Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-4hhp2 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-06-02T02:56:33.778Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

8 - True or false: ‘I'd know a false confession if I saw one’

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  22 September 2009

Saul Kassin
Affiliation:
Williams College, USA
Pär Anders Granhag
Affiliation:
Göteborgs Universitet, Sweden
Leif A. Strömwall
Affiliation:
Göteborgs Universitet, Sweden
Get access

Summary

Let me begin with a recent story that already has historic value in the annals of wrongful convictions. This was an infamous case that took place in 1989 in New York City. Known as the ‘Central Park jogger case’, it involved a young woman, an investment banker, who was beaten senseless, raped, and left for dead. It was a heinous crime that horrified the city. The victim's skull had multiple fractures, her eye socket was crushed, and she lost three-quarters of her blood. Defying the odds, she managed to survive, but she was – and, to this day, still is – completely amnesic for the incident (Meili, 2003). Soon thereafter, solely on the basis of police-induced confessions taken within seventy-two hours of the crime, five African- and Hispanic-American boys, 14 to 16 years old, were convicted of the attack and sentenced to prison. At the time, it was easy to understand why detectives aggressively interrogated the boys, some of whom were ‘wilding’ in the park that night (Sullivan, 1992). Yet based on a recent confession volunteered from prison by a convicted serial rapist whose DNA was found in semen at the crime scene, the five boys' convictions were vacated, their confessions apparently false (Kassin, 2002; New York v. Wise, 2002; Saulny, 2002).

Four of the five jogger confessions were videotaped and presented to the juries at trial. The tapes – which showed only the confessions, not the precipitating fourteen to thirty hours of interrogation – were highly compelling.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2004

