Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-p2v8j Total loading time: 0.001 Render date: 2024-05-14T19:39:49.125Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

33 - Specific Learning Disorders as a Working Memory Deficit

from Part VI - Language Disorders, Interventions, and Instruction

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 July 2022

John W. Schwieter
Affiliation:
Wilfrid Laurier University
Zhisheng (Edward) Wen
Affiliation:
Hong Kong Shue Yan University
Get access

Summary

Working memory (WM) deficits are fundamental problems of children with average intelligence but with specific learning disorders in reading and/or math. Depending on the task, these deficits manifest themselves as a domain-specific storage constraint (i.e., the inefficient accessing and availability of phonological representations, e.g., numbers, phonemes) and/or a domain-general monitoring constraint (limitations in controlled attentional processing, i.e., updating, inhibition). Recent studies suggest that growth in the executive component of WM is significantly related to such children’s growth in reading and/or math. Although constraints in WM can be modified, WM constraints in performance in children with reading disorders (RD) and/or math disorders (MD) remain when compared to their average achieving counterparts across a broad age span. Taken together, children with RD and/MD suffer fundamental problems related to the phonological loop (STM) and controlled attention (executive) component of WM.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2022

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Acheson, D. J., Hamidi, M., Binder, J. R., & Postle, B. R. (2011). A common neural substrate for language production and verbal working memory. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23 (6), 13581367. doi:10.1162/jocn.2010.21519Google Scholar
Ackerman, P. L., Beier, M., E., & Boyle, M. O. (2002). Individual differences in working memory within a nomological network of cognitive and perceptual speed abilities. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 131, 567589. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.131.4.567CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alloway, T. P., Gathercole, S. E., Willis, C., & Adams, A. (2004). A structural analysis of working memory and related cognitive skills in young children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 87, 85106. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2003.10.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed). American Psychiatric Association.Google Scholar
Attout, L., & Majerus, S. (2015). Working memory deficits in developmental dyscalculia: The importance of serial order. Child Neuropsychology, 21(4), 432450. doi:10.1080/09297049.2014.922170Google Scholar
Baddeley, A. D. (2012). Working memory: Theories, models, and controversies. Annual Review of Psychology, 63, 129. doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100422Google Scholar
Baddeley, A. D., Eldridge, M., Lewis, V., & Thomson, N. (1984). Attention and retrieval from long-term memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 113, 518540.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baddeley, A., Gathercole, S., & Papagno, C. (1998). The phonological loop as a language-learning device. Psychological Review, 105, 158173. doi:10.1037//0033-295X.105.1.158Google Scholar
Baddeley, A. D., & Logie, R. H. (1999). The multiple-component model. In Miyake, A. & Shah, P. (Eds.), Models of working memory: Mechanisms of active maintenance and executive control (pp. 2861). Cambridge University Press. doi:10.1017/CBO9781139174909.00CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beneventi, H., Tønnessen, F. E., & Ersland, L. (2009). Dyslexic children show short-term memory deficits in phonological storage and serial rehearsal: An fMRI study. International Journal of Neuroscience, 119(11), 20172043. doi:10.1080/00207450903139671Google Scholar
Beneventi, H., Tønnessen, F. E., Ersland, L., & Hugdahl, K. (2010). Executive working memory processes in dyslexia: Behavioral and fMRI evidence. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 51(3), 192202. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9450.2010.00808.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bledowski, C., Kaiser, J., & Rahm, B. (2010). Basic operations in working memory: Contributions from functional imaging studies. Behavioural Brain Research, 214(2), 172179. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2010.05.041Google Scholar
