Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-ttngx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-01T17:31:07.573Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

21 - The Cultural Basis of Language and Thought in Development

from Part III - Language and Communicative Development

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 May 2017

Nancy Budwig
Affiliation:
Clark University, Massachusetts
Elliot Turiel
Affiliation:
University of California, Berkeley
Philip David Zelazo
Affiliation:
University of Minnesota
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2017

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Ambridge, B. & Lieven, E. V. M. (2011). Child language acquisition: Contrasting theoretical approaches. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Astington, J. W. & Baird, J., (2005). Why language matters to theory of mind. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bauer, P. J. (2007). Remembering the times of our lives: Memory in infancy and beyond. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bauer, P. J., & Fivush, R. (2014). (Eds.). The Wiley handbook on the development of children’s memory. New York, NY: Wiley Blackwell.Google Scholar
Bauer, P. J., & Mandler, J. M. (1990). Remembering what happened next: Very young children’s recall of event sequences. In Fivush, R. & Hudson, J. A. (Eds.), Knowing and remembering in young children. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Bloom, L. (1973). One word at a time. The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
Bloom, P. (2000). How children learn the meaning of words. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Blum-Kulka, S., & Snow, C. E. (2002). Talking to adults: The contribution of multiparty discourse to language acquisition. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bogdan, R. J. (2003). Interpreting minds. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Bogdan, R. J. (2009). Predicative minds: The social ontogeny of propositional thinking. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Bogdan, R. J. (2010). Our own minds. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Bogdan, R. J. (2013). Mindvaults. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Bruner, J. S. (1986). Actual minds possible worlds. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Campos, J. J., Anderson, D. I., Barbu-Roth, M. A., Hubbard, E. M., Hertenstein, M. J., & Witherington, D. (2000). Travel broadens the mind. Infancy, 1, 149220.Google Scholar
Cekaite, A., Blum-Kulka, S., Grover, V., & Teubal, E. (2014). (Eds.). Children’s peer talk: Learning from each other. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Deacon, T. W. (1997). The symbolic species: Coevolution of language and the brain. New York, NY: Norton.Google Scholar
Donald, M. (1991). Origins of the modern mind. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Donald, M. (2001). A mind so rare: The evolution of human consciousness. New York, NY: Norton.Google Scholar
Donald, M. (in press). The evolutionary origins of human cultural memory. In Wagoner, B. (Ed.). Oxford handbook of culture and memory. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Fenson, L., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., Bates, E., Thal, D. J., & Pethick, S. J. (1994). Variability in early communicative development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development. 59 (5).Google Scholar
Fivush, R. (2008). Remembering and reminiscing: how individual lives are constructed in family narratives. Memory Studies, 1, 4554.Google Scholar
Fivush, R. (2011). The development of autobiographical memory. Annual Review of Psychology, 62, 559582.Google Scholar
Fodor, J. A. (1975). The language of thought. New York, NY: Crowell.Google Scholar
Gallaway, C., & Richards, B. J. (1994). (Eds.). Input and interaction in language acquisition. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Golinkoff, R. M., Hirsh-Pasekl, K., Bloom, L., Smith, L. B., Woodward, A. L., Akhtar, N., Tomasello, M., & Hollich, G. (2000). (Eds.). Becoming a word learner: A debate on lexical acquisition. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Halliday, M. A. K. (1975). Learning how to mean. London: Edwin Arnold.Google Scholar
Hampson, J., & Nelson, K. (1993). The relation of maternal language to variation in rate and style of language acquisition. Journal of Child Language, 20, 313342.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harris, P. L. (2012). Trusting what you’re told: How children learn from others. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Hart, B. & Risley, T. (1995). Meaningful differences in the everyday lives of young American children. Baltimore: Brookes.Google Scholar
Hobson, P. (2002). The cradle of thought: Exploring the origins of thinking. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hutto, D. (2010). Folk psychological narratives: The socio-cultural basis of understanding reasons. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Jablonka, E., & Lamb, M. J. (2005). Evolution in four dimensions: Genetic, epigenetic, behavioral, and symbolic variation in the history of life. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Jusczyk, P. W. (2000). The discovery of spoken language. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Kemler Nelson, D. G. (1999). Attention to functional properties in toddlers’ naming and problem-solving. Cognitive Development, 14, 77100.Google Scholar
