Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-vvkck Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-28T14:15:22.132Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Bibliography

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  07 August 2018

Simon Harrison
Affiliation:
City University of Hong Kong
Get access
Type
Chapter
Information
The Impulse to Gesture
Where Language, Minds, and Bodies Intersect
, pp. 216 - 227
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2018

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Andrén, M. (2010). Children’s gestures from 18 to 30 months. Lund: Centre for Languages and Literature, Lund University.Google Scholar
Andrén, M.(2014). Multimodal constructions in children: Is the headshake part of language? Gesture 14(2), 141–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bavelas, J. B., Chovil, N., Lawrie, D. A., and Wade, A. (1992). Interactive gestures. Discourse Processes, 15, 469–89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beate, H. (ed.). (2005). From perception to meaning: Image schemas in cognitive linguistics. Berlin and Boston: De Gruyter Mouton.Google Scholar
Beattie, G. and Shovelton, H. (1999). Mapping the range of information contained in the iconic hand gestures that accompany spontaneous speech. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 18, 438–62.Google Scholar
Beaupoil-Hourdel, P., Boutet, D., and Morgenstern, A. (2015). A child’s multimodal negations from 1 to 4: The interplay between modalities. In Larrivée, P. and Lee, C. (Eds.), Negation and polarity: Experimental perspectives, language, cognition, and mind (pp. 95123). Cham, Switzerland: Springer.Google Scholar
Bergmann, K., Aksu, V., and Kopp, S. (2011). The relation of speech and gestures: Temporal synchrony follows semantic synchrony. Proceedings of the 2nd Workshop on Gesture and Speech in Interaction (GeSpIn 2011).Google Scholar
Bhatia, V. (2004). Worlds of written discourse: A genre-based view. London: Continuum.Google Scholar
Boutet, D. (2010). Structuration physiologique de la gestuelle: modèles et tests. Lidil, 42, 7796.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bressem, J. (2012). Repetitions in gesture: Structures, functions, and cognitive aspects. Ph.D. diss., Europa-Universtität Viadrina, Frankfurt (Oder).Google Scholar
Bressem, J.(2013). A linguistic perspective on the notation of form features in gestures. In Müller, C., Cienki, A., Fricke, E., Ladewig, S., McNeill, D., and Teßendorf, S. (Eds.), Body – Language – Communication: An international handbook on multi-modality in human interaction (pp. 1079–98). Berlin and Boston: De Gruyter Mouton.Google Scholar
Bressem, J. and Müller, C. (2014a). The family of Away gestures: Negation, refusal, and negative assessment. In Müller, C., Cienki, A., Fricke, E., Ladewig, S., McNeill, D., and Bressem, J. (Eds.), Body – Language – Communication: An international handbook on multi-modality in human interaction (pp. 1592–604). Berlin and Boston: De Gruyter Mouton.Google Scholar
Bressem, J. and Müller, C. (2014b). A repertoire of German recurrent gestures with pragmatic functions. In Müller, C., Cienki, A., Fricke, E., Ladewig, S., McNeill, D. and Bressem, J. (Eds.), Body – Language – Communication: An international handbook on multi-modality in human interaction (pp. 1575–91). Berlin and Boston: De Gruyter Mouton.Google Scholar
Bressem, J. and Müller, C. (2017). The ‘Negative-Assessment-Construction’ – A multimodal pattern based on a recurrent gesture? Linguistics Vanguard, 3(s1). https://doi.org/10.1515/lingvan-2016-0053Google Scholar
Bressem, J., Ladewig, S., and Müller, C. (2013). Linguistic annotation system for gestures. In Müller, C., Fricke, E., Ladewig, S., McNeill, D., and Teßendorf, S. (Eds.), Body – Language – Communication: An international handbook on multi-modality in human interaction) (pp. 1098–124). Berlin and Boston: De Gruyter Mouton.Google Scholar
Bressem, J., Stein, N., and Wegener, C. (2015). Structuring and highlighting speech – Discursive functions of holding away gestures in Savosavo. Proceedings of the GESPIN 2015 Gesture Conference, September 2015, Nantes, France.Google Scholar
Bressem, J., Stein, N., and Wegener, C. (2017). Multimodal language use in Savosavo: Refusing, excluding and negating with speech and gesture. Pragmatics, 27(2), 173206.Google Scholar
Brookes, H. (2014). Gesture in the communicative ecology of a South African township. In Seyfeddinipur, M. and Gullberg, M. (Eds.), From gesture in conversation to visible action as utterance (pp. 59–73). Amsterdam and Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Brookes, H.(2015). The social nature of cognitive-semiotic processes in the semantic expansion of gestural forms. In Ferré, G. and Tutton, M. (Eds.), Proceedings of Gesture and Speech in Interaction 4. www.gespin4.univ-nantes.frGoogle Scholar
Calbris, G. (1990). The semiotics of French gesture. Bloomington, IN: Indiana University Press.Google Scholar
Calbris, G.(2003). From cutting an object to a clear-cut analysis: Gesture as the representation of a preconceptual schema linking concrete actions to abstract notions. Gesture, 3(1), 1946.Google Scholar
