Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-qxdb6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-29T13:27:16.329Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

16 - Differences between individuals with and without autism in copying tasks: how knowledge interferes when drawing perspective

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  22 September 2009

Elizabeth Sheppard
Affiliation:
University of Nottingham
Peter Mitchell
Affiliation:
University of Nottingham
Danielle Ropar
Affiliation:
University of Nottingham
Chris Lange-Küttner
Affiliation:
London Metropolitan University
Annie Vinter
Affiliation:
Université de Bourgogne, France
Get access

Summary

Sheppard, Mitchell and Ropar investigate whether individuals with autism are less likely than control participants to use object-specific drawing schemata which do not depict the model they actually see. Self-taught savant skills are much more frequent in autistic populations, and it is assumed that these may be responsible for a less conceptual approach which allows lower-level perceptual processing to prevail. However, because the empirical results from previous research did not prove to be conclusive, Sheppard, Mitchell and Ropar in several studies used tasks where in one condition a purely geometrical form was presented, while in the other condition features were added to convey the impression of a more meaningful object from everyday life. In children and adults without autism, meaningfulness could lower or enhance the level of drawing, depending on the kind of object, while spatial complexity appeared always to make drawing more difficult. Individuals with autism, however, were better able either to overcome or to ignore conceptual object knowledge, in particular when recreating a line drawing. It is concluded that this result reflects a special ability for literal understanding in autism.

a great deal of research on drawing has focused on the effects of conceptual knowledge on performance. Generally, the bulk of findings suggest that prior knowledge about the subject matter interferes with accurate depiction. It seems that participants are liable to rely upon this instead of the viewpoint-specific information projected onto the retina. In other words, people sometimes draw what they know rather than what they see.

Type
Chapter
Information
Drawing and the Non-Verbal Mind
A Life-Span Perspective
, pp. 325 - 343
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2008

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Baron-Cohen, S. (1987). Autism and symbolic play. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 5, 139–48.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S. (1989). Are autistic children behaviorists? An examination of their mental–physical and appearance–reality distinctions. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 19, 579–600.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M. and Frith, U. (1986). Mechanical, behavioral and intentional understanding of picture stories in autistic children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 4, 113–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bremner, J. G. (1984). Errors towards the perpendicular in children's copies of angular figures: a test of the bisection interpretation. Perception, 13, 117–28.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Charman, T. and Baron-Cohen, S. (1993). Drawing development in autism: the intellectual to visual realism shift. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 11, 171–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chen, M. J. (1985). Young children's representational drawings of solid objects: a comparison of drawing and copying. In Freeman, N. H. and Cox, M. V. (eds.), Visual order: the nature and development of pictorial representation (pp. 157–75). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Cohen, D. J. and Bennett, S. (1997). Why can't most people draw what they see?Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 23, 609–21.Google ScholarPubMed
Eames, K. and Cox, M. V. (1994). Visual realism in the drawings of autistic, Down's syndrome and normal children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 12, 235–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Freeman, N. and Janikoun, R. (1972). Intellectual realism in children's drawings of a familiar object with distinctive features. Child Development, 43, 1116–21.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frith, U. (2003). Autism: explaining the enigma. Second edition. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Gombrich, E. H. (1984). Art and illusion. (Bollingen Series 35.)Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Hill, A. L. (1977). Idiot savants: rate of incidence. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 44, 161–2.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kubovy, M. (1986). The psychology of perspective and Renaissance art. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Lee, M. (1989). When is an object not an object? The effect of ‘meaning’ upon the copying of line drawings. British Journal of Psychology, 80, 15–37.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leslie, A. M. (1987). Pretense and representation: the origins of ‘Theory of Mind’. Psychological Review, 94, 412–26.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luquet, G. H. (2001). Children's drawings. Trans. Alan Costall. London: Free Association Books.Google Scholar
Mitchell, P. and Isaacs, J. E. (1994). Understanding of verbal representation in children with autism: the case of referential opacity. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 12, 439–54.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mitchell, P., Ropar, D., Ackroyd, K. and Rajendran, G. (2005). How perception impacts on drawings. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31, 996–1003.Google ScholarPubMed
Moore, V. (1987). The influence of experience on children's drawings of a familiar and unfamiliar object. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 5, 221–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mottron, L., Belleville, S. and Ménard, E. (1999). Local bias in autistic subjects as evidenced by graphic tasks: perceptual hierarchization or working memory deficit?Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 40, 743–55.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Phillips, W. A., Hobbs, S. B. and Pratt, F. R. (1978). Intellectual realism in children's drawings of cubes. Cognition, 6, 15–33.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rimland, B. (1978). Savant capabilities of autistic children and their cognitive implications. In Serban, G. (ed.), Cognitive defects in the development of mental illness (pp. 43–65). Oxford: Brunner/Mazel.Google Scholar
Ropar, D. and Mitchell, P. (2002). Shape constancy in autism: the role of prior knowledge and perspective cues. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 43, 647–53.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sheppard, E., Ropar, D. and Mitchell, P. (2005). The impact of meaning and dimensionality on the accuracy of children's copying. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 23, 365–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sheppard, E., Ropar, D. and Mitchell, P. (2007). The impact of meaning and dimensionality on copying accuracy in individuals with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 37: 1913–24.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sheppard, E., Ropar, D. and Mitchell, P. (in prep.). Autism and dimensionality: differences between drawing and copying tasks.
Snyder, A. W. and Thomas, M. (1997). Autistic artists give clues to cognition. Perception, 26, 93–6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thouless, R. H. (1931). Phenomenal regression to the real object. Ⅱ. British Journal of Psychology, 22, 1–30.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×