Brandenburg, J., Klesczewski, J., Fischbach, A., Schuchardt, K., Büttner, G., & Hasselhorn, M. (2015). Working memory in children with learning disabilities in reading versus spelling: Searching for overlapping and specific cognitive factors. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 48(6), 622634. doi:10.1177/0022219414521665CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chein, J. M., & Fiez, J. A. (2010). Evaluating models of working memory through the effects of concurrent irrelevant information. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 139(1), 117137. doi:10.1037/a0018200Google Scholar
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). L. Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Cowan, N. (1997). The development of working memory. In Cowan, N. (Ed.), The development of working memory in childhood (pp. 163199). Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Cowan, N. (2014). Working memory underpins cognitive development, learning, and education. Educational Psychology Review, 26(2), 197223.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Daneman, M., & Carpenter, P. A. (1980). Individual differences in working memory and reading. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 19(4), 450466. doi:10.1016/S0022-5371(80)90312-6Google Scholar
De Weerdt, F., Desoete, A., & Roeyers, H. (2013). Working memory in children with reading disabilities and/or mathematical disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 46(5), 461472.Google Scholar
Engle, R. W., Tuholski, S. W., Laughlin, J. E., & Conway, A. R. (1999). Working memory, short-term memory, and general fluid intelligence: A latent variable approach. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 128, 309331.Google Scholar
Evans, T. M., Flowers, D. L., Luetje, M. M., Napoliello, E., & Eden, G. F. (2016). Functional neuroanatomy of arithmetic and word reading and its relationship to age. NeuroImage, 143, 304315. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.08.048Google Scholar
Fürst, A. J., & Hitch, G. J. (2000). Separate roles for executive and phonological components of working memory in mental arithmeticMemory & Cognition28(5), 774782.Google Scholar
Gathercole, S. E., Pickering, S. J., Ambridge, B., & Wearing, H. (2004). The structure of working memory from 4 to 15 years of age. Developmental Psychology, 40, 177190.Google Scholar
Geary, D. C. (2011). Cognitive predictors of achievement growth in mathematics: A 5-year longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 47(6), 15391552.Google Scholar
Geary, D. C. (2013). Learning disabilities in mathematics: Recent advances. In Swanson, H. L., Harris, K., & Graham, S. (Eds.), Handbook of learning disabilities (2nd ed., pp. 239256). Guilford.Google Scholar
Geary, D. C., Hoard, M. K., Nugent, L., & Bailey, D. H. (2012). Mathematical cognition deficits in children with learning disabilities and persistent low achievement: A five-year prospective study. Journal of Educational Psychology, 104(1), 206223.Google Scholar
Gray, S., Green, S., Alt, M., Hogan, T., Kuo, T., Brinkley, S., & Cowan, N. (2017). The structure of working memory in young children and its relation to intelligence. Journal of Memory and Language, 92, 183201.Google Scholar
Hecht, S. A., Torgesen, J. K., Wagner, R. K., & Rashotte, C. A. (2001). The relations between phonological processing abilities and emerging individual differences in mathematical computational skills: A longitudinal study from second to fifth grades. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 79(2), 192227.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hutton, U. M. Z., & Towse, J. N. (2001). Short-term memory and working memory as indices of children’s cognitive skills. Memory, 9(4–6), 383394.Google Scholar
Kaufmann, L., Wood, G., Rubinsten, O., & Henik, A. (2011). Meta-analyses of developmental fMRI studies investigating typical and atypical trajectories of number processing and calculation. Developmental Neuropsychology, 36(6), 763787.Google Scholar
Kudo, M. F., Lussier, C. M., & Swanson, H. L. (2015). Reading disabilities in children: A selective meta-analysis of the cognitive literature. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 40, 5162.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kyllonen, P. C., & Christal, R. E. (1990). Reasoning ability is (little more than) working-memory capacity? Intelligence,14, 389433. doi:10.1016/S0160-2896(05)80012-1CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, E. S., Humphrey, M., Mellard, D. F., Woods, K., & Swanson, H. L. (2010). Cognitive processing deficits and students with specific learning disabilities: A selective meta-analysis of the literature. Learning Disability Quarterly, 33(1), 318.Google Scholar
