendoza, F., & Pérez Hernández, L. (2003). Metonymy and the grammar: Motivation, constraints and interaction. Language & Communication, 21, 321357.Google Scholar
Ruiz de Mendoza, F., & Pérez Hernández, L. (2011). The contemporary theory of metaphor: Myths, developments and challenges. Metaphor and Symbol, 26, 161185.Google Scholar
Ryan, C., & Ryan, K. (1981). A private battle. London: New English Library.Google Scholar
Samaniego, E. (2011). Translation Studies and the cognitive theory of metaphor. Review of Cognitive Linguistics, 9, 262279.Google Scholar
Sandra, D., & Rice, S. (1995). Network analyses of prepositional meaning: Mirroring whose mind – the linguist’s or the language user’s? Cognitive Linguistics, 6, 89130.Google Scholar
Sanford, D. (2008). Metaphor and phonological reduction in English idiomatic expressions. Cognitive Linguistics, 19, 585604.Google Scholar
Sanford, D. (2012). Metaphors are conceptual schemata that are emergent over tokens of use. Journal of Cognitive Science, 13, 211235.Google Scholar
Santiago, J., Lupiáñez, J., Pérez, E., & Funes, M. J. (2007). Time (also) flies from left to right. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14, 512516.Google Scholar
Sato, M., Schafer, A., & Bergen, B. (2014). Metaphor priming in sentence production: Concrete pictures affect abstract language production. Acta Psychologica, 56, 136142.Google Scholar
Schaffman, K. (2003). Weighting Metaphors: A response to Raymond W. Gibbs and “Hilary”. In Albright, A. & Gere, D. (Eds.), Taken by surprise: A dance improvisation reader (pp. 197202). Wesleyan University PressGoogle Scholar
Schmidt-Snoek, G., Drew, A., Barile, E., & Agausas, S. (2015). Auditory and motion metaphors have different scalp distributions: An ERP study. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 9, 126. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2015.00126.Google Scholar
Schmitt, C., Greifenstein, S., & Kappelhoff, H. (2014). Expressive movement and metaphoric meaning making in audio-visual media. In Müller, C., Cienki, A., & Fricke, E. (Eds), Body-language-communication: An international handbook on multimodality in human interaction (pp. 20922112). Berlin/New York: De Gruyter Mouton.Google Scholar
Schnall, S. (2011). Clean, proper, and tidy are more than the absence of dirty, disgusting and wrong. Emotion Review, 3, 264266.Google Scholar
Schnall, S. (2014). Are there basic metaphors? In Landau, M., Robinson, M., & Meier, B. (Eds.), The power of metaphor: Examining its influence on social life (pp. 225247). Washington D.C.: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Schnall, S., Benton, J., & Harvey, S. (2008). With a clean conscience: Cleanliness reduces the severity of moral judgments. Psychological Science, 19, 12191222.Google Scholar
Schroder, U. (2009). Preferential metaphorical conceptualizations in everyday discourse about love in the Brazilian and German speech communities. Metaphor and Symbol, 24, 105120.Google Scholar
Schubert, T.W. (2005). Your highness: Vertical positions as perceptual symbols of power. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 121.Google Scholar
Schweik, R. (1996). Painting as exploring and related metaphors in 20th century art commentary. Metaphor and Symbol, 12, 285296.Google Scholar
Sec, K., & Sweetser, E. (2014). Borobudur and Chartres: Religious spaces as performative real space blends, In Caballero, R. & Diaz, J. (Eds.), Sensuous cognition: Explorations into human sentience: Imagination, (e)motion and perception (pp. 265292). Berlin: Mouton.Google Scholar
Semino, E. (2005). The metaphorical construction of complex domains: The case of speech activity in English. Metaphor and Symbol, 20, 3570.Google Scholar
