Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-ttngx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-28T16:02:57.851Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

21 - Evolutionary Developmental Psychology

Developing Adaptations in Infancy and Childhood

from Part VI - Evolution and Development

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 March 2020

Lance Workman
Affiliation:
University of South Wales
Will Reader
Affiliation:
Sheffield Hallam University
Jerome H. Barkow
Affiliation:
Dalhousie University, Nova Scotia
Get access

Summary

Evolutionary developmental psychology examines development through the lens of evolutionary theory. We first discuss adaptations of infancy and childhood, including ontogenetic, deferred, and conditional adaptations. The important ontogenetic adaptations reviewed include neonatal imitation and cognitive immaturity during childhood. Sex differences in play style as deferred adaptations are also discussed. Conditional adaptations, with children adjusting their developmental trajectory as a function of early life conditions, are examined from the perspective of life history theory. We then provide a framework for how adaptations develop, discussing evolved probabilistic cognitive mechanisms, evolved information-processing mechanisms that are expressed in a probabilistic fashion in each individual in a generation based on the continuous and bidirectional interaction over time at all levels of organization, from the genetic through the cultural. We provide examples of evolved probabilistic cognitive mechanisms from the domains of infant face perception, social learning, prepared fears, and tool use. Adaptations are central to an evolutionary psychological explication, and understanding how such adaptations develop enhances our understanding of adaptations.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2020

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abravanel, E., & Sigafoos, A. D. (1984). Exploring the presence of imitation during early infancy. Child Development, 55, 381392.Google Scholar
Anzures, G., Wheeler, A., Quinn, P. C., et al. (2012). Brief daily exposures to Asian females reverses perceptual narrowing for Asian faces in Caucasian infants. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 112(4), 484495.Google Scholar
Bardi, L., Regolin, L., & Simion, F. (2014). The first time ever I saw your feet: Inversion effect in newborns’ sensitivity to biological motion. Developmental Psychology, 50(4), 986.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beier, J. S., & Spelke, E. S. (2012). Infants’ developing understanding of social gaze. Child Development, 83(2), 486496.Google Scholar
Belsky, J., & Most, R. K. (1981). From exploration to play: A cross-sectional study of infant free play behavior. Developmental Psychology, 17(5), 630.Google Scholar
Belsky, J., Steinberg, L., & Draper, P. (1991). Childhood experience, interpersonal development, and reproductive strategy: An evolutionary theory of socialization. Child Development, 62(4), 647670.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Belsky, J., Steinberg, L. D., Houts, R. M., et al. (2007). Family rearing antecedents of pubertal timing. Child Development, 78(4), 13021321.Google Scholar
Bjorklund, D. F. (1987). How age changes in knowledge base contribute to the development of children’s memory: An interpretive review. Developmental Review, 7(2), 93130.Google Scholar
Bjorklund, D. F. (1997). The role immaturity in human development. Psychological Bulletin, 122(2), 153169.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bjorklund, D. F. (2003). Evolutionary psychology from a developmental systems perspective: comment on Lickliter and Honeycutt (2003). Psychological Bulletin, 129, 836841.Google Scholar
Bjorklund, D. F. (2007). Why Youth Is Not Wasted on the Young: Immaturity in Human Development. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Bjorklund, D. F. (2015). Developing adaptations. Developmental Review, 38, 1335.Google Scholar
Bjorklund, D. F. (2016). Prepared is not preformed: Comment on Witherington and Lickliter. Human Development, 59, 235241.Google Scholar
Bjorklund, D. F., & Ellis, B. J. (2014). Children, childhood, and development in evolutionary perspective. Developmental Review, 34(3), 225264.Google Scholar
Bjorklund, D. F., Gaultney, J. F., & Green, B. L. (1993). “I watch therefore I can do”: The development of meta-imitation over the preschool years and the advantage of optimism in one’s imitative skills. In Pasnak, R. & Howe, M. L., eds., Emerging Themes in Cognitive Development, Vol. 1. New York: Springer-Verlag, pp. 79102.Google Scholar
Bjorklund, D. F., & Green, B. L. (1992). The adaptive nature of cognitive immaturity. American Psychologist, 47, 4654.Google Scholar
