Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-ttngx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-01T12:17:40.948Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

16 - Social separation models of depression

from Part VII - Affective illness

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  19 January 2010

Keith Matthews
Affiliation:
Ninewells Medical School, Dundee, UK
Maria A. Ron
Affiliation:
Institute of Neurology, London
Trevor W. Robbins
Affiliation:
University of Cambridge
Get access

Summary

Introduction

The clinical presentations that attract the label ‘depression’ represent a major public health concern because of their high prevalence, stigma, morbidity and mortality. Despite best efforts, research has, thus far, failed to illuminate a plausible pathophysiology of depression, to explain why some individuals are more at risk than others or to explain the efficacy of available treatments. Modelling pathophysiological states in experimental animals has proven an effective strategy in improving our understanding of many other major medical conditions and has led to the development of better treatments. In this chapter, I shall review a range of work with experimental animals that may contribute to our eventual understanding of some aspects of depression. This review focuses on the manipulation of social environment as a means of perturbing physiology and behaviour and attempts to relate findings from animal studies to core clinical concepts.

What is depression?

Depression is a common, complex and poorly understood condition. Perhaps because the same word is used to describe the brief periods of unhappiness and disappointment that everyone experiences in response to minor life upsets, there is a popular view that depression is a relatively trivial and self-limiting problem. The reality is very different.

It (depression) was the worst experience of my life. More terrible even than watching my wife die of cancer … I was in a state that bears no resemblance to anything I had experienced before, I was not just feeling low … I was seriously ill.

(Wolpert 1999)

I was feeling in my mind a sensation close to, but indescribably different from pain … for myself, the pain is most closely connected to drowning or suffocation — but even these images are off the mark.