Semino, E. (2008). Metaphor in discourse. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Semino, E. (2010). Descriptions of pain, metaphor and embodied simulation. Metaphor and Symbol, 25, 205226.Google Scholar
Semino, E., & Steen, G. (2008). Metaphor in literature. In Gibbs, R. (Ed.), Cambridge handbook of metaphor and thought (pp. 232246). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Semino, E., Deignan, A., & Littlemore, J. (2013). Metaphor, genre and recontextualisation. Metaphor and Symbol, 28, 4159.Google Scholar
Semino, E., Heywood, J., & Short, M. (2004). Methodological problems in the analysis of a corpus of conversations about cancer. Journal of Pragmatics, 36, 12711294.Google Scholar
Sharifian, F. (2011). Cultural conceptualisations and language: Theoretical framework and applications. Amsterdam: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Sharifian, F., Dirven, R., Ning, Y., & Niemeier, S. (Eds.) (2008). Culture, body, and language. Conceptualizations of internal body organs across cultures and languages. Berlin: Mouton.Google Scholar
Sherman, M. (2001). One man’s battle with terminal illness. Lawrence, MA: Eagle-Tribune, January 28.Google Scholar
Shinohara, K., & Matsunaka, Y. (2009). Pictorial metaphors of emotion in Japanese comics. In Forceville, C. & Urios-Aparisi, E. (Eds.), Multimodal metaphor (pp. 265296). Berlin: Mouton De Gruyter.Google Scholar
Shutova, E. (2013). Metaphor identification as interpretation. In Second Joint Conference on Lexical and Computational Semantics (*SEM), Volume 1: Proceedings of the Main Conference and the Shared Task, (pp. 276285). Atlanta, Georgia, June 13–14.Google Scholar
Shutova, E. (2015). Design and evaluation of metaphor processing systems. Computational Linguistics, 41(4), 579623.Google Scholar
Simó, J. (2009). Chess metaphors in American English and Hungarian. Metaphor and Symbol, 24, 2954.Google Scholar
Siquerra, M., & Gibbs, R. (2007). Children’s acquisition of primary metaphors: A cross-linguistic study. Organon, 43, 161179.Google Scholar
Skoufaki, S. (2009). Investigating the source of idiom transparency intuitions. Metaphor and Symbol, 24, 2041.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Slepian, M., & Ambody, N. (2014). Simulating sensorimotor metaphors: Novel metaphor influence sensory judgments. Cognition, 130, 309314.Google Scholar
Slingerland, E. (2003). Effortless action: Wu-wei as conceptual metaphor and spiritual ideal in early China. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Sontag, S. (1991). Illness as metaphor and AIDS as metaphor. New York: Penguin.Google Scholar
Soriano, C., & Valenzuela, J. (2009). Are conceptual metaphors accessible online? A psycholinguistic exploration of the CONTROL IS UP metaphor. In Valenzuela, J., Rojo, A., & Soriano, C. (Eds.), Trends in cognitive linguistics: Theoretical and applied models (pp. 2949). Frankfurt: Peter Lang.Google Scholar
Soto-Andrade, J. (2014). Metaphors in mathematics education. In Leran, S. (Ed.), Encyclopedia of mathematics education (pp. 447453). Netherlands: Springer.Google Scholar
Sperber, D., & Wilson, D. (2008). A deflationary account of metaphors. In Gibbs, R. (Ed.), Cambridge handbook of metaphor and thought (pp. 84105). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Spitzer, M. (2004). Metaphor and musical thought. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Stec, K., & Sweetser, E. (2013). Borobudur and Chartres: Religious spaces as performative real-space blends. In Caballero, R. & Diaz Vera, J. E. (Eds.), Sensuous cognition (pp. 265291). Berlin: Mouton De Gruyter.Google Scholar
Steen, G. (1999). From linguistic to conceptual metaphor in five steps. In Gibbs, R. & Steen, G. (Eds.), Metaphor in cognitive linguistics (pp. 5577). Amsterdam: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Steen, G. (2007). Finding metaphor in grammar and usage. Amsterdam: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Steen, G. (2008). The paradox of metaphor: Why we need a three-dimensional model of metaphor. Metaphor and Symbol, 23, 213241.Google Scholar