Bjorklund, D. F., & Pellegrini, A. D. (2002). The Origins of Human Nature: Evolutionary Developmental Psychology. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Bjorklund, D. F., Ellis, B. J., & Rosenberg, J. S. (2007). Evolved probabilistic cognitive mechanisms: An evolutionary approach to gene × environment × development interactions. In Kail, R. V., ed., Advances in Child Development and Behavior, Vol. 35. Oxford: Elsevier, pp. 139.Google Scholar
Bjorklund, D. F., Hernández Blasi, C., & Ellis, B. J. (2016). Evolutionary developmental psychology. In Buss, D. M., ed., Evolutionary Psychology Handbook, 2nd ed., Vol. 2. New York: Wiley, pp. 904925.Google Scholar
Boyce, W. T., & Ellis, B. J. (2005). Biological sensitivity to context: I. An evolutionary-developmental theory of the origins and functions of stress reactivity. Development and Psychopathology, 17, 271301.Google Scholar
Brooks, R., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2002). The importance of eyes: How infants interpret adult looking behavior. Developmental Psychology, 38, 958966.Google Scholar
Buss, D. M., Haselton, M. G., Shackelford, T. K., Bleske, A. L., & Wakefield, J. C. (1998). Adaptations, exaptations, and spandrels. American Psychologist, 53, 533548.Google Scholar
Byrne, R. W. (2005). Social cognition: Imitation, imitation, imitation. Current Biology, 15, R489R500.Google Scholar
Cook, M., & Mineka, S. (1989). Observational conditioning of fear to fear-relevant versus fear-irrelevant stimuli in rhesus monkeys. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 98, 448459.Google Scholar
Costello, E. J., Sung, M., Worthman, C., & Angold, A. (2007). Pubertal maturation and the development of alcohol use and abuse. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 88(Suppl. 1), S50S59.Google Scholar
Csibra, G., & Gergely, G. (2011). Natural pedagogy as evolutionary adaptation. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 366, 11491157.Google Scholar
Cutting, N., Apperly, I. A., Chappell, J., & Beck, S. R. (2014). The puzzling difficulty of tool innovation: Why can’t children piece their knowledge together? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 125, 110117.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DeLoache, J. S., & LoBue, V. (2009). The narrow fellow in the grass: Human infants associate snakes and fear. Developmental Science, 12, 201207.Google Scholar
Di Giorgio, E., Leo, I., Pascalis, O., & Simion, F. (2012). Is the face-perception system human-specific at birth? Developmental Psychology, 48, 10831090.Google Scholar
Dobzhansky, T. (1973). Genetic Diversity and Human Equality. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Dunlosky, J., & Rawson, K. A. (2012). Overconfidence produces underachievement: Inaccurate self-evaluations undermine students’ learning and retention. Learning and Instruction, 22, 271280.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, B. J. (2004). Timing of pubertal maturation in girls: An integrated life history approach. Psychological Bulletin, 130, 920958.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ellis, B. J., & Graber, J. (2000). Psychosocial antecedents of variation in girls’ pubertal timing: Maternal depression, stepfather presence, and marital and family stress. Child Development, 71, 485501.Google Scholar
Ellis, B. J., Figueredo, A. J., Brumbach, B. H., & Schlomer, G. L. (2009). Fundamental dimensions of environmental risk: The impact of harsh versus unpredictable environments on the evolution and development of life history strategies. Human Nature, 20, 204268.Google Scholar
Ellis, B. J., McFadyen-Ketchum, S., Dodge, K. A., Pettit, G. S., & Bates, J. E. (1999). Quality of early family relationships and individual differences in the timing of pubertal maturation in girls: A longitudinal test of an evolutionary model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 387401.Google Scholar
Flynn, E., & Whiten, A. (2012). Experimental “microcultures” in young children: Identifying biographic, cognitive, and social predictors of information transmission. Child Development, 83(3), 911925.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Furlow, F. B. (1997). Human neonatal cry quality as an honest signal of fitness. Evolution and Human Behavior, 18, 175193.Google Scholar
Gardiner, A. K. (2014). Beyond irrelevant actions: Understanding the role of intentionality in children’s imitation of relevant actions. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 119, 5472.Google Scholar
Gardiner, A., Bjorklund, D. F., Greif, M. L., & Gray, S. K. (2012). Choosing and using tools: Prior experience and task difficulty influence preschoolers’ tool-use strategies. Cognitive Development, 27, 240254.Google Scholar
Gardiner, A., Greif, M., & Bjorklund, D. F. (2011). Guided by intention: Preschoolers’ imitation reflects inferences of causation. Journal of Cognition and Development, 12, 355373.Google Scholar