Calbris, G.(2005). La négation: son symbolisme physique. Paper presented at the Interacting Bodies, Corps en interaction. http://gesture-lyon2005.ens-lyon.fr/IMG/pdf/CalbrisFinal.pdfGoogle Scholar
Calbris, G.(2011). Elements of meaning in gesture. Amsterdam and Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Calbris, G. and Porcher, L. (1989). Geste et communication. Paris: Credif-Hatier.Google Scholar
Carter, R. and McCarthy, M. (2006). Cambridge grammar of English: A comprehensive guide: Spoken and written English grammar and usage. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Celce-Murcia, M. and Larsen-Freeman, D. (1999). The grammar book. Rowley, MA: Heinle & Heinle.Google Scholar
Cheshire, J. (1999). English negation from an interactional perspective. In Tieken-Boon van Ostade, I., Gunnel, T., and van der Wurff, W. (Eds.), Negation in the history of English (pp. 29–53). London: Longman.Google Scholar
Chilton, P. (2006). Negation as maximal distance in discourse space theory. In Bonnefille, S. and Salbayre, S. (Eds.), Negation: Form, figure of speech, conceptualization (pp. 351–78). Tours: Publications universitaires François Rabelais.Google Scholar
Chilton, P.(2014). Language, space and mind: The conceptual geometry of linguistic meaning.Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chui, K. (2005). Temporal patterning of speech and iconic gestures in conversational discourse. Journal of Pragmatics, 37, 871–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chui, K.(2009). Conversational coherence and gesture. Discourse Studies, 11(6), 661–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chui, K.(2015). Mimicked gestures and the joint construction of meaning in conversation. Journal of Pragmatics, 70, 6885.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cibulka, P. (2015). When the hands do not go home: A micro-study of the role of gesture phases in sequence suspension and closure. Discourse Studies, 17(1), 324.Google Scholar
Cienki, A. (2012). Usage events of spoken language and the symbolic units we (may) abstract from them. In Kosecki, K. and Badio, J. (Eds.), Cognitive processes in language (pp. 149–58). Bern: Peter Lang.Google Scholar
Cienki, A.(2013). Cognitive Linguistics: Language and gesture as expressions of conceptualization. In Müller, C., Cienki, A., Fricke, E., Ladewig, S. H., McNeill, D., and Teßendorf, S. (Eds.), Body – Language – Communication: An international handbook on multimodality in human interaction (pp. 182201). Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton.Google Scholar
Cienki, A.(2016). Cognitive linguistics, gesture studies, and multimodal communication. Cognitive Linguistics, 27(4), 603–18.Google Scholar
Cienki, A. and Müller, C. (Eds.). (2008). Metaphor and gesture. Amsterdam and Philadelphia: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coupland, N. (2007). Style: Language variation and identity. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Cuffari, E. and Jensen, T. W. (2014). Living bodies: Co-enacting experience. In Müller, C., Cienki, A., Fricke, E., Ladewig, S., McNeill, D., and Bressem, J. (Eds.), Body – Language – Communication. An international handbook on multi-modality in human interaction (pp. 2016–25). Berlin and Boston: De Gruyter Mouton.Google Scholar
Cutting, J. (2008). Pragmatics and discourse: A resource book for students (2nd ed.). Routledge: Routledge English Language Introductions.Google Scholar
de Brabanter, P. (2007). Uttering sentences made up of words and gestures. In Romero, E. and Soria, B. (Eds.), Explicit communication: Robyn Carston’s pragmatics (pp. 199216). Basingstoke: Palgrave.Google Scholar
de Ruiter, J. P. (2000). The production of gesture and speech. In McNeill, D. (Ed.), Language and gesture (pp. 284311). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Dahl, Ö. (1979). Typology of sentence negation. Linguistics, 17, 79106.Google Scholar
Dahlmann, I. and Adolphs, S. (2009). Spoken corpus analysis: Multimodal approaches to language description. In Baker, P. (Ed.), Contemporary corpus linguistics (pp. 136–50). London: Continuum International Publishing.Google Scholar
Debras, C. (2015). Stance-taking functions of multimodal constructed dialogue during spoken interaction. In Ferré, G. and Tutton, M. (Eds.), Proceedings of Gesture and Speech in Interaction 4.Google Scholar
Dörnyei, Z. (2007). Research methods in applied linguistics. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Dosse, F. (2010). Gilles Deleuze and Félix Guattari: Intersecting lives. New York: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
Downing, A. and Locke, P. (2006). English grammar: A university course. Abingdon: Routledge.Google Scholar
Duffley, P. and Larrivée, P. (2012). Collocation, interpretation and explanation: The case of just any. Lingua, 122, 2440.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Edley, N. and Litosseliti, L. (2010). Contemplating interviews and focus groups. In Litosseliti, L (Ed.), Research methods in linguistics (pp. 155–79). London: Bloomsbury.Google Scholar