Steen, G. (2013). Deliberate metaphor affords conscious metaphorical cognition. Journal of Cognitive Semiotics, 5, 179197.Google Scholar
Steen, G. (2014). The cognitive-linguistic revolution in metaphor studies. In Littlemore, J. & Taylor, J. (Eds.), Bloomsbury companion to cognitive linguistics (pp. 117142). London: Bloomsbury.Google Scholar
Steen, G. (2015). Developing, testing, and interpreting deliberate metaphor theory. Journal of Pragmatics, 90, 6772.Google Scholar
Steen, G., Dorst, A., Herrmann, J., Kaal, A., Krennmayr, T., & Pasma, T. (2010). A method for linguistic metaphor identification. From MIP to MIPVU. Amsterdam: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Stefanowitsch, A. (2006). Words and their metaphors: A corpus-based approach. In Gries, S. & Stefanowitsch, A. (Eds.), Corpora in cognitive linguistics: Corpus-based approaches to syntax and lexis (pp. 63104). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Stefanowitsch, A. (2011). Cognitive linguistics as a cognitive science. In Callies, M., Keller, W. R., & Lohöfer, A. (Eds.), Bi-directionality in the cognitive sciences: Avenues, challenges, and limitations (pp. 295310). Amsterdam: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Stevens, W. (1965). The necessary angel: Essays on reality and the imagination. New York: Vintage.Google Scholar
Storbeck, J., & Clore, G. L. (2008). Affective arousal as information: How affective arousal influences judgments, learning, and memory. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 2, 18241843.Google Scholar
Sullivan, K. (2006). How does art ‘speak’ and what does it ‘say’: Conceptual metaphor theory as a tool for understanding the artistic process. In Thought tools for a new generation: Essays on thought, ideas, and the power of expression (pp. 8189). Eugene, OR: Robert D. Clark Honors College.Google Scholar
Sullivan, K. (2009). The languages of art: How representational and abstract painters conceptualize their work in terms of language. Poetics Today, 30, 517560.Google Scholar
Sullivan, K. (2013). Frames and constructions in metaphoric language. Amsterdam: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Svanlund, J. (2007). Metaphor and convention. Cognitive Linguistics, 18, 4789.Google Scholar
Swan, J. (2002). Life without parole: Metaphor and discursive commitment. Style, 36, 446465.Google Scholar
Sweetser, E. (1990). From etymology to pragmatics: Metaphorical and cultural aspects of semantic structure. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Talebinejad, M., & Dastjeri, H. (2005). A cross-cultural study of animal metaphors: When owls are not wise! Metaphor and Symbol, 20, 133150.Google Scholar
Taub, S. (2002). Language from the body. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Tendahl, M. (2009). A hybrid theory of metaphor: Cognitive linguistics and relevance theory. Basingstoke, UK: Palgrave.Google Scholar
Tendahl, M., & Gibbs, R. (2008). Complementary perspectives on metaphor: Cognitive linguistics and relevance theory. Journal of Pragmatics, 40, 18231864.Google Scholar
Theodoropoulou, Μ. (2012). Metaphor and metonymies of joy and happiness in Greek: Toward an interdisciplinary perspective. Review of Cognitive Linguistics, 10, 156183.Google Scholar
Thibodeau, P., & Boroditsky, L. (2011). Metaphors we think with: The role of metaphor in reasoning. PLoS ONE, 6: e16782. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0016782.Google Scholar
Thibodeau, P., & Durgin, F. (2008). Productive figurative communication: Conventional metaphors facilitate the comprehension of related novel metaphors. Journal of Memory and Language, 58, 521540.Google Scholar