Gava, L., Valenza, E., Turati, C., & de Schonen, S. (2008). Effect of partial occlusion on newborns’ face preference and recognition. Developmental Science, 11, 563574.Google Scholar
Geary, D. C. (1995). Reflections of evolution and culture in children’s cognition: Implications for mathematical development and instruction. American Psychologist, 50, 2437.Google Scholar
Geary, D. C. (2002). Sexual selection and human life history. In Kail, R. V., ed., Advances in Child Development and Behavior, Vol. 30. San Diego, CA: Academic Press, pp. 41101.Google Scholar
Geary, D. C. (2005). The Origin of Mind: Evolution of Brain, Cognition, and General Intelligence. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Geary, D. C. (2010). Male, Female: The Evolution of Human Sex Differences, 2nd ed. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Geary, D. C., & Bjorklund, D. F. (2000). Evolutionary developmental psychology. Child Development, 71, 5765.Google Scholar
Gergely, G., & Csibra, G. (2005). The social construction of the cultural mind: Imitative learning as a mechanism of human pedagogy. Interaction Studies, 6, 463481.Google Scholar
German, T. P., & Johnson, S. C. (2002). Function and the origins of the design stance. Journal of Cognition and Development, 3, 279300.Google Scholar
Graber, J. A., Brooks-Gunn, J., & Warren, M. P. (1995). The antecedents of menarcheal age: Heredity, family environment, and stressful life events. Child Development, 66, 346359.Google Scholar
Gredlein, J. M., & Bjorklund, D. F. (2005). Sex differences in young children’s use of tools in a problem-solving task: The role of object-oriented play. Human Nature, 16, 211232.Google Scholar
Heimann, M. (1989). Neonatal imitation gaze aversion and mother–infant interaction. Infant Behavior & Development, 12, 495505.Google Scholar
Hernández Blasi, C., & Bjorklund, D. F. (2003). Evolutionary developmental psychology: A new tool for better understanding human ontogeny. Human Development, 46, 259281.Google Scholar
Hill, K. (1993). Life history theory and evolutionary anthropology. Evolutionary Anthropology, 2, 7888.Google Scholar
Hill, K., & Kaplan, H. (1999). Life history traits in humans: Theory and empirical studies. Annual Review of Anthropology, 28, 397430.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoehl, S., & Pauen, S. (2017). Do infants associate spiders and snakes with fearful facial expressions? Evolution and Human Behavior, 38, 404413.Google Scholar
Hornik, R., Risenhoover, N., & Gunnar, M. (1987). The effects of maternal positive, neutral, and negative affective communications on infant responses to new toys. Child Development, 58, 937944.Google Scholar
Jacobson, S. W. (1979). Matching behavior in the young infant. Child Development, 50, 425430.Google Scholar
Kaplan, H., & Lancaster, J. B. (2003). An evolutionary and ecological analysis of human fertility, mating patterns and parental investment. In Wachter, K. W. & Bulatao, R. A., eds., Offspring: Fertility Behavior in Biodemographic Perspective. Washington, DC: National Research Council, National Academies Press, pp. 170223.Google Scholar
Kaplan, H., Hill, K., Lancaster, J., & Hurtado, A. M. (2000). A theory of human life history evolution: Diet intelligence, and longevity. Evolutionary Anthropology, 9, 156185.3.0.CO;2-7>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kasturiratne, A., Wickremasinghe, A. R., de Silva, N., et al. (2008). The global burden of snakebite: A literature analysis and modelling based on regional estimates of envenoming and deaths. PLoS Medicine, 5, 15911604.Google Scholar
Keeley, L. H. (1996). War before Civilization: The Myth of the Peaceful Savage. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Kelly, D. J., Liu, S., Lee, K., et al. (2009). Development of the other-race effect in infancy: Evidence toward universality? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 104, 105114.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kelly, D. J., Quinn, P. C., Slater, A. M., et al. (2007). The other-race effect develops during infancy. Psychological Science, 18, 10841089.Google Scholar
Kenward, B. (2012). Over-imitating preschoolers believe unnecessary actions are normative and enforce their performance by a third party. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 112, 195207.Google Scholar
Konner, M. (2010). The Evolution of Childhood: Relationships, Emotions, Mind. Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press.Google Scholar
Lee, K., Quinn, P. C., & Pascalis, O. (2017). Face race processing and racial bias in early development: A perceptual-social linkage. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 26, 256262.Google Scholar
Legare, C. H. (2017). Cumulative cultural learning: Development and diversity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 114(30), 78777883.Google Scholar