Levy, E. T., & McNeill, D. (2015). Narrative development of young children: Gesture, imagery, and cohesion. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Macnamara, J. (1982). Names for things. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Mandler, J. M. (2004). The foundations of mind: Origins of conceptual thought. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Miller, P. J., Potts, R., Fung, H., Hoogstra, L., & Mintz, J. (1990). Narrative practices and the social construction of self in childhood. American Ethnologist, 17, 292311.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Millikan, R. G. (2004). Varieties of meaning: The 2002 Nicod Lectures. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. (1973). Structure and strategy in learning to talk. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 38 (12, Serial No. 149).Google Scholar
Nelson, K. (1974). Concept, word, and sentence: Interrelations in acquisition and development. Psychological Review, 81, 267285.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. (1978). How young children represent knowledge of their world in and out of language. In Siegler, R. S. (Ed.), Children’s thinking: What develops? Hillsdale, NJ: L. Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. (1985). Making sense: The acquisition of shared meaning. New York, NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. (1986). Event knowledge: Structure and function in development. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Assoc.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. (Ed.). (1989). Narratives from the crib. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. (1993). The psychological and social origins of autobiographical memory. Psychological Science, 4, 18.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. (1996). Language in cognitive development: The emergence of the mediated mind. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. (2005). Language pathways to the community of minds. In Astington, J. W. & Baird, J. (Eds.), Why language matters to theory of mind. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. (2007). Young minds in social worlds: Experience, meaning and memory. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. (2014a). A matter of meaning: reflections on forty years of JCL. Journal of Child Language, 14 (Supplement S1), 93104.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. (2014b). Sociocultural theories of memory development. In Bauer, P. J. & Fivush, R. (Eds.). The Wiley handbook on the development of children’s memory. New York, NY: Wiley BlackwellGoogle Scholar
Nelson, K. (2015). Making sense with private speech. Cognitive Development, 36 · October–December 2015, 171179. doi: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2015.09.004.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, K., & Fivush, R. (2004). The emergence of autobiographical memory: A social cultural developmental theory. Psychological Review, 1119, 486511.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, K., & Gruendel, J. (1981). Generalized event representations: Basic building blocks of cognitive development. In Lamb, M. & Brown, A. (Eds.), Advances in developmental psychology, Vol. 1. Hillsdale, NJ: L. Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Nelson, K., Hampson, J., & Kessler-Shaw, L. (1993). The noun bias in early lexicons: Evidence, explanations, and implications. Journal of Child Language, 20, 6184.Google Scholar
Roberts, K. P., & Blades, M. (2000). (Eds.). Children’s source monitoring. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum Assoc.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sawyer, R. K. (1997). Pretend play as improvisation: Conversation in the preschool classroom. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Schaller, S. (1991). A man without words. New York, NY: Summit Books.Google Scholar
Schank, R. C., & Abelson, R. P. (1977). Scripts, plans, goals and understanding. Hillsdale NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Shipley, T. F., & Zacks, J. M. (2008). (Eds.). Understanding events: From perception to action. New York, NY: Oxford.Google Scholar
Snow, C. (2014). Input to interaction to instruction: Three key shifts in the history of child language research. Journal of Child Language, 14 (Supplement S1), 117123.Google Scholar
Suddendorf, T., & Moore, C. (2011). Introduction to special issue: The development of episodic foresight. Cognitive Development, 26, 295298.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M. (1995). Joint attention as social cognition. In Moore, C. & Dunham, S. (Eds.) Joint attention: Its origin and role in development. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum (pp. 103130).Google Scholar
Tulving, E. (1983). Elements of episodic memory. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Tulving, E. (1993). What is episodic memory? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 2 (3), 6770.Google Scholar
Tulving, E. (2005). Episodic memory and autonoesis: Uniquely human? In Terrace, H. S. & Metcalfe, J. (Eds.) The missing link in Cognition: Origins of self-reflective consciousness. New York, NY: Oxford University Press (pp. 354).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vygotsky, L. (1962). Thought and language (Hanfmann, E. & Vakar, G., Trans.). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Wang, Qi. (2014). The cultured self and remembering. In Bauer, P. J. & Fivush, R. (Eds.) The Wiley handbook on the development of children’s memory. New York, NY: Wiley Blackwell (pp. 605625).Google Scholar
Werner, H., & Kaplan, B. (1963). Symbol formation. New York, NY: Wiley.Google Scholar
Winsler, A. (2009). Still talking to ourselves after all these years: A review of current research on private speech. In Winsler, A., Fernyhough, C., & Montero, I., (Eds.). Private speech, executive functioning, and the development of verbal self-regulation. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press (pp. 341).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wittgenstein, L. (1953). Philosophical investigations. New York, NY: Macmillan.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×