(Styron 1992)
Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2003

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Ainsworth M D (1962). The Effects of Maternal Deprivation: A Review of Findings and Controversy in the Context of Research Strategy. Geneva: World Health Organization
Alexander, G E, DeLong, M R and Strick, P L (1986). Parallel organisation of functionally segregated circuits linking basal ganglia and cortex. Annu Rev Neurosci, 9, 357–81Google Scholar
Altman, J, Everitt, B J, Glautier, S et al. (1996). The biological, social and clinical bases of drug addiction: commentary and debate. Psychopharmacology, 125, 285–345Google Scholar
American Psychiatric Association (1994). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th edn. Washington, DC: APA
Arango, V, Underwood, M D and Mann, J J (1997). Biologic alterations in the brainstem of suicides. Psychiatric Clin N Am, 20, 581–93Google Scholar
Ashton, A B and Trowill, J A (1970). Effects of reinforcement shifts upon lick rate. Psychonom Sci, 21, 7–8Google Scholar
Axelson, D A, Doraiswamy, P M, McDonald, W M et al. (1993). Hypercortisolemia and hippocampal changes in depression. Psychiatry Res, 47, 163–73Google Scholar
Baumann, B, Danos, P, Krell, D et al. (1999). Reduced volume of limbic system-affiliated basal ganglia in mood disorders: preliminary data from a postmortem study. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci, 11, 71–8Google Scholar
Beck, A T, Sethi, B B and Tuthill, R W (1963). Childhood bereavement and adult depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry, 9, 295–302Google Scholar
Beck A T, Rush A J, Shaw B and Emery G (1979). Cognitive Therapy of Depression. New York: Guilford
Bernstein, H and Bogerts, B (1999). Reduced volume of limbic system-affiliated basal ganglia in mood disorders: preliminary data from a post-mortem study. J Neuropsych Clin Neurosci, 11, 71–8Google Scholar
Bohn, M C (1980). Granule cell genesis in the hippocampus of rats treated neonatally with hydrocortisone. Neuroscience, 5, 2003–12Google Scholar
Bowlby, J (1944). Forty-four juvenile thieves: their characters and home life (II). Int J Psychoanal, 25, 107–27Google Scholar
Bowlby J (1951). Maternal Care and Mental Health. Geneva: World Health Organization
Bowlby J (1969). Attachment and Loss: Volume 1 — Attachment. London: Hogarth Press
Bowlby, J, Ainsworth, M, Boston, M and Rosenbluth, D (1956). The effects of mother-child separation: a follow-up study. Br J Med Psychol, 29, 211–44Google Scholar
Bremner, J D, Innis, R B, Salomon, R M et al. (1997). Positron emission tomography measurement of cerebral metabolic correlates of tryptophan depletion-induced depressive relapse. Arch Gen Psychiatry, 54, 364–74Google Scholar
Brown G W and Harris T O (1978). Social Origins of Depression: A Study of Psychiatric Disorder in Women. London: Tavistock Publications
Burton R (1989). The Anatomy of Melancholy, ed. N C Faulkner, T C Faulkner and R L Blair. Oxford: Oxford University Press
Cador, M, Taylor, J R and Robbins, T W (1991). Potentiation of the effects of reward-related stimuli by dopaminergic mechanisms in the nucleus accumbens. Psychopharmacology, 104, 377–85Google Scholar
Cirruli, F, Gottlieb, S L, Rosenfeld, P and Levine, S (1992). Maternal factors regulate stress responsiveness in the neonatal rat. Psychobiology, 20, 143–52Google Scholar
Coplan, J D, Andrews, M W, Rosenblum, L A et al. (1996). Persistent elevations of cerebrospinal fluid concentrations of corticotrophin-releasing factor in adult non-human primates exposed to early life stressors: implications for the pathophysiology of mood and anxiety disorders. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA, 93, 1619–23Google Scholar
Crespi, L P (1944). Amount of reinforcement and level of performance. Psychol Rev, 51, 341–57Google Scholar
D'Haenen, H A and Bossuyt, A (1994). Dopamine D2 receptors in depression measured with single photon emission computed tomography. Biol Psychiatry, 35, 128–32Google Scholar
Drevets, W C (1998). Functional neuroimaging studies of depression: the anatomy of melancholia. Ann Rev Med, 49, 341–61Google Scholar
Drevets, W C, Price, J L, Simpson, J R et al. (1997) Subgenual prefrontal cortex abnormalities in mood disorders. Nature, 386, 824–7Google Scholar
Ebert, D, Feistel, H, Loew, T and Pirner, A (1996). Dopamine and depression-striatal dopamine D2 receptor SPECT before and after antidepressant therapy. Psychopharmacology, 126, 91–4Google Scholar
Everitt B J and Keverne E B (1979). Models of depression based on behavioural observations of experimental animals. In Psychopharmacology of Affective Disorders, ed. E S Paykel and A Coppen, pp. 41–59. Oxford: Oxford University Press
Fawcett, J and Siomopoulos, V (1971). Dextroamphetamine response as a possible predictor of improvement with tricyclic therapy in depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry, 25, 247–55Google Scholar
Flaherty, C F (1982). Incentive contrast: a review of behavioral changes following shifts in reward. Anim Learn Behav, 10, 409–40Google Scholar
Flaherty, C F and Driscoll, C (1980). Amylobarbitone sodium reduces successive negative gustatory contrast. Psychopharmacology, 69, 161–2Google Scholar
Forbes, N, Stewart, C A, Matthews, K and Reid, I C (1996). Chronic mild stress and sucrose consumption: validity as a model of depression. Physiol Behav, 60, 1481–4Google Scholar
Freud S (1963). The Standard Edition of the Complete Psychological Works of Sigmund Freud. (Translated by J Strachey.) London: Hogarth Press
Goldman, L, Coover, G D and Levine, S (1973). Bidirectional effects of reinforcement shifts on pituitary adrenal activity. Physiol Behav, 10, 209–14Google Scholar
Gwirtsman, H E and Guze, B H (1989). Amphetamine, but not methylphenidate, predicts antidepressant response. J Clin Psychopharmacol, 9, 453–4Google Scholar
Harlow, H (1958). The nature of love. Am Psychol, 13, 673–85Google Scholar