Tilley, C. (1999). Metaphor and material culture. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Tourangeau, R., & Rips, L. (1991). Interpreting and evaluating metaphors. Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 452472.Google Scholar
Turner, M. (1996). The literary mind. Princeton: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Türker, E. (2013). A corpus-based approach to emotion metaphors in Korean: A case study of anger, happiness and sadness. Review of Cognitive Linguistics, 11, 73144.Google Scholar
Utsumi, A. (2005). The role of feature emergence in metaphor appreciation. Metaphor and Symbol, 20, 151172.Google Scholar
Valenzuela, J., & Soriano, C. (2007). Reading anger stories: A lexical decision task as a test for the existence of metaphorical representation. In Ibarretxe-Antuñano, I., Inchaurralde, C., & Sánchez, J., (Eds.), Language, mind and the lexicon (pp. 281303). Frankfurt: Peter Lang.Google Scholar
Van Camp, J. (1996). Non-verbal metaphor: A non-explanation of meaning in dance. British Journal of Aesthetics, 36, 177187.Google Scholar
Velasco-Sacristán, M., & Fuentes-Olivera, P. (2006). Olfactory and olfactory-mixed metaphors in print ads of perfume. Annual of Review of Cognitive Linguistics, 4, 217252.Google Scholar
Vervaeke, J., & Kennedy, J. (1996). Metaphors in language and thought: Disproof and multiple meanings. Metaphor and Symbolic Activity, 11, 273284.Google Scholar
Vidali, A. (2010). Seeing what we know: Disability and theories of metaphor. Journal of Literary & Cultural Disability Studies, 4, 3354.Google Scholar
Vignone, M. (2012). Family, buildings and war: Organizations and conceptual metaphor. OD Practitioner, 44, 3437.Google Scholar
Wear, . (2015). Macmillian Dictionary Online. Retrived October 5, 2015, from www.macmillandictionary.com/dictionary/british/wear_1Google Scholar
Wei-lun, L., & Chiang, W-Y. (2007). Emptiness we live by: Metaphors and paradoxes in Buddhism’s Heart Sutra. Metaphor and Symbol, 25, 331335.Google Scholar
White, G. (1987). Proverbs and cultural models: An American psychology of problem solving. In Holland, D. & Quinn, N. (Eds.), Cultural models in language and thought (pp. 151172). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Wierzbicka, A. (1986). Metaphors linguists live by: Lakoff and Johnson contra Aristotle. Papers in Linguistics, 19, 287318.Google Scholar
Wilcox, P. (2004). A cognitive key: Metonymic and metaphorical mappings in American Sign Language. Cognitive Linguistics, 15, 197222.Google Scholar
Wilkie, K., Holland, S., & Mulholland, P. (2010). What can the language of musicians tell us about music interaction design? Computer Music Journal, 34, 3448.Google Scholar
Wilkowski, B., Meier, B., Robinson, M., Carter, M., & Feltman, R. (2009). “Hot-headed” is more than an expression: The embodied representation of anger in terms of heat. Emotion, 9, 464477.Google Scholar
Williams, L., & Bargh, J. (2008). Experiencing physical warm influences interpersonal warmth. Science, 322, 606607.Google Scholar
Wilson, D. (2011). Parallels and differences in the treatment of metaphor in relevance theory and cognitive linguistics. Intercultural Pragmatics, 8, 177196.Google Scholar
Wilson, D. & Sperber, D. (2002). Truthfulness and relevance. Mind, 111, 583632.Google Scholar
Wilson, N., & Gibbs, R. (2007). Real and imagined body movement primes metaphor comprehension. Cognitive Science, 31, 721731.Google Scholar
Winter, S. (1995). A clearing in the forest. Metaphor and Symbolic Activity, 10, 223245.Google Scholar
Winter, S. (2002). A clearing in the forest: Law, life, and mind. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Wiseman, R. (2007). Ancient Roman metaphors for communication. Metaphor and Symbol, 22, 4178.Google Scholar