Lanfranchi, S., & Swanson, H. L. (2005). Short-term memory and working memory in children as a function of language-specific knowledge in English and Spanish. Learning and Individual Differences, 15(4), 299319.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Locascio, G., Mahone, E. M., Eason, S. H., & Cutting, L. E. (2010). Executive dysfunction among children with reading comprehension deficitsJournal of Learning Disabilities43(5), 441454.Google Scholar
Logie, R. H., Gilhooly, K. J., & Wynn, V. (1994). Counting on working memory in arithmetic problem solving. Memory & Cognition, 22(4), 395410.Google Scholar
Maguire, M. J., Schneider, J. M., Middleton, A. E., Ralph, Y., Lopez, M., Ackerman, R. A., & Abel, A. D. (2018). Vocabulary knowledge mediates the link between socioeconomic status and word learning in grade schoolJournal of Experimental Child Psychology166, 679695.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mammarella, I. C., Caviola, S., Giofrè, D., & Szűcs, D. (2018). The underlying structure of visuospatial working memory in children with mathematical learning disability. British Journal of Developmental Psychology36(2), 220235.Google Scholar
McLean, J. F., & Hitch, G. J. (1999). Working memory impairments in children with specific arithmetic learning difficulties. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 74(3), 240260.Google Scholar
Melby-Lervåg, M., & Hulme, C. (2013). Is working memory training effective? A meta-analytic review. Developmental Psychology, 49(2), 270291.Google Scholar
Melby-Lervåg, M., Lyster, S. H., & Hulme, C. (2012). Phonological skills and their role in learning to read: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 138(2), 322352.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., & Howerter, A. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49100.Google Scholar
National Assessment of Educational Progress (2017). The condition of education (update 2017). US Department of Education.Google Scholar
National Center for Education Statistics (2020) Common Core of Data (CCD): Local education agency universe surveyU.S. Department of Education.Google Scholar
Nee, D. E., Brown, J. W., Askren, M. K., Berman, M. G., Demiralp, E., Krawitz, A., & Jonides, J. (2013). A meta-analysis of executive components of working memory. Cerebral Cortex, 23(2), 264282.Google Scholar
Passolunghi, M. C., & Siegel, L. S. (2004). Working memory and access to numerical information in children with disability in mathematics. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 88(4), 348367.Google Scholar
Peña, E. D., Bedore, L. M., & Kester, E. S. (2016). Assessment of language impairment in bilingual children using semantic tasks: Two languages classify better than one. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 51(2), 192202.Google Scholar
Peng, P., Barnes, M., Wang, C., Wang, W., Li, S., Swanson, H. L., & Tao, S. (2018). A meta-analysis on the relation between reading and working memory. Psychological Bulletin144(1), 4876.Google Scholar
Peng, P., Congying, S., Beilei, L., & Sha, T. (2012). Phonological storage and executive function deficits in children with mathematics difficulties. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 112(4), 452466.Google Scholar
Peng, P., & Fuchs, D. (2016). A meta-analysis of working memory deficits in children with learning difficulties: Is there a difference between verbal domain and numerical domain? Journal of Learning Disabilities, 49(1), 320. doi:10.1177/0022219414521667Google Scholar
Richlan, F. (2012). Developmental dyslexia: Dysfunction of a left hemisphere reading network. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6 doi:10.3389/fnhum.2012.00120CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Richlan, F., Kronbichler, M., & Wimmer, H. (2011). Meta-analyzing brain dysfunctions in dyslexic children and adults. NeuroImage, 56(3), 17351742. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.02.040Google Scholar
Rosselli, M., Ardila, A., Lalwani, L. N., & Vélez-Uribe, I. (2016). The effect of language proficiency on executive functions in balanced and unbalanced Spanish–English bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 19(3), 489503.Google Scholar
Rosenberg-Lee, M., Lovett, M. C., & Anderson, J. R. (2009). Neural correlates of arithmetic calculation strategies. Cognitive, Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, 9(3), 270285.Google Scholar