Legerstee, M. (1991). The role of person and object in eliciting early imitation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 51, 423433.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liszkowski, U., Carpenter, M., Striano, T., & Tomasello, M. (2006). 12- and 18-month-olds point to provide information for others. Journal of Cognition and Development, 7, 173187.Google Scholar
Liszkowski, U., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2007). Pointing out new news, old news, and absent referents at 12 months of age. Developmental Science, 10, F1F7.Google Scholar
LoBue, V. (2010). And along came a spider: An attentional bias for the detection of spiders in young children and adults. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 107, 5966.Google Scholar
LoBue, V. (2013). What are we so afraid of? How early attention shapes our most common fears. Child Development Perspectives, 7, 3842.Google Scholar
LoBue, V., & DeLoache, J. S. (2010). Superior detection of threat-relevant stimuli in infancy. Developmental Science, 13, 221228.Google Scholar
LoBue, V., & Rakison, D. H. (2013). What we fear most: Developmental advantage for threat-relevant stimuli. Developmental Review, 33, 285303.Google Scholar
Lockhart, K. L., Chang, B., & Tyler, S. (2002). Young children’s belief about the stability of traits: Protective optimism. Child Development, 73, 14081430.Google Scholar
Lockman, J. J., & Kahrs, B. A. (2017). New insights into the development of human tool use. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 26, 330334.Google Scholar
Lyons, D. E., Young, A. G., & Keil, F. C. (2007). The hidden structure of overimitation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104, 1975119756.Google Scholar
McGuigan, N., Makinson, J., & Whiten, A. (2011). From over-imitation to super-copying: Adults imitate causally irrelevant aspects of tool use with higher fidelity than young children. British Journal of Psychology, 102, 118.Google Scholar
Meaney, M. J. (2007). Environmental programming of phenotypic diversity in female reproductive strategies. Advances in Genetics, 59, 173215.Google Scholar
Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1977). Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates. Science, 198, 7578.Google Scholar
Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1992). Early imitation within a functional framework: The importance of person identity, movement, and development. Infant Behavior and Development, 15(4), 479505.Google Scholar
Menard, J. L., & Hakvoort, R. M. (2007). Variations of maternal care alter offspring levels of behavioural defensiveness in adulthood: Evidence for a threshold model. Behavioural Brain Research, 176, 302313.Google Scholar
Mondloch, C. J., Lewis, T. M., Budreau, D. R., et al. (1999). Face perception during early infancy. Psychological Science, 10, 419422.Google Scholar
Mood, D. W. (1979). Sentence comprehension in preschool children: Testing an adaptive egocentrism hypothesis. Child Development, 50: 247250.Google Scholar
Nagy, E., & Molnar, P. (2004). Homo imitans or Homo provocans? Human imprinting model of neonatal imitation. Infant Behavior and Development, 27(1), 5463.Google Scholar
Nettle, D. (2010). Dying young and living fast: Variation in life history across English neighborhoods. Behavioral Ecology, 21, 387395.Google Scholar
Nettle, D., & Cockerill, M. (2010). Development of social variation in reproductive schedules: A study of an English urban area. PLoS ONE, 5, e12690.Google Scholar
Nielsen, M. (2006). Copying actions and copying outcomes: Social learning through the second year. Developmental Psychology, 42, 555565.Google Scholar
Nielsen, M., & Tomaselli, K. (2010). Over-imitation in the Kalahari Desert and the origins of human cultural cognition. Psychological Science, 5, 729736.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Öhman, A., Flykt, A., & Esteves, F. (2001). Emotion drives attention: Detecting the snake in the grass. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 466478.Google Scholar
Oostenbroek, J., Suddendorf, T., Nielsen, M., et al. (2016). Comprehensive longitudinal study challenges the existence of neonatal imitation in humans. Current Biology, 26(10), 13341338.Google Scholar
Oppenheim, R. W. (1981). Ontogenetic adaptations and retrogressive processes in the development of the nervous system and behavior. In Connolly, K. J. & Prechtl, H. F. R., eds., Maturation and Development: Biological and Psychological Perspectives. Philadelphia, PA: International Medical Publications, pp. 73108.Google Scholar
Parent, C. I., & Meaney, M. J. (2008). The influence of natural variations in maternal care on play fighting in the rat. Developmental Psychobiology, 50, 767776.Google Scholar
Pascalis, O., de Haan, M., & Nelson, C. A. (2002). Is face processing species-specific during the first year of life? Science, 296, 13211323.Google Scholar