Emmorey, K. (2002). Language, cognition, and the brain: Insights from sign language research. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Enfield, N. (2009). The anatomy of meaning: Speech, gesture, and composite utterances. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Fauconnier, G. (1997). Mappings in thought and language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Fauconnier, G. and Turner, M. (2000). The way we think: Conceptual blending and the mind’s hidden complexities. New York: Basic BooksGoogle Scholar
Ferré, G. (2010). Timing relationships between speech and co-verbal gestures in spontaneous French. Proceedings of Language Resources and Evaluation, Workshop on Multimodal Corpora, Malta.Google Scholar
Fricke, E. (2012). Grammatik multimodal: Wie Wörter und Gesten zusammenwirken. Berlin and Boston: De Gruyter Mouton.Google Scholar
Fricke, E.(2013). Towards a unified grammar of gesture and speech: A multimodal approach. In Müller, C., Cienki, A., Fricke, E., Ladewig, S. H., McNeill, D, and Teßendorf, S. (Eds.), Body – Language – Communication. An international handbook on multimodality in human interaction (pp. 733–54). Berlin and Boston: De Gruyter Mouton.Google Scholar
Fricke, E.(2014a). Kinesthemes: Morphological complexity in co-speech gestures. In Müller, C., Cienki, A., Fricke, E., Ladewig, S. H., McNeill, D., and Bressem, J. (Eds.), Body – Language – Communication. An international handbook on multimodality in human interaction (pp. 1618–29). Berlin and Boston: De Gruyter Mouton.Google Scholar
Fricke, E.(2014b). Syntactic complexity in co-speech gestures: Constituency and recursion. In Müller, C., Cienki, A., Fricke, E., Ladewig, S. H., McNeill, D., and Bressem, J. (Eds.), Body – Language – Communication. An international handbook on multimodality in human interaction (pp. 1650–61). Berlin and Boston: De Gruyter Mouton.Google Scholar
Gee, Paul (2014). How to do discourse analysis: A tool kit. Abingdon: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Givón, T. (1993). English grammar: A function-based introduction. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Givón, T.(2001). Syntax. Volume 1. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Goffman, E. (1974). Frame analysis: An essay on the organization of experience. Boston: Northeastern University Press.Google Scholar
Goldberg, A. (1995). Constructions: A construction grammar approach to argument structure. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, S. (2003). Hearing gesture: How our hands help us think. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Goodwin, C. (2000). Action and embodiment within situated human interaction. Journal of Pragmatics, 32(10), 1489–522.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodwin, C.(2007). Environmentally coupled gestures. In Duncan, S., Cassell, H., and Levy, E. (Eds.), Gesture and the dynamic dimension of language. Essays in honor of David McNeill. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Green, J. (2014). Signs and space in Arandic sand narratives. In Seyfeddinipur, M. and Gullberg, M. (Eds.), From gesture in conversation to visible action as utterance: Essays in honor of Adam Kendon (pp. 219–43). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Hadar, U. (1989). Two types of gesture and their role in speech production. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 8(3–4), 221–8.Google Scholar
Haddington, P., Keisanen, T., Mondada, L. and Nevile, M. (Eds.). (2014). Multiactivity in social interaction: Beyond multitasking. Amsterdam and Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Handford, M. (2010). The language of business meetings. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Harré, R. and Secord, P. F. (1972) The explanation of social behavior. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Harrison, S. (2009a). The expression of negation through grammar and gesture. In Zlatev, J., Andrén, M., Johansson Falck, M., and Lundmark, C. (Eds.), Studies in language and cognition (pp. 421–35). Cambridge: Cambridge Scholars.Google Scholar
Harrison, S.(2009b). Grammar, gesture, and cognition: The case of negation in English. Ph.D. diss., Université de Bordeaux.Google Scholar
Harrison, S.(2010). Evidence for node and scope of negation in coverbal gesture. Gesture, 10(1), 2951.Google Scholar
Harrison, S.(2013). The creation and implementation of a gesture code for factory communication. Proceedings of GESPIN 2011: Gesture and speech in interaction, Bielefeld, 5–7 September 2011.Google Scholar
Harrison, S.(2014a). The organisation of kinesic ensembles related to negation. Gesture, 14(2), 117–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harrison, S.(2014b). Head shakes: Variation in form, function, and cultural distribution of a head movement related to ‘no’. In Müller, C., Cienki, A., Fricke, E., McNeill, D., and Bressem, J. (Eds.), Body – Language – Communication: An international handbook on multimodality in human interaction (pp. 1496–501). Berlin and Boston: De Gruyter Mouton.Google Scholar
