Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-tn8tq Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-06-15T04:34:03.877Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

11 - Social Cognition and Social Relationships

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 September 2009

Frieder R. Lang
Affiliation:
Martin Luther-Universität Halle-Wittenburg, Germany
Karen L. Fingerman
Affiliation:
Purdue University, Indiana
Fredda Blanchard-Fields
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, Georgia Institute of Technology, 274, 5th Street, Atlanta, GA 30332-0170
Carolyn Cooper
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, Georgia Institute of Technology, 274, 5th Street, Atlanta, GA 30332-0170
Get access

Summary

From a social cognitive and developmental perspective, we offer a content-based explanation for age differences in social judgment biases when individuals are presented with relationship dilemmas. Older adults' tendency to blame individuals for negative outcomes in relationship situations may reflect age-related differences in the nature and content of information stored in long-term memory (knowledge, beliefs, and values) that are relevant to the particular social judgment. Older adults may have highly schematized social rules for appropriate behavior that are chronically accessible and drive social judgments in particular situations, especially in the domain of marital relationships. In addition, we suggest that changes in emotional functioning as we grow older also impact the attributional processes in the context of marital relationships.

There is a substantive body of research examining the importance of social cognitive mechanisms in understanding adaptive social relationships. These include how relationship enhancing attributions relate to marital satisfaction (Bradbury & Fincham, 1990; Lopez, 1993), how selectively attending to negative aspects of a relationship creates relationship distress (Baucom, Epstein, Sayers, & Sher, 1989; Sillars, Roberts, Leonard, & Dun, 2000), how individuals preserve positive self-views in the context of a relationship (Fincham & Beach, 1999), and the accessibility of attitudes, values, and beliefs when engaging in interpersonal interactions (Baldwin, 1992; Fincham & Beach, 1999). Individual differences in the influence of such cognitive representations and processes may help us to differentiate adaptive from dysfunctional cognitions in dealing with relationship situations across the life span.

Type
Chapter
Information
Growing Together
Personal Relationships Across the Life Span
, pp. 268 - 289
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2003