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Akehurst, L., and Vrij, A. (1999). Creating suspects in police interviews. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 29, 192–210CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anderson, D. E., DePaulo, B. M., Ansfield, M. E., Tickle, J. J., and Green, E. (1999). Beliefs about cues to deception: Mindless stereotypes or untapped wisdom?Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 23, 67–89CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aubry, A. S., and Caputo, R. R. (1980). Criminal interrogation (3rd edn). Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas
Bedau, H. A., and Radelet, M. L. (1987). Miscarriages of justice in potentially capital cases. Stanford Law Review, 40, 21–179CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blagrove, M. (1996). Effects of length of sleep deprivation on interrogative suggestibility. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 2, 48–59Google Scholar
Bull, R. (1989). Can training enhance the detection of deception? In J. C. Yuille (ed.), Credibility assessment (pp. 83–99). London: Kluwer AcademicCrossRef
Buller, D. B., Strzyzewski, K. D., and Hunsaker, F. G. (1991). Interpersonal deception: II. The inferiority of conversational participants as deception detectors. Communication Monographs, 58, 25–40CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cassell, P. G. (1999). The guilty and the ‘innocent’: An examination of alleged cases of wrongful conviction from false confessions. Harvard Journal of Law and Public Policy, 22, 523Google Scholar
Darley, J. M., and Fazio, R. H. (1980). Expectancy confirmation processes arising in the social interaction sequence. American Psychologist, 35, 867–81CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davey, M. (2003). Illinois will require taping of homicide interrogations. New York Times, July 17, Section A, p. 16Google Scholar
Davis, D., and O'Donohue, W. (2003). The road to perdition: ‘Extreme influence’ tactics in the interrogation room. In W. O'Donohue, P. Laws, and C. Hollin (eds.), Handbook of Forensic Psychology (pp. 897–996). New York: Basic Books
DePaulo, B. M. (1994). Spotting lies: Can humans learn to do better?Current Directions in Psychological Science, 3, 83–6CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DePaulo, B. M., Lassiter, G. D., and Stone, J. I. (1982). Attentional determinants of success at detecting deception and truth. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 8, 273–9CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DePaulo, B. M., Lindsay, J. J., Malone, B. E., Muhlenbruck, L., Charlton, K., and Cooper, H. (2003). Cues to deception. Psychological Bulletin, 129, 74–112CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DePaulo, B. M., and Pfeifer, R. L. (1986). On-the-job experience and skill at detecting deception. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 16, 249–67CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Drizin, S. A., and Colgan, B. A. (2001). Let the cameras roll: Mandatory videotaping of interrogations is the solution to Illinois' problem of false confessions. Loyola University Chicago Law Journal, 32, 337–424Google Scholar
Ekman, P., and O'Sullivan, M. (1991). Who can catch a liar?American Psychologist, 46, 913–20CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ekman, P., O'Sullivan, M., and Frank, M. G. (1999). A few can catch a liar. Psychological Science, 10, 263–6CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garrido, E., and Masip, J. (1999). How good are police officers at spotting lies?Forensic Update, 58, 14–21Google Scholar
Garrido, E., Masip, J., and Herrero, C. (in press). Police officers' credibility judgements: Accuracy and estimated ability. International Journal of Psychology
Geller, W. A. (1993). Videotaping interrogations and confessions. National Institute of Justice: Research in Brief. Washington, DC: US Department of Justice
Gilbert, D. T., and Malone, P. S. (1995). The correspondence bias. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 21–38CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gudjonsson, G. H. (1992). The psychology of interrogations, confessions, and testimony. London: Wiley
Gudjonsson, G. H. (2003). The psychology of interrogations and confessions: A handbook. West Sussex, England: John Wiley
Harrison, Y., and Horne, J. A. (2000). The impact of sleep deprivation on decision making: A review. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 6, 236–49Google ScholarPubMed
Hartwig, M., Granhag, P. A., Strömwall, L. A., and Vrij, A. (in press). Police officers' lie detection accuracy: Interrogating freely vs. observing video. Police Quarterly
Hilgendorf, E. L., and Irving, M. (1981). A decision-making model of confessions. In M. Lloyd-Bostock (ed.), Psychology in legal contexts: Applications and limitations (pp. 67–84). London: MacmillanCrossRef
Horselenberg, R., Merckelbach, H., and Josephs, S. (2003). Individual differences and false confessions: A conceptual replication of Kassin and Kiechel (1996). Psychology, Crime, and Law, 9, 1–18CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Inbau, F. E., Reid, J. E., Buckley, J. P., and Jayne, B. C. (2001). Criminal interrogation and confessions (4th edn). Gaithersberg, MD: Aspen
Jones, E. E. (1990). Interpersonal perception. New York: Freeman
Kassin, S. M. (1997). The psychology of confession evidence. American Psychologist, 52, 221–33CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kassin, S. M. (2002). False confessions and the jogger case, New York Times, November 1, p. A-31Google Scholar
Kassin, S. M. (2004). Videotape police interrogations. Boston Globe, April 26, p. A-13
Kassin, S. M., and Fong, C. T. (1999). ‘I'm innocent!’ Effects of training on judgements of truth and deception in the interrogation room. Law and Human Behavior, 23, 499–516CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kassin, S. M., Goldstein, C. J., and Savitsky, K. (2003). Behavioral confirmation in the interrogation room: On the dangers of presuming guilt. Law and Human Behavior, 27, 187–203CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kassin, S. M., and Kiechel, K. L. (1996). The social psychology of false confessions: Compliance, internalization, and confabulation. Psychological Science, 7, 125–8CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kassin, S., Leo, R., Crocker, C., and Holland, L. (2003). Videotaping interrogations: Does it enhance the jury's ability to distinguish true and false confessions? Paper presented at the Psychology & Law International, Interdisciplinary Conference, Edinburgh, Scotland
Kassin, S. M., and McNall, K. (1991). Police interrogations and confessions: Communicating promises and threats by pragmatic implication. Law and Human Behavior, 15, 233–51CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kassin, S. M., Meissner, C. A., and Norwick, R. J. (2004). ‘I'd know a false confession if I saw one’: A comparative study of college students and police investigators. Unpublished manuscript