Harrison, S.(2014c). Gestures in industrial settings. In Müller, C., Cienki, A., Fricke, E., McNeill, D., and Bressem, J. (Eds.), Body – Language – Communication: An international handbook on multimodality in human interaction (pp. 1413–19). Berlin and Boston: De Gruyter Mouton.Google Scholar
Harrison, S.(2015). Organisation of kinesic ensembles associated with negation. Gesture, 14(2), 117–41.Google Scholar
Harrison, S. and Larrivée, P. (2015). Morphosyntactic correlates of gestures: A gesture associated with negation in French and its organisation with speech. In Larrivée, P. and Chungmin, L. (Eds.), Negation and negative polarity. Experimental and cognitive perspectives (pp. 75–94). Dordrecht: Springer.Google Scholar
Harrison, S. and Williams, R. F. (2017). Monitoring the swimzone whilst finding south. Sustained orientation in multiactivity among beach lifeguards. Text & Talk, 37(6), 683711.Google Scholar
Hassemer, J. (2015). Towards a theory of gesture form analysis: Principles of gesture conceptualisation, with empirical support from motion-capture data. Ph.D. diss., RWTH Aachen University.Google Scholar
Heine, B. (1997). Cognitive foundations of grammar. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Heritage, J. (1984). Garfinkel and ethnomethodology. Cambridge: Polity Press.Google Scholar
Holmes, J. and Stubbe, M. (2003). Power and politeness in the workplace. A sociolinguistic analysis of talk at work. Harlow: Longman.Google Scholar
Horn, L. R. (1989). A natural history of negation. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Horn, L. R.(Ed.). (2010). The expression of negation. Berlin and Boston: De Gruyter Mouton.Google Scholar
Horn, L. R. and Wansing, H. (2017). Negation. In Zalta, E. N. (Ed.), The Stanford encyclopedia of philosophy (Spring edition). https://plato.stanford.edu/archives/spr2017/entries/negation/Google Scholar
Hoza, J. (2008). The discourse and politeness functions of hey and well in American sign language. In Roy, C. B. (Ed.), Discourse in signed languages. Washington, DC: Gallaudet University Press.Google Scholar
Huddleston, R. D. and Pullum, G. K. (2002). The Cambridge grammar of the English language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Huddleston, R. D. and Pullum, G. K.(2005). A student’s introduction to English grammar. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Hutchins, E. (1995). Cognition in the wild. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Hutchins, E.(2005). Material anchors for conceptual blends. Journal of Pragmatics, 37, 1555–77.Google Scholar
Hutchins, E. and Nomura, S. (2011). Collaborative construction of multimodal utterances. In Streeck, J., Goodwin, C., and LeBaron, C. (Eds.), Embodied interaction: Language and body in the material world (pp. 289304). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Jensen, T. W. and Cuffari, E. (2014). Doubleness in experience: Toward a distributed enactive approach to metaphoricity. Metaphor and Symbol, 29(4), 278–97.Google Scholar
Jespersen, O. (1924). The philosophy of grammar. London: Unwin Brothers.Google Scholar
Johnson, M. (1987). The body in the mind: The bodily basis of meaning, imagination, and reason. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Johnston, T. (2013). Towards a comparative semiotics of pointing actions in signed and spoken languages. Gesture, 13(2), 109–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnston, T. and Schembri, A. (2007). Australian sign language: An introduction to sign language linguistics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jones, J. H. (1994). All together now. Managing people in the business world in the 90s. London: Heinemann.Google Scholar
Kendon, A. (1980). Gesticulation and speech: Two aspects of the process of utterance. In Key, M. R. (Ed.), The relation between verbal and nonverbal communication (pp. 207–27). The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
Kendon, A. (1988). How gestures can become like words. In Poyatos, F. (Ed.), Cross-cultural perspectives in nonverbal communication (pp. 131–41). Toronto: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Kendon, A.(1994). An agenda for gesture studies. Semiotic Review of Books, 7(3). www.univie.ac.at/wissenschaftstheorie/srb/srb/gesture.htmlGoogle Scholar
Kendon, A.(1995). Gestures as illocutionary and discourse structure markers in southern Italian conversation. Journal of Pragmatics, 23, 247–79.Google Scholar
Kendon, A.(2002). Some uses of the head shake. Gesture, 2(2), 147–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kendon, A.(2004). Gesture. Visible action as utterance. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Kirchhof, C. (2011). So what’s your affiliation with gesture? In: Kirchhof, C., Malisz, Z., and Wagner, P. (Eds.) GeSpIn. Bielefeld.Google Scholar
Kirchhof, C.(2017). The shrink point: Audiovisual integration of speech-gesture synchrony. Bielefeld: Universität Bielefeld.Google Scholar
Kita, S. (2009). Cross-cultural variation of speech-accompanying gesture: A review. Language and Cognitive Processes, 24(2), 145–67.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kita, S., van Gijn, I., and van der Hulst, H. (1998). Movement phases in signs and co-speech gestures, and their transcription by human coders. In Wachsmuth, I. and Fröhlich, M. (Eds.), Gesture and sign language in human-computer interaction (pp. 23–35). Berlin and Heidelberg: Springer.Google Scholar