Wiseman, R. (2014). Social distance in hunter-gather settlement sites: A conceptual metaphor in material culture. Metaphor and Symbol, 29, 129143.Google Scholar
Wolter, S., Dudschig, C., de la Vega, I., & Kaup, B. (2015). Musical metaphors: Evidence for a spatial grounding of non-literal sentences describing auditory events. Acta Psychologica, 156, 126135.Google Scholar
Yu, N. (1995). Metaphorical expressions of anger and happiness in English and Chinese. Metaphor and Symbolic Activity, 10, 5992.Google Scholar
Yu, N. (1999). The contemporary theory of metaphor: A perspective from Chinese. Amsterdam: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Yu, N. (2002). Metaphor, Body, and Culture: The Chinese understanding of gallbladder and courage. Metaphor and Symbol, 18, 1331.Google Scholar
Yu, N. (2003). Chinese metaphors of thinking. Cognitive Linguistics 14, 141165.Google Scholar
Yu, N. (2008). The Chinese heart in a cognitive perspective: Culture, body, and language. Berlin: Mouton.Google Scholar
Yu, N. (2011). A decompositional approach to metaphorical compound analysis: The case of a TV commercial. Metaphor and Symbol, 26, 243259.Google Scholar
Zaltman, G., & Zaltman, L. (2008). Marketing metaphoria: What deep metaphors reveal about the minds of consumers. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Business Press.Google Scholar
Zanolie, K., van Dantzig, S., Boot, I., Wijnen, J., Schubert, T. Giessner, S., & Pechner, D. (2012). Mighty metaphors: Behavioral and ERP evidence that power shifts attention on a vertical dimension. Brain and Cognition, 78, 5058.Google Scholar
Zbikowski, L. (2002). Conceptualizing music. Cognitive structure, theory, and analysis. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Zbikowski, L. (2009). Music, language and multimodal metaphor. In Forceville, C. & Urios-Aparisi, E. (Eds.), Multimodal metaphor (pp. 359382). Berlin: Mouton De Gruyter.Google Scholar
Zbikowski, L. (2013). Listening to music. In Chapin, K. & Clarke, A. (Eds.), Speaking of music: Addressing the sonorous (pp. 101119). New York: Fordham University Press.Google Scholar
Zhong, C. B., & Liljenquist, K. (2006). Washing away your sins: Threatened morality and physical cleansing. Science, 313, 14511452.Google Scholar
Zhong, C. B., Lake, V., & Gino, F. (2010). A good lamp is the best police: Darkness increases dishonesty and self-interested behavior. Psychological Science, 21, 311314.Google Scholar
Zinken, J. (2007). Discourse metaphors: The link between figurative language and habitual analogies. Cognitive Linguistics, 18, 445466.Google Scholar
Zinken, J., Hellsten, I., & Nerlich, B. (2008). Discourse metaphors. In Frank, R., Dirven, R., Ziemke, T., & Bernárdez, E. (Eds.), Body, language, and mind: Vol. 2. Sociocultural situatedness (pp. 363386). Amsterdam: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Zlatev, J., Racine, T., Sinha, C., & Itkonen, E. (Eds.) (2008). The shared mind: Perspectives on intersubjectivity. Amsterdam: Benjamins.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

  • References
  • Raymond W. Gibbs, Jr, University of California, Santa Cruz
  • Book: Metaphor Wars
  • Online publication: 01 June 2017
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/9781107762350.008
Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

  • References
  • Raymond W. Gibbs, Jr, University of California, Santa Cruz
  • Book: Metaphor Wars
  • Online publication: 01 June 2017
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/9781107762350.008
Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

  • References
  • Raymond W. Gibbs, Jr, University of California, Santa Cruz
  • Book: Metaphor Wars
  • Online publication: 01 June 2017
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/9781107762350.008
Available formats
×