Kassin, S. M., and Neumann, K. (1997). On the power of confession evidence: An experimental test of the ‘fundamental difference’ hypothesis. Law and Human Behavior, 21, 469–84CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kassin, S. M., and Norwick, R. J. (2004). Why suspects waive their Miranda rights: The power of innocence. Law and Human Behavior, 28, 211–21CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kassin, S. M., and Sukel, H. (1997). Coerced confessions and the jury: An experimental test of the ‘harmless error’ rule. Law and Human Behavior, 21, 27–46CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kassin, S. M., and Wrightsman, L. S. (1980). Prior confessions and mock juror verdicts. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 10, 133–46CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kassin, S. M., and Wrightsman, L. S. (1985). Confession evidence. In S. M. Kassin and L. S. Wrightsman (eds.), The psychology of evidence and trial procedure (pp. 67–94). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage
Köhnken, G. (1987). Training police officers to detect deceptive eyewitness statements: Does it work?Social Behavior, 2, 1–17Google Scholar
Lassiter, G. D., and Geers, A. L. (2004). Bias and accuracy in the evaluation of confession evidence. In G. D. Lassiter (ed.), Interrogations, Confessions, and Entrapment. New York: Kluwer AcademicCrossRef
Lassiter, G. D., Geers, A. L., Handley, I. M., Weiland, P. E., and Munhall, P. J. (2002). Videotaped confessions and interrogations: A simple change in camera perspective alters verdicts in simulated trials. Journal of Applied Psychology, 87, 867–74CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lassiter, G. D., Geers, A. L., Munhall, P. J., Handley, I. M., and Beers, M. J. (2001). Videotaped confessions: Is guilt in the eye of the camera?Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 33, 189–254CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leo, R. A. (1996a). Inside the interrogation room. Journal of Criminal Law and Criminology, 86, 266–303CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leo, R. A. (1996b). Miranda's revenge: Police interrogation as a confidence game. Law and Society Review, 30, 259–88CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leo, R. A., and Ofshe, R. J. (1998). The consequences of false confessions: Deprivations of liberty and miscarriages of justice in the age of psychological interrogation. Journal of Criminal Law and Criminology, 88, 429–96CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leo, R. A., and Ofshe, R. J. (2001). The truth about false confessions and advocacy scholarship. Criminal Law Bulletin, 37, 293–370Google Scholar
Macdonald, J. M., and Michauel, D. L. (1987). The confession: Interrogation and criminal profiles for police officers. Denver: Apache
Markman, S. J., and Cassell, P. G. (1988). Protecting the innocent: A response to the Bedau–Radelet study. Stanford Law Review, 41, 121CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Masip, J., Alonso, H., Garrido, E., and Anton, C. (in press). Generalized Communicative Suspicion (GCS) among police officers: Accounting for the investigator bias effect. Journal of Applied Social Psychology
McCann, J. T. (1998). Broadening the typology of false confessions. American Psychologist, 53, 319–20CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McNatt, D. B. (2000). Ancient Pygmalion joins contemporary management: A meta-analysis of the result. Journal of Applied Psychology, 85, 314–22CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meili, T. (2003). I am the Central Park jogger: A story of hope and possibility. New York: Scribner
Meissner, C. A., and Kassin, S. M. (2002). ‘He's guilty!’: Investigator bias in judgements of truth and deception. Law & Human Behavior, 26, 469–80CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meissner, C. A., and Kassin, S. M. (2004). ‘You're guilty, so just confess!’ Cognitive and behavioral confirmation biases in the interrogation room. In D. Lassiter (ed.), Interrogations, confessions, and entrapment. New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum
Memon, A., Vrij, A., and Bull, R. (2003). Psychology and law: Truthfulness, accuracy and credibility. London: Jossey-Bass
New York v. McCray, Richardson, Salaam, Santana, and Wise (2002). Affirmation in response to motion to vacate judgment of conviction indictment No. 4762/89
Nickerson, R. S. (1998). Confirmation bias: A ubiquitous phenomenon in many guises. Review of General Psychology, 2, 175–220CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ofshe, R. J., and Leo, R. A. (1997). The social psychology of police interrogation: The theory and classification of true and false confessions. Studies in Law, Politics, and Society, 16, 189–251Google Scholar
O'Hara, C. E., and O'Hara, G. L. (1981). Fundamentals of criminal investigation. Springfield. IL: Charles C. Thomas
Porter, S., Woodworth, M., and Birt, A. R. (2000). Truth, lies, and videotape: An investigation of the ability of federal parole officers to detect deception. Law and Human Behavior, 24, 643–58CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Redlich, A. D., and Goodman, G. S. (2003). Taking responsibility for an act not committed: The influence of age and suggestibility. Law and Human Behavior, 27, 141–56CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rimer, S. (2002). Convict's DNA sways labs, not a determined prosecutor. New York Times, February 6Google Scholar
Rosenthal, R., and Jacobson, L. (1968). Pygmalion in the classroom: Teacher expectation and pupils' intellectual development. New York: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston
Saulny, S. (2002). Why confess to what you didn't do?New York Times, December 8, 2002, Section 4Google Scholar
Scheck, B., Neufeld, P., and Dwyer, J. (2000). Actual innocence. Garden City, NY: Doubleday
Simon, D. (1991). Homicide: A year on the killing streets. New York: Ivy Books
Slobogin, C. (2003). Toward taping. Ohio State Journal of Criminal Law, 1, 309–22Google Scholar
Snyder, M. (1992). Motivational foundations of behavioral confirmation. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 25, 67–114CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Snyder, M., and Stukas, A. (1999). Interpersonal processes: The interplay of cognitive, motivational, and behavioral activities in social interaction. Annual Review of Psychology, 50, 273–303CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sullivan, T. (1992). Unequal verdicts: The Central Park jogger trials. NY: Simon and Schuster
Vrij, A. (1994). The impact of information and setting on detection of deception by police detectives. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 18, 117–37CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vrij, A. (2000). Detecting lies and deceit: The psychology of lying and the implications for professional practice. London: John Wiley
Vrij, A., and Mann, S. (2001). Who killed my relative? Police officers' ability to detect real-life high-stake lies. Psychology, Crime, and Law, 7, 119–32CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Walkley, J. (1987). Police interrogation. Handbook for investigators. London: Police Review Publication
Walters, S. B. (2003). Principles of kinesic interview and interrogation (2nd edn), Boca Ratan: CRC Press
Wrightsman, L. S., and Kassin, S. M. (1993). Confessions in the courtroom. Newbury Park, CA: Sage
Zimbardo, P. G. (1967, June). The psychology of police confessions. Psychology Today, 1, 17–20, 25–7Google Scholar
Zuckerman, M., DePaulo, B. M., and Rosenthal, R. (1981). Verbal and nonverbal communication of deception. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 14, 1–59CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zuckerman, M., Koestner, R., and Alton, A. O. (1984). Learning to detect deception. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 46, 519–28CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×