Klima, E. and Bellugi, U. (1979). The signs of language. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Kok, K. (2016). The grammatical potential of co-speech gesture: A Functional Discourse Grammar perspective. Functions of Language, 23(2), 149–78.Google Scholar
Kok, K. and Cienki, A. (2016). Cognitive grammar and gesture: Points of convergence, advances and challenges. Cognitive Linguistics, 27(1), 67100.Google Scholar
Krysthaliuk, A. (2012). The image-schematic dimension of English negation. In Kosecki, K. and Badio, J. (Eds.), Cognitive processes in language (pp. 99108). Bern: Peter Lang.Google Scholar
Labov, W. and Waletzky, J. (1967). Narrative analysis. In Helm, J. (Ed.), Essays on the verbal and visual arts (pp. 1244). Seattle: University of Washington Press.Google Scholar
Ladewig, S. H. (2011). Putting the cyclic gesture on a cognitive basis. CogniTextes, 6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ladewig, S. H.(2012). Syntactic and semantic integration of gestures into speech: Structural, cognitive, and conceptual aspects. Ph.D. diss., European University Viadrina, Frankfurt (Oder).Google Scholar
Ladewig, S. H.(2014a). The cyclic gesture. In Müller, C., Cienki, A., Fricke, E., Ladewig, S., McNeill, D., and Bressem, J. (Eds.), Body – Language – Communication: An international handbook on multi-modality in human interaction (pp. 1605–17). Berlin and Boston: De Gruyter Mouton.Google Scholar
Ladewig, S. H.(2014b). Recurrent gestures. In Müller, C., Cienki, A., Fricke, E., Ladewig, S., McNeill, D., and Bressem, J. (Eds.), Body – Language – Communication: An international handbook on multi-modality in human interaction (pp. 1558–74). Berlin and Boston: De Gruyter Mouton.Google Scholar
Lakoff, G. (1987). Women, fire and dangerous things. What categories reveal about the mind. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Lakoff, G. and Johnson, M. (1980). Metaphors we live by. Chicago: Chicago University Press.Google Scholar
Lakoff, G. and Johnson, M.(1999). Philosophy in the flesh: The embodied mind and its challenge to western thought. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Langacker, R. (1987). Foundations of cognitive grammar. Vol. 1: Theoretical prerequisites. Stanford: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Langacker, R. (1991a). Concept, image, and symbol: The cognitive basis of grammar. Berlin and New York: De Gruyter Mouton.Google Scholar
Langacker, R. (1991b). Foundations of cognitive grammar. Vol. 2: Descriptive application. Stanford: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Langacker, R. (2008). Cognitive grammar: A basic introduction. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Lapaire, J.-R. (2005). La grammaire anglaise en mouvement. Paris: Hachette.Google Scholar
Lapaire, J.-R.(2006a). Negation, reification and manipulation in a cognitive grammar of substance. In Bonnefille, S. and Salbayre, S. (Eds.), La négation (pp. 333–49). Tours: Press universitaires François-Rabelais.Google Scholar
Lapaire, J.-R.(2006b). From sensory to propositional modality. Towards a phenomenology of epistemic modal meanings. Corela, 4(1).Google Scholar
Lapaire, J.-R.(2007). The meaning of meaningless grams – or emptiness revisited. In Oleksy, W. and Stalmaszczyk, P. (Eds.), Cognitive approaches to language and linguistic data (pp. 241–58). Frankfurt: Peter Lang.Google Scholar
Lapaire, J.-R.(2011). Grammar, gesture and cognition: Insights from multimodal utterances and applications for gesture analysis. Visnyk of Lviv University, Philology Series 52, 88103.Google Scholar
Lapaire, J.-R.(2013). Gestualité cogrammaticale: de l’action corporelle spontanée aux postures de travail métagestuel guidé. Maybe et le balancement épistémique en anglais. Langages, 192, 5772.Google Scholar
Lapaire, J.-R.(2016). From ontological metaphor to semiotic make-believe: Giving shape and substance to fictive objects of conception with the ‘globe gesture’. Santa Cruz do Sul, 41(70), 2944.Google Scholar
Lapaire, J. R. and Rotgé, W. (2002). Linguistique et grammaire de l’anglais. Tours: Presses Universitaires du Mirail Toulouse.Google Scholar
Larrivée, P. (2017). Negation and polarity. In Stark, E. and Dufter, A. (Eds.), Manual of Romance morphosyntax and syntax (pp. 449–71). Berlin and Boston: De Gruyter Mouton.Google Scholar
Larrivée, P.(2001). L’interprétation des phrases négatives: portée et foyer des négations en français. Paris: Duculot.Google Scholar
Larrivée, P. and Lee, C. (Eds). (2016). Negation and polarity: Experimental perspectives. Berlin: Springer.Google Scholar
Lawler, J. (2005). Negation and NPIs. www-personal.umich.edu/~jlawler/NPIs.pdfGoogle Scholar
Leech, G. and Svartvik, J. (1994). A communicative grammar of English (2nd ed.). London and New York: Longman.Google Scholar
Lemmens, M. (2016). Idiogests: Semantically motivated gestural idiolects. Paper presented at the 7th Conference of the International Society of Gesture Studies. Paris, 18–20 July.Google Scholar
