Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-hfldf Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-16T22:01:55.486Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

11 - What explains the differences found? A summary, and prospects for an ecology of the future

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 September 2009

Klaus Rohde
Affiliation:
University of New England, Australia
Get access

Summary

What explains the differences between communities?

Rohde (1980a) suggested that animal communities can be arranged in a continuum from random and unstructured to highly structured, depending on ecological characteristics of species in the communities. Animals with little vagility and/or small population or individual size live in largely empty niche space. They are less subject to structuring mechanisms, in particular competition, than are large animals or animals that live in large populations with much vagility (although they may be nonrandom to a degree because of nonrandom colonization events). The latter have filled extant niche space to a greater degree, i.e., they are closer to saturation, and include the predominantly large mammals and birds, and free-living vagile insects occurring in large populations. (Saturation, however, does not exclude the possibility of further increases in diversity by subdivision of niches.) Gotelli and Rohde (2002) tested this hypothesis using null-model analysis to check for nonrandomness in the structure of metazoan ectoparasites of 45 species of marine fish, and compared the results with those for herps, birds, and mammals. In parasites, co-occurrence patterns could not be distinguished from those that might arise by random colonization and extinction. Presence–absence matrices for small-bodied taxa (parasites, herps) with low vagility and/or small population size were mostly random, whereas presence–absence matrices for large-bodied taxa with high vagility and/or large population size (birds, mammals) were highly structured, supporting Rohde's hypothesis. For Figure 11.1, some data from Gotelli and McCabe (2002) were also used, which support the hypothesis even more strongly.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2006

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×