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Baldwin, M. W. (1992). Relational schemas and the processing of social information. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 461–484CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baldwin, M. W., Keelan, J. P. R., Fehr, B., Enns, V., & Koh-Rangarajoo, E. (1996). Social-cognitive conceptualization of attachment working models: Availability and accessibility effects. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 94–109CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bargh, J. A. (1989). Conditional automaticity: Varieties of automatic influence in social perception and cognition. In J. S. Uleman and J. A. Bargh (Eds.), Unintended thought (pp. 3–51). New York: The Guilford Press
Bargh, J. A. (1997). The automaticity of everyday life. In R. S. Wyer, Jr. (Ed.), The automaticity of everyday life: Advances in social cognition (Vol. 10, pp. 1–61). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates
Baucom, D. H., Epstein, N., Daiuto, A. D., Carels, R. A., Rankin, L. A., & Burnett, C. K. (1996). Cognitions in marriage: The relationship between standards and attributions. Journal of Family Psychology, 10, 209–222CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baucom, D. H., Epstein, N., Sayers, S., & Sher, T. G. (1989). The role of cognitions in marital relationships: Definitional, methodological, and conceptual issues. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 57, 31–38CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baucom, D. H., Sayers, S. L., & Duhe, A. (1989). Attributional style and attributional patterns among married couples. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 596–607CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blanchard-Fields, F. (1994). Age differences in causal attributions from an adult development perspective. Journal of Gerontology, 49, P43–P51CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blanchard-Fields, F. (1996). Causal attributions across the adult life span: The influence of social schemas, life context, and domain specificity. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 10, 137–1463.0.CO;2-Z>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blanchard-Fields, F. (1997). The role of emotion in social cognition across the adult life span. In K. W. Schaie & M. P. Lawton (Eds.), Annual review of gerontology and geriatrics (Vol. 17, pp. 238–265). New York: Springer Publishing Company, Inc.
Blanchard-Fields, F. (1999). Social schematicity and causal attributions. In T. M. Hess & F. Blanchard-Fields (Eds.), Social cognition and aging. San Diego, CA: Academic Press, Inc.
Blanchard-Fields, F., Chen, Y., Schocke, M., & Hertzog, C. (1998). Evidence for content-specificity of causal attributions across the adult life span. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 5, 241–263Google Scholar
Blanchard-Fields, F., & Hertzog, C. (2000). Age differences in schematicity. In U. von Hecker, S. Dutke, & G. Sedek (Eds.), Processes of generative mental representation and psychological adaptation. Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer
Blanchard-Fields, F., & Norris, L. (1994). Causal attributions from adolescence through adulthood: Age differences, ego level, and generalized response style. Aging and Cognition, 1, 67–86Google Scholar
Bradbury, T. N., & Fincham, F. D. (1990). Attributions in marriage: Review and critique. Psychological Bulletin, 107, 3–33CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Campos, J. J., Campos, R. G., & Barrett, K. C. (1991). Emergent themes in the study of emotional development and emotion regulation. Developmental Psychology, 24, 394–402Google Scholar
Carstensen, L. L. (1992). Selectivity theory: Social activity in life-span context. Annual Review of Gerontology and Geriatrics, 11, 195–217Google Scholar
Carstensen, L. L., Graff, J., Levenson, R. W., & Gottman, J. M. (1996). Affect in intimate relationships: The developmental course of marriage. In C. Magai & S. H. McFadden (Eds.), Handbook of emotion, adult development, and aging (pp. 227–247). San Diego, CA: Academic PressCrossRef
Chen, Y., & Blanchard-Fields, F. (1997). Age differences in stages of causal attributional processing. Psychology and Aging, 12, 694–703CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cooper, C., & Blanchard-Fields, F. (2001, November). Do personal identification and interpersonal involvement explain age differences in attributional biases? Poster session presented at the annual meeting of the Gerontological Society of America, Chicago, IL
Damasio, A. R. (1994). Descartes' error. New York: Grosset Putnam
Diener, E., Sandvik, E., & Larsen, R. J. (1985). Age and sex differences for emotional intensity. Developmental Psychology, 21, 542–546CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Erber, R., & Erber, M. W. (1994). Beyond mood and social judgment: Mood incongruent recall and mood recognition. European Journal of Social Psychology, 24, 79–88CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fazio, R. H., Sanbonmatsu, D. M., Powell, M. C., & Kardes, F. R. (1986). On the automatic activation of attitudes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 229–238CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ferring, D., & Filipp, S. H. (1995). The structure of subjective well-being in the elderly: A test of different models by structural equation modeling. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 11, 32Google Scholar
Filipp, S. H. (1996). Motivation and emotion. In J. E. Birren & K. W. Schaie (Eds.), Handbook of psychology and aging (4th ed., pp. 218–235). San Diego, CA: Academic Press
Fincham, F. D., & Beach, S. R. H. (1999). Marriage in the new millennium: Is there a place for social cognition in marital research? Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 16, 685–704CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fincham, F. D., Garnier, P. C., Gano-Phillips, S., & Osborne, L. N. (1995). Preinteraction expectations, marital satisfaction, and accessibility: A new look at sentiment override. Journal of Family Psychology, 9, 3–14CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fiske, S. (1993). Social cognition and social perception. Annual Review of Psychology, 44, 155–194CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fletcher, G. J. O., Rosanowski, J., & Fitness, J. (1994). Automatic processing in intimate contexts: The role of close-relationship beliefs. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 888–897CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Forgas, J. P. (1991). Affect and cognition in close relationships. In G. Fletcher & F. D. Fincham (Eds.), Cognition in close relationships (pp. 151–174). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates
Forgas, J. P. (1995). Mood and judgment: The affect infusion model (AIM). Psychological Bulletin, 117, 39–66CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Forgas, J. P. (1998). Happy and mistaken? Mood effects on the fundamental attribution error. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 318–331CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Forgas, J. P. (2001). Affective influences on communication and attribution in relationships. In V. Manusov & J. H. Harvey (Eds.), Attribution, communication behavior, and close relationships (pp. 3–20). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
Gilbert, D. T., & Malone, P. S. (1995). The correspondence bias. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 21–38CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gilbert, D. T., & Osborne, R. E. (1989). Thinking backward: Some curable and incurable consequences of cognitive busyness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 940–949CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gilbert, D. T., Pelham, B. W., & Krull, D. S. (1988). On cognitive busyness: When person perceivers meet persons perceived. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 733–740CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gilbert, D. T., Tafarodi, R. W., & Malone, P. S. (1993). You can't not believe everything you read. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 221–233CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heider, F. (1958). The psychology of interpersonal relations. New York: Wiley
Hess, T. M., & Pullen, S. M. (1994). Adult age differences in impression change processes. Psychology and Aging, 9, 237–250CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holmes, J. G. (2000). Social relationships: The nature and function of relational schemas. European Journal of Social Psychology, 30, 447–4953.0.CO;2-Q>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holtzworth-Munroe, A., & Jacobson, N. S. (1985). Causal attributions of married couples: When do they search for causes? What do they conclude when they do? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 1398–1412CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lawton, M. P., Kleban, M. H., & Rajagopal, D., & Dean, J. (1992). Dimensions of affective experience in three age groups. Psychology and Aging, 7, 171–184CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levenson, R. (1997, August). “The social context of emotion throughout the life span,” symposium for the APA Presidential Mini-convention on Psychology and the Aging Revolution, annual American Psychological Association Conference, Chicago, IL
Levenson, R. W., Carstensen, L. L., Friesen, W. V., & Ekman, P. (1991). Emotion, physiology, and expression in old age. Psychology and Aging, 6, 28–35CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levenson, R. W., Carstensen, L. L., & Gottman, J. M. (1993). Long-term marriage: Age, gender, and satisfaction. Psychology and Aging, 82, 301–313CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levenson, R. W., Carstensen, L. L., & Gottman, J. M. (1994). The influence of age and gender on affect, physiology, and their interrelations: A study of long-term marriages. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 56–68CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lopez, F. G. (1993). Cognitive processes in close relationships: Recent findings and implications for counseling. Journal of Counseling and Development, 71, 310–315CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Manusov, V., & Koenig, J. (2001). The content of attributions in couples' communication. In V. Manusov & J. H. Harvey (Eds.), Attribution, communication behavior, and close relationships. Advances in personal relations. New York: Cambridge University Press
Neff, L. A., & Karney, B. R. (2002, February). Differential importance and relationship maintenance: The dynamic structure of relationship beliefs. Paper presented at the meeting of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Savannah, GA
Newman, H. (1981). Communication within ongoing intimate relationships: An attributional perspective. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 7, 59–70CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pierce, T., & Lydon, J. (1998). Priming relational schemas: Effects of contextually activated and chronically accessible interpersonal expectations on responses to a stressful event. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 1441–1448CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salthouse, T. A. (1991). Theoretical perspectives on cognitive aging. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates
Sansone, C., & Berg, C. A. (1993). Adapting to the environment across the life span: Different process or different inputs? International Journal of Behavioral Development, 16, 215–241CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schaie, K. W. (1996). Intellectual development in adulthood. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
Schaie, K. W., & Lawton, M. P. (Eds.) (1997). Annual review of gerontology and geriatrics (Vol. 17). New York: Springer
Schwarz, N., Strack, F., & Mai, H. P. (1991). Assimilation and contrast effects in part-whole question sequences: A conversation logic analysis. Public Opinion Quarterly, 55, 3–23CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scott, C. K., Fuhrman, R. W., & Wyer, R. S. (1991). Information processing in close relationships. In G. J. O. Fletcher & F. D. Fincham (Eds.), Cognition in close relationships (pp. 37–67). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates
Sillars, A., Roberts, L. J., Leonard, K. E., & Dun, T. (2000). Cognition during marital conflict: The relationship of thought and talk. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 17, 479–502CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, J., & Baltes, P. B. (1993). Differential psychological ageing: Profiles of the old and very old. Ageing and Society, 13, 551–587CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Solomon, D. H. (2001). Affect, attribution, and communication: Uniting interaction episodes and global relationship judgments. In V. Manusov & J. H. Harvey (Eds.), Attribution, communication behavior, and close relationships (pp. 79–90). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
Strough, J., Berg, C. A., & Sansone, C. (1996). Goals for solving everyday problems across the life span: Age and gender differences in the salience of interpersonal concerns. Developmental Psychology, 32, 1106–1115CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weiss, R. L. (1980). Strategic behavioral marital therapy: Toward a model for assessment and intervention. In J. P. Vincent (Ed.), Advances in family intervention, assessment, and theory (Vol. 1, pp. 229–271). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press
Wyer, R. S., Jr., & Srull, T. K. (1989). Memory and cognition in its social context. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×