Lempert, M. (2011). Barrack Obama, being sharp. Indexical order in the pragmatics of precision-grip gesture. Gesture, 11(3), 241–70.Google Scholar
Lewandowska-Tomaszczyk, B. (2006). A cognitive-interactional model of direct and indirect negation. GRAAT, 35, 379402.Google Scholar
Licoppe, C. and Tuncer, S. (2014). Attending to a summons and putting other activities ‘on hold’: Multiactivity as a recognisable interactional accomplishment. In Haddington, P., Keisanen, T., Mondada, L., and Nevile, M. (Eds.), Multiactivity in social interaction: Beyond multitasking (pp. 167–90). Amsterdam and Berlin: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Liddell, S. K. (2003). Grammar, gesture, and meaning in American sign language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Liskova, E. (2012). Negation of KNOW, WANT, LIKE, HAVE, and GOOD in American Sign Language. Master’s diss., University of Texas at Austin.Google Scholar
McCleary, L. and Viotti, E. (2010). Sign-gesture symbiosis in Brazilian sign language narrative. In Parrill, F., Tobin, V., and Turner, M. (Eds.), Meaning, form, and body (pp. 181201). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
McNeill, D. (1992). Hand and mind: What gestures reveal about thought. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
McNeill, D.(1998). Models of speaking (to their amazement) meet speech-synchronized gestures. http://cogprints.org/665/1/McNeill_Catchments.htmlGoogle Scholar
McNeill, D.(2005). Gesture and thought. Chicago: Chicago University Press.Google Scholar
McNeill, D.(2012). How language began: Gesture and speech in human evolution. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McNeill, D.(2016). Why we gesture. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Mondada, L. (2016). Challenges of multimodality: Language and the body in social interaction. Journal of Sociolinguistics, 20(3), 336–66.Google Scholar
Morgenstern, A., Beaupoil-Hourdel, P., Blondel, M., and Boutet, D. (2016). A multimodal approach to the development of negation in signed and spoken languages: Four case studies. In Ortega, L., Tyler, A., Park, H., and Uno, M. (Eds.), The usage-based study of language learning and multilingualism (pp. 1536). Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press.Google Scholar
Mori, J. and Hayashi, M. (2006). The achievement of intersubjectivity through embodied completions: A study of interactions between first and second language speakers. Applied Linguistics, 27(2), 195219.Google Scholar
Morris, D. (1994). Bodytalk: A world guide to gestures. London: Jonathan Cape.Google Scholar
Morris, D.(2002). People watching: A field guide to human behaviour (rev. rpt edn). London: Vintage.Google Scholar
Müller, C. (2004). Forms and uses of the Palm Up Open Hand. A case of a gesture family? In Posner, R. and Müller, C. (Eds.), The semantics and pragmatics of everyday gestures (pp. 234–56). Berlin: Weidler Buchverlag.Google Scholar
Müller, C.(2010). Wie Gesten bedeuten. Eine kognitiv-linguistische und sequenzanalytische Perspektive. Sprache und Literatur, 41(1), 3768.Google Scholar
Müller, C.(2013). Gestures as a medium of expression: The linguistic potential of gestures. In Müller, C., Cienki, A., Fricke, E., Ladewig, S., McNeill, D., and Teßendorf, S. (Eds.), Body –Language – Communication: An international handbook on multi-modality in human interaction (pp. 202–17). Berlin and Boston: De Gruyter Mouton.Google Scholar
Müller, C.(2014). Gestural modes of representation as techniques of depiction. In Müller, C., Cienki, A., Fricke, E., Ladewig, S., McNeill, D., and Bressem, J. (Eds.), Body – Language – Communication: An international handbook on multi-modality in human interaction (pp. 1687–702). Berlin and Boston: De Gruyter Mouton.Google Scholar
Müller, C. and Tag, S. (2010). The dynamics of metaphor: Foregrounding and activating metaphoricity in conversational interaction. Cognitive Semiotics, 10(6), 85120.Google Scholar
Müller, C., Bressem, J., and Ladewig, S. (2013a). Towards a grammar of gestures: A form-based view. In Müller, C., Cienki, A., Fricke, E., Ladewig, S., McNeill, D. and Teßendorf, S. (Eds.), Body – Language – Communication: An international handbook on multi-modality in human interaction (HSK) (pp. 707–33). Berlin and Boston: De Gruyter Mouton.Google Scholar
Müller, C., Cienki, A., Fricke, E., Ladewig, S., McNeill, D., and Teßendorf, S. (2013b) Body – Language – Communication. An international handbook on multimodality in human interaction. Berlin and Boston: De Gruyter Mouton.Google Scholar
Müller, C., Cienki, A., Fricke, E., Ladewig, S., McNeill, D., and Bressem, J. (2014) Body – Language – Communication: An international handbook on multimodality in human interaction. Berlin and Boston: De Gruyter Mouton.Google Scholar