Pascalis, O., Scott, L. S., Kelly, D. J., et al. (2005). Plasticity of face processing in infancy. Proceeding of the National Academy of Sciences, 102, 52975300.Google Scholar
Pellegrini, A. D. (2013). Play. In Zelazo, P., ed., Oxford Handbook of Developmental Psychology. New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 276299.Google Scholar
Pellegrini, A. D., & Smith, P. K. (1998). Physical activity play: The nature and function of neglected aspect of play. Child Development, 69, 577598.Google Scholar
Placek, C. D., & Quinlan, R. J. (2012). Adolescent fertility and risky environments: A population-level perspective across the lifespan. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 279, 40034008.Google Scholar
Quinlan, R. J. (2007). Human parental effort and environmental risk. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 274, 121125.Google Scholar
Rakison, D. H. (2005). Infant perception and cognition: An evolutionary perspective on early learning. In Ellis, B. J. & Bjorklund, D. F., eds., Origins of the Social Mind: Evolutionary Psychology and Child Development. New York: Guilford, pp. 317353.Google Scholar
Ross, J., Anderson, J. R., & Campbell, R. N. (2011). I remember me: Mnemonic self-reference effects in preschool children. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 76(3), 300.Google Scholar
Saxbe, D. E., & Repetti, R. L. (2009). Brief report: Fathers’ and mothers’ marital relationship predicts daughters’ pubertal development two years later. Journal of Adolescence, 32, 415423.Google Scholar
Shin, H. E., Bjorklund, D. F., & Beck, E. F. (2007). The adaptive nature of children’s overestimation in a strategic memory task. Cognitive Development, 22, 197212.Google Scholar
Simpson, J. A., & Belsky, J. (2008). Attachment theory within a modern evolutionary framework: Theory, research, and clinical applications. In Cassidy, J. & Shaver, P. R., eds., Handbook of Attachment: Theory, Research, and Clinical Applications, 2nd ed. New York: Guilford Press, pp. 131157.Google Scholar
Simpson, J. A., Griskevicius, V., Kuo, S., Sung, S., & Collins, W. A. (2012). Evolution, stress, and sensitive periods: The influence of unpredictability in early versus late childhood on sex and risky behavior. Developmental Psychology, 48, 674686.Google Scholar
Stearns, S. (1992). The Evolution of Life Histories. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Thompson, R. A. (2006). The development of the person: Social understanding, relationships, conscience, self. In Damon, W. & Lerner, R. M., gen. eds.; Eisenberg, N., vol. ed., Handbook of Child Psychology: Vol. 3, Social, Emotional, and Personality Development, 6th ed. New York: Wiley, pp. 2498.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2000). Culture and cognitive development. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 9, 3740.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2009). Why We Cooperate. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M., & Carpenter, M. (2007). Shared intentionality. Developmental Science, 10, 121125.Google Scholar
Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (1992). The psychological foundations of culture. In Cosmides, L., Tooby, J., & Barkow, J. H., eds., The Adapted Mind. New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 19136.Google Scholar
Trickett, P. K., Noll, J. G., & Putnam, F. W. (2011). The impact of sexual abuse on female development: Lessons from a multigenerational, longitudinal research study. Development and Psychopathology, 23, 453476.Google Scholar
Vaish, A., & Striano, T. (2004). Is visual reference necessary? Contributions of facial versus vocal cues in 12-month-olds’ social referencing behavior. Developmental Science, 7, 261269.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Volk, A. A., & Atkinson, J. A. (2013). Infant and child death in the human environment of evolutionary adaptation. Evolution and Human Behavior, 34, 182192.Google Scholar
Wakako, S., Wada, K., Yamamoto, T., Mohri, I., & Taniike, M. (2014). Development of preference for conspecific faces in human infants. Developmental Psychology, 50, 979985.Google Scholar
Whiten, A., McGuigan, N., Marshall-Pescini, S., & Hopper, L. M. (2009). Emulation, imitation, over-imitation and the scope of culture for child and chimpanzee. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 364, 24172428.Google Scholar
Williams, G. C. (1966). Adaptation and Natural Selection. Princeton, NJ: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Williamson, R. A., & Markman, E. M. (2006). Precision of imitation as a function of preschoolers’ understanding of the goal of the demonstration. Developmental Psychology, 42, 723731.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilson, M., & Daly, M. (1997). Life expectancy, economic inequality, homicide, and reproductive timing in Chicago neighborhoods. British Medical Journal, 314, 12711274.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×