Scarborough, H. S. (1998). Early identification of children at risk for reading disabilities: Phonological awareness and some other promising predictors. In Shapiro, B, Accardo, P., & Capute, A (Eds.). Specific reading disability: A view of the spectrum (pp. 75119). York Press.Google Scholar
Schwaighofer, M., Fischer, F., Bühner, M. (2015). Does working memory training transfer? A meta-analysis including training conditions as moderators. Educational Psychologist, 50(2), 138166Google Scholar
Shipstead, Z., Redick, T. S., and Engle, R. W. (2010). Does working memory training generalize? Psychologica Belgica, 50(3&4), 245276Google Scholar
Siegel, L. S., & Ryan, E. B. (1989). The development of working memory in normally achieving and subtypes of learning disabled children. Child Development, 60(4), 973980.Google Scholar
Smith, E. E., & Jonides, J. (1997). Working memory: A view from neuroimaging. Cognitive Psychology, 33(1), 542.Google Scholar
Smith, E. E., & Jonides, J. (1999). Storage and executive processes in the frontal lobes. Science, 283(5408), 16571661.Google Scholar
Swanson, H. (1981). Vigilance deficit in learning disabled children: A signal detection analysis. Child Psychology & Psychiatry & Allied Disciplines, 22(4), 393399.Google Scholar
Swanson, H. (1983). A developmental study of vigilance in learning-disabled and nondisabled children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 11(3), 415429.Google Scholar
Swanson, H. (1988). Learning disabled children’s problem solving: Identifying mental processes underlying intelligent performance. Intelligence, 12(3), 261278.Google Scholar
Swanson, H. (1989). The effects of central processing strategies on learning disabled, mildly retarded, average, and gifted children’s elaborative encoding abilities. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 47(3), 370397.Google Scholar
Swanson, H. (1992). Generality and modifiability of working memory among skilled and less skilled readers. Journal of Educational Psychology, 84(4), 473-488.Google Scholar
Swanson, H. (1993a). An information processing analysis of learning-disabled children’s problem solving. American Educational Research Journal, 30(4), 861893.Google Scholar
Swanson, H. (1993b). Executive processing in learning-disabled readers. Intelligence, 17(2), 117149.Google Scholar
Swanson, H. (1993c). Working memory in learning disability subgroups. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 56(1), 87114.Google Scholar
Swanson, H. (1995). S-Cognitive Processing Test (S-CPT): A dynamic assessment measure. PRO-ED.Google Scholar
Swanson, H. (1996). Individual and age-related differences in children’s working memory. Memory & Cognition, 24, 7082.Google Scholar
Swanson, H. (1999). What develops in working memory? A life span perspective. Developmental Psychology, 35(4), 9861000.Google Scholar
Swanson, H. (2000). Are working memory deficits in readers with learning disabilities hard to change? Journal of Learning Disabilities, 33(6), 551566.Google Scholar
Swanson, H. (2003). Age-related differences in learning disabled and skilled readers’ working memory. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 85(1), 131.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swanson, H. (2004). Working memory and phonological processing as predictors of children’s mathematical problem solving at different ages. Memory & Cognition, 32(4), 648661.Google Scholar
Swanson, H. (2008). Working memory and intelligence in children: What develops? Journal of Educational Psychology, 100(3), 581602.Google Scholar
Swanson, H. (2011). The influence of working memory growth on reading and math performance in children with math and/or reading disabilities. In Barrouillet, P & Gaillard, V (Eds.), Cognitive development and working memory: A dialogue between neo-Piagetian theories and cognitive approaches (pp. 203231). Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Swanson, H. (2012). Intellectual growth in children as a function of domain specific and domain general working memory subgroups. Intelligence, 39, 481492.Google Scholar
Swanson, H. (2014). Does cognitive strategy training on word problems compensate for working memory capacity in children with math difficulties? Journal of Educational Psychology, 106(3), 831848.Google Scholar
Swanson, H. (2016). Word problem solving, working memory and serious math difficulties: Do cognitive strategies really make a difference? Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 5(4), 368383.Google Scholar
Swanson, H. (2017). Verbal and visual-spatial working memory: What develops over a life span? Developmental Psychology, 53(5), 971995.Google Scholar