Neidle, C., Kegl, J., MacLaughlin, D., Bahan, B., and Lee, R. G. (2000). The syntax of American sign language: Functional categories and hierarchical structure. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Nobe, S. (2000). Where do most spontaneous representational gestures actually occur with respect to speech? In McNeill, D. (Ed.), Language and gesture (pp. 186–98). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Oakley, T. and Hougaard, A. (Eds.). (2008). Mental spaces in discourse and interaction. Amsterdam and Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Olsher, D. (2004). Talk and gesture: The embodied completion of sequential actions in spoken interaction. In Gardner, R. and Wagner, J. (Eds.), Second language conversations (pp. 221–45). London: Continuum International Publishing.Google Scholar
Oxford Concise English Dictionary, 9th ed. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Parrill, F. (2007). Metagesture: An analysis of theoretical discourse about multimodal language. In Duncan, S. D, Cassell, J. and Levy, E. T. (Eds.), Gesture and the dynamic dimension of language (pp. 83–9). Amsterdam and Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Parril, F., Bergen, B. K. and Lichtenstein, P. V. (2013). Grammatical aspect, gesture, and conceptualization: Using co-speech gesture to reveal event representations. Cognitive Linguistics, 24(1), 135–58.Google Scholar
Poletto, C. (2008). On negative doubling. Quaderni di lavoro ASIT, 1, 5784.Google Scholar
Prieto, P., Borràs-Comes, J., Tubau, S., and Espinal, M. T. (2013) Prosody and gesture constrain the interpretation of double negation. Lingua 131, 136–50. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.lingua.2013.02.008Google Scholar
Pruszynski, A. J. and Johansson, R. S. (2014). Edge-orientation processing in first-order tactile neurons. Nature Neuroscience, 17(10), 1404–9.Google Scholar
Pullum, G. (2012). A few notes on negative clauses, polarity items, and scope. www.lel.ed.ac.uk/~gpullum/grammar/negationGoogle Scholar
Quinto-Pozos, D. (2007). Why does constructed action seem obligatory? An analysis of classifiers and the lack of articulator-referent correspondence. Sign Language Studies, 7(4), 458506.Google Scholar
Quirk, R., Greenbaum, S., Leech, G., and Svartvick, J. (2000). Comprehensive grammar of the English language. Harlow: Longman.Google Scholar
Reddy, J. (1979). The conduit metaphor: A case of frame conflict in our language about language. In Ortony, A. (Ed.), Metaphor and thought (pp. 284310). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Sacks, H. and Schegloff, E. A. (2002). Home position. Gesture, 2, 133–46.Google Scholar
Schegloff, E. A. (1984). On some gestures’ relation to talk. In Atkinson, J. M. and Heritage, J. (Eds.), Structures of social action (pp. 266–98). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Schoonjans, S. (2018). Modalpartikeln als multimodale Konstruktionen: Eine korpusbasierte Kookkurrenzanalyse von Modalpartikeln und Gestik im Deutschen. Berlin and Boston: De Gruyter Mouton.Google Scholar
Schoonjans, S.(2017). Multimodal construction grammar issues are construction grammar issues. Linguistics Vanguard, 3(s1). https://doi.org/10.1515/lingvan-2016-0050CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schoonjans, S., Sambre, P., Brône, G. and Feyaerts, K. (2016). Vers une analyse multimodale du sens. Perspectives constructionnelles sur la gestualité co-grammaticale [Towards a multi-modal meaning analysis. Constructional perspectives on co-grammatical gesture]. Langages 201(1), 3349.Google Scholar
Shogan, D. (2002) Characterizing constraints of leisure: A Foucaultian analysis of leisure constraints. Leisure Studies, 21(1), 2738.Google Scholar
Silverstein, M. (1981). The limits of awareness. Sociolinguistic Working Paper Number 84.Google Scholar
Sime, D. (2008). ‘Because of her gesture, it’s very easy to understand’: Learner’s perceptions of teacher’s gestures in the foreign language class. In McCafferty, S. G. and Stam, G. (Eds.), Gesture in second language acquisition and classroom research (pp. 259279). London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Smotrova, T. (2014). Instructional functions of speech and gesture in the L2 classroom. Ph.D. diss., The Pennsylvania State University.Google Scholar
Steen, F. and Turner, M. (2013). Mutlimodal construction grammar. In Brokent, M., Dancygier, B., and Hinnell, J. (Eds.), Language and the creative mind (pp. 255–74). Stanford: CSLI.Google Scholar
Stivers, T. and Rossano, F. (2010). Mobilizing response. Research on Language & Social Interaction, 43(1), 331.Google Scholar
Stokoe, W. (1960). Sign language structure. An outline of the visual communication systems of the American Deaf. Studies in linguistics: Occasional papers (No. 8). Buffalo: Dept. of Anthropology and Linguistics, University of Buffalo.Google Scholar
Streeck, J. (2009). Gesturecraft. The manufacture of meaning. Amsterdam and Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Swales, J. (1990). Genre analysis. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Sweetser, E. (1990). From etymology to pragmatics: Metaphorical and cultural aspects of semantic structure. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Sweetser, E.(1998). Regular metaphoricity in gesture: Bodily-based models of speech interaction Actes du 16e Congrès International des Linguistes (CD-ROM).Amsterdam: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Sweetser, E.(2006). Negative spaces: Levels of negation and kinds of spaces. In Bonnefille, S. and Salbayre, S. (Eds.), Proceedings of the conference ‘Negation: Form, figure of speech, conceptualization’. Publication du groupe de recherches anglo-américaines de l’Université de Tours. Tours: Publications universitaires François Rabelais.Google Scholar