Harlow, H and Harlow, M (1962). Social deprivation in monkeys. Sci Am, 207, 137–46Google Scholar
Hennessy, M B (1997). Hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal responses to brief social separation. Neurosci Biobehav Rev, 21, 11–29Google Scholar
Hill, O W (1969). The association of childhood bereavement with suicidal attempt in depressive illness. Br J Psychiatry, 115, 301–4Google Scholar
Hill, S D, McCormack, S A and Mason, W A (1973). Effects of artificial mothers and visual experience on adrenal responsiveness of infant monkeys. Dev Psychobiol, 6, 421–9Google Scholar
Hinde, R A and Spencer-Booth, Y (1971). Effects of brief separation from mothers on rhesus monkeys. Science, 173, 117–18Google Scholar
Hinde, R A, Spencer-Booth, Y and Bruce, M (1966). Effects of 6-day maternal deprivation on rhesus monkey infants. Nature, 210, 1021–33Google Scholar
Hofer, M A (1994 a). Early relationships as regulators of infant physiology and behavior. Acta Paediatr, 397 (Suppl. 1), S9–18Google Scholar
Hofer M A (1994b). Hidden regulators in attachment, separation, and loss. In The Development of Emotion Regulation: Biological and Behavioral Considerations, ed. N A Fox. Monogr Soc Res Child Dev, 59, 192–283
Hrdina, P D, Kulmiz, P and Stretch, R (1979). Pharmacological modification of experimental depression in infant macaques. Psychopharmacology, 64, 89–93Google Scholar
Jacoby, R J and Levy, R (1980). Computed tomography in the elderly: affective disorder. Br J Psychiatry, 136, 270–5Google Scholar
Judd, L L (1997). The clinical course of unipolar major depressive disorders. Arch Gen Psychiatry, 54, 989–91Google Scholar
Kaneko, W M, Riley, E P and Ehlers, C L (1994). Behavioral and electrophysiological effects of early repeated maternal separation. Depression, 2, 43–53Google Scholar
Katz, R J (1981). Animal models and human depressive disorders. Neurosci Biobehav Rev, 5, 231–46Google Scholar
Katz, R J (1982). Animal model of depression: pharmacological sensitivity of a hedonic deficit. Pharmacol Biochem Behav, 16, 965–8Google Scholar
De Kloet E R, Rosenfeld P, Van Eekelen J, Sutanato W and Levine S (1988). Stress, glucocorticoids and development. In Progress in Brain Research, Vol. 73, ed. G J Boer, M Feenstra, M Mirmira, D Swaab and F Van Haaren, pp. 101–20. Amsterdam: Elsevier
Kraemer, G W (1988). Speculations on the developmental neurobiology of protest and despair. Anim Mod Psychiatr Disord, 2, 101–39Google Scholar
Kraemer, G W and McKinney, W T (1979). Interactions of pharmacological agents which alter biogenic amine metabolism and depression: an analysis of contributing factors within a primate model of depression. J Affect Disord, 1, 33–54Google Scholar
Kraemer, G W, Ebert, M H, Lake, C R and McKinney, W T (1984 a). Cerebrospinal fluid measures of neurotransmitter changes associated with pharmacological alteration of the despair response to social separation in rhesus monkeys. Psychiatry Res, 11, 303–15Google Scholar
Kraemer, G W, Ebert, M H, Lake, C R and McKinney, W T (1984 b). Hypersensitivity to d-amphetamine several years after early social deprivation in rhesus monkeys. Psychopharmacology, 82, 266–71Google Scholar
Krishnan, K R, McDonald, W M, Doraiswamy, P M et al. (1993). Neuroanatomical substrates of depression in the elderly. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci, 243, 41–6Google Scholar
Kuhn, C M, Butler, S R and Schanberg, S M (1978). Selective depression of growth hormone during maternal deprivation in rat pups. Science, 201, 1034–6Google Scholar
Kuhn, C M, Pauk, J and Schanberg, S M (1990). Endocrine responses to mother–infant separation in developing rats. Dev Psychobiol, 23, 395–410Google Scholar
Kupfer, D J and Foster, F G (1972). Interval between onset of sleep and rapid eye movement sleep as an indicator of depression. Lancet, 2, 684–6Google Scholar
Laasonen-Balk, T, Kuikka, J, Viinamaki, H, Husso-Saastamoinen, M, Lehtonen, J and Tiihonen, J (1999). Striatal dopamine transporter density in major depression. Psychopharmacology, 144, 282–5Google Scholar
Ladd, C O, Owens, M J and Nemeroff, C B (1996). Persistent changes in corticotrophin-releasing factor neuronal systems induced by maternal deprivation. Endocrinology, 137, 1212–18Google Scholar
Lewis, M H, Gluck, J P, Beauchamp, A J, Keresztruy, M F, and Mailman, R B (1990). Long-term effects of early social isolation In Macaca mulatta: changes in dopamine receptor function following apomorphine challenge. Brain Res, 513, 67–73Google Scholar
Mackintosh N J (1974). The Psychology of Animal Learning. London: Academic Press
Mann, J J and Stoff, D M (1997). A synthesis of current findings regarding neurobiological correlates and treatment of suicidal behavior. Ann NY Acad Sci, 836, 352–63Google Scholar
Markou, A, Kosten, T R and Koob, G F (1998). Neurobiological similarities in depression and drug dependence: a self-medication hypothesis. Neuropsychopharmacology, 18, 135–74Google Scholar
Martin, L J, Spicer, D M, Lewis, M H, Gluck, J P, and Cork, L C (1991). Social deprivation of infant rhesus monkeys alters the chemoarchitecture of the brain: I. Subcortical regions. J Neurosci, 11, 3344–58Google Scholar
Matthews, K, Forbes, N and Reid, I C (1995). Sucrose consumption as an hedonic measure following chronic unpredictable mild stress. Physiol Behav, 57, 241–8Google Scholar
Matthews, K, Wilkinson, L S and Robbins, T W (1996 a). Repeated maternal separation of pre-weanling rats attenuates the behavioural responses to primary and conditioned incentives in adulthood. Physiol Behav, 59, 99–107Google Scholar
Matthews, K, Hall, F S, Wilkinson, L S and Robbins, T W (1996 b). Retarded acquisition and reduced expression of conditioned locomotor activity in adult rats following repeated early maternal separation: effects of prefeeding, d-amphetamine, dopamine antagonists and clonidine. Psychopharmacology, 126