Swanson, H (2020). Learning disabilities as a working memory deficit: A revised model. In Sperling, R., Martin, A., & Newton, K, Handbook of educational psychology & students with special needs (pp. 1951). Routledge.Google Scholar
Swanson, H., & Alexander, J. E. (1997). Cognitive processes as predictors of word recognition and reading comprehension in learning-disabled and skilled readers: Revisiting the specificity hypothesis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89(1), 128158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Swanson, H., & Alloway, T. P. (2012). Working memory, learning, and academic achievement. APA educational psychology handbook. Vol 1: Theories, constructs, and critical issues (pp. 327366). American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Swanson, H., & Ashbaker, M. H. (2000). Working memory, short-term memory, speech rate, word recognition and reading comprehension in learning disabled readers: Does the executive system have a role? Intelligence, 28(1), 130.Google Scholar
Swanson, H., Ashbaker, M. H., & Lee, C. (1996). Learning-disabled readers’ working memory as a function of processing demands. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 61(3), 242275.Google Scholar
Swanson, H., & Beebe-Frankenberger, M. (2004). The relationship between working memory and mathematical problem solving in children at risk and not a risk for serious math difficulties. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96, 471491.Google Scholar
Swanson, H., & Berninger, V. (1995). The role of working memory in skilled and less skilled readers’ comprehension. Intelligence, 21(1), 83108.Google Scholar
Swanson, H., & Fung, W. (2016). Working memory components and problem-solving accuracy: Are there multiple pathways? Journal of Educational Psychology, 108(8), 11531177.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Swanson, H., Howard, C. B., & Sáez, L. (2006). Do different components of working memory underlie different subgroups of reading disabilities? Journal of Learning Disabilities, 39(3), 252269.Google Scholar
Swanson, H., & Jerman, O. (2006). Math disabilities: A selective meta-analysis of the literature. Review of Educational Research, 76(2), 249274.Google Scholar
Swanson, H., & Jerman, O. (2007). The influence of working memory on reading growth in subgroups of children with reading disabilities. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 96(4), 249283.Google Scholar
Swanson, H., Jerman, O., & Zheng, X. (2008). Growth in working memory and mathematical problem solving in children at risk and not at risk for serious math difficulties. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100(2), 343379.Google Scholar
Swanson, H., Jerman, O., & Zheng, X. (2009). Math disabilities and reading disabilities: Can they be separated? Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 27 (3), 175196.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Swanson, H., Kehler, P., & Jerman, O. (2010). Working memory, strategy knowledge, and strategy instruction in children with reading disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 43(1), 2447.Google Scholar
Swanson, L., & Kim, K. (2007). Working memory, short-term memory, and naming speed as predictors of children’s mathematical performance. Intelligence, 35(2), 151168.Google Scholar
Swanson, H., Kong, J., & Petcu, S. (2018). Math difficulties and working memory growth in English language learner children: Does bilingual proficiency play a significant role? Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools49(3), 379394.Google Scholar
Swanson, H., Kong, J. E., & Petcu, S. D. (2019a). Individual differences in math problem solving and executive processing among emerging bilingual childrenJournal of Experimental Child Psychology187, 25.Google Scholar
Swanson, H., Kong, J., & Petcu, S. D. (2019b) Growth in math computation among monolingual and English language learners: Does the executive system have a role? Developmental Neuropsychology44(8), 566593.Google Scholar
Swanson, H., Kong, J, & Petcu, S. (2020) Emerging bilingual children at risk and not at risk for math difficultiesChild Neuropsychology26(4), 489517.Google Scholar
Swanson, H., Kudo, M., & Guzman-Orth, D. (2016). Cognition and literacy in English language learners at risk for reading disabilities: A latent transition analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 108(6), 830856.Google Scholar
Swanson, H., Kudo, M. F., & Van Horn, M. L. (2019c). Does the structure of working memory in EL children vary across age and two language systems? Memory27(2), 174191.Google Scholar