Sweetser, E.(2007). Looking at space to study mental spaces. Co-speech gesture as a crucial data source. In Gonzalez-Marquez, M., Mittleberg, I., Coulson, S., and Spivey, M. (Eds.), Methods in cognitive linguistics (pp. 203–26). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Sweetser, E. and Sizemore, M. (2008). Personal and interpersonal gesture space: Functional contrasts in language and gesture. In Tylet, A. (Ed.), Language in the context of use: Cognitive and discourse approaches to language and language learning (pp. 2551). Berlin and Boston: De Gruyter Mouton.Google Scholar
Talmy, L. (2000). Toward a cognitive semantics. Volume 1: Concept structuring systems. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Taub, S. (2001). Language from the body: Iconicity and metaphor in American sign language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Teßendorf, S. (2014). Pragmatic and metaphoric – combining functional with cognitive approaches in the analysis of the ‘brushing aside gesture’. In Cornelia, M., Cienki, A., Fricke, E., Ladewig, S., McNeill, D., and Bressem, J. (Eds.), Body – Language – Communication. An international handbook on multi-modality in human interactionHandbooks of Linguistics and Communication Science (pp. 1540–57). Berlin and Boston: De Gruyter Mouton.Google Scholar
Wehling, E. (2010). Argument is gesture war: Function, form and prosody of discourse structuring gestures in political argument. Paper presented at the 35th Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society, Berkeley, CA.Google Scholar
Williams, R. F. (2008). Gesture as a conceptual mapping tool. In Cienki, A. and Mueller, C. (Eds.), Metaphor and Gesture (pp. 5592). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Williams, R. F.(2011). Coordinating and sharing gesture space in collaborative reasoning. Paper presented in the theme session ‘Within and across spaces: Towards multi-dimensional models of gesture space’ at the 3rd Conference of the Scandinavian Association for Language and Cognition, University of Copenhagen, Denmark, 1416 June.Google Scholar
Williams, R. F.(2013). Distributed cognition and gesture. In Müller, C., Cienki, A., Fricke, E., Ladewig, S., McNeill, D., and Tessendorf, S. (Eds.), Body – Language – Communication: An International Handbook on Multimodality in Human Interaction (pp. 240258). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Williams, R. F. and Harrison, S. (2014). Distributed cognition and gesture: Propagating a functional system through impromptu teaching. In Polman, J. L., Kyza, E. A., O’Neil, D. K., Tabak, I., Penuel, W. R., Jurow, A. S., O’Connor, K., Lee, T., and D’Amico, L. (Eds.), Learning and becoming in practice: The International Conference of the Learning Sciences (ICLS), Vol. 3 (pp. 1655–6). Boulder, CO: International Society of the Learning Sciences.Google Scholar
Wilson, R. A. and Foglia, L. (2017). Embodied cognition. In Zalta, E. N. (Ed.), The Stanford encyclopedia of philosophy (Spring edition).Google Scholar
Xiao, R. and McEnery, T. (2008). Negation in Chinese: A corpus based study. Journal of Chinese Linguistics, 36(2), 333–67.Google Scholar
Yerian, K. (2016). The performative ‘ring’: Semi-conventionalized gesture as a resource for constructing social stance and identity. Presentation at ISGS 2016, Sorbonne Nouvelle Paris, 18–22 July.Google Scholar
Zeshan, U. (2004). Hand, head, and face: Negative constructions in sign languages. Linguistic Typology, 8, 158.Google Scholar
Zima, E. (2013). English multimodal motion constructions. A construction grammar perspective. Studies van de BKL 8, 1428.Google Scholar
Zima, E.(2014). Gibt es multimodale Konstruktionen? Eine Studie zu [V(motion) in circles] und [all the way from X PREP Y]. Gesprächsforschung – Online-Zeitschrift zur verbalen Interaktion, 15, 148. www.gespraechsforschung-ozs.de/fileadmin/dateien/heft2014/ga-zima.pdfGoogle Scholar
Zima, E.(2017). On the multimodality of [all the way from X PREP Y]. Linguistics Vanguard, 3(s1). https://doi.org/10.1515/lingvan-2016-0055Google Scholar
Zima, E and Bergs, A. (2017). Multimodality and Construction Grammar. Linguistics Vanguard, 3(s1). https://doi.org/10.1515/lingvan-2016-1006Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

  • Bibliography
  • Simon Harrison
  • Book: The Impulse to Gesture
  • Online publication: 07 August 2018
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108265065.010
Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

  • Bibliography
  • Simon Harrison
  • Book: The Impulse to Gesture
  • Online publication: 07 August 2018
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108265065.010
Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

  • Bibliography
  • Simon Harrison
  • Book: The Impulse to Gesture
  • Online publication: 07 August 2018
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108265065.010
Available formats
×