Swanson, H., Lussier, C. M., & Orosco, M. J. (2015). Cognitive strategies, working memory, and growth in word problem solving in children with math difficulties. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 48(4), 339358.Google Scholar
Swanson, H., Lussier, C. M., & Orosco, M. J. (2015). Cognitive strategies, working memory, and growth in word problem solving in children with math difficulties. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 48(4), 339358.Google Scholar
Swanson, H., & McMurran, M. (2018). The impact of working memory training on near and far transfer measures: Is it all about fluid intelligence? Child Neuropsychology24(3), 370395.Google Scholar
Swanson, H., Moran, A. S., Bocian, K., Lussier, C., & Zheng, X. (2013). Generative strategies, working memory, and word problem solving accuracy in children at risk for math disabilities. Learning Disability Quarterly, 36(4), 203214.Google Scholar
Swanson, H., Moran, A., Lussier, C., & Fung, W. (2014). The effect of explicit and direct generative strategy training and working memory on word problem-solving accuracy in children at risk for math difficulties. Learning Disability Quarterly, 37(2), 111122.Google Scholar
Swanson, H., Olide, A. F., & Kong, J. E. (2018). Latent class analysis of children with math difficulties and/or math learning disabilities: Are there cognitive differences? Journal of Educational Psychology, 110(7), 931951.Google Scholar
Swanson, H., Orosco, M. J., & Kudo, M. (2017). Does growth in the executive system of working memory underlie growth in literacy for bilingual children with and without reading disabilities? Journal of Learning Disabilities, 50(4), 386407.Google Scholar
Swanson, H., Orosco, M. J., & Lussier, C. M. (2012). Cognition and literacy in English language learners at risk for reading disabilities. Journal of Educational Psychology, 104(2), 302320.Google Scholar
Swanson, H., Orosco, M. J., & Lussier, C. M. (2015). Growth in literacy, cognition, and working memory in English language learners. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 132, 155188.Google Scholar
Swanson, H., Orosco, M. J., Lussier, C. M., Gerber, M. M., & Guzman-Orth, D. (2011). The influence of working memory and phonological processing on English language learner children’s bilingual reading and language acquisition. Journal of Educational Psychology, 103(4), 838856.Google Scholar
Swanson, H., & Sachse-Lee, C. (2001a). A subgroup analysis of working memory in children with reading disabilities: Domain-general or domain-specific deficiency? Journal of Learning Disabilities, 34(3), 249263.Google Scholar
Swanson, H., & Sachse-Lee, C. (2001b). Mathematical problem solving and working memory in children with learning disabilities: Both executive and phonological processes are important. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 79(3), 294321.Google Scholar
Swanson, H., Sáez, L., Gerber, M., & Leafstedt, J. (2004). Literacy and cognitive functioning in bilingual and nonbilingual children at or not at risk for reading disabilitiesJournal of Educational Psychology96(1), 318.Google Scholar
Swanson, H., & Siegel, L. (2001a). Elaborating on working memory and learning disabilities: A reply to commentators. Issues in Education: Contributions from Educational Psychology, 7, 107129.Google Scholar
Swanson, H., & Siegel, L. (2001b). Learning disabilities as a working memory deficit. Issues in Education: Contributions from Educational Psychology, 7, 148.Google Scholar
Swanson, H., & Zheng, X. (2013). Memory difficulties in children and adults with learning disabilities. In Swanson, H. L., Harris, K., & Graham, S. (Eds), Handbook of learning disabilities (2nd ed, pp. 214238). Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Swanson, H., Zheng, X., & Jerman, O. (2009). Working memory, short-term memory, and reading disabilities: A selective meta-analysis of the literature. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 42(3), 260287.Google Scholar
Uppal, H. K., & Swanson, H. L. (2016). Teachers’ ratings of working memory in English language learners: Do laboratory measures predict classroom analogues? Applied Cognitive Psychology, 30(6), 871884.Google Scholar
Unsworth, N., & Engle, R. W. (2007). On the division of short-term and working memory: An examination of simple and complex span and their relation to higher order abilities. Psychological Bulletin, 133(6), 10381066.Google Scholar
Wang, S., & Gathercole, S. E. (2013). Working memory deficits in children with reading difficulties: Memory span and dual task coordination. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 115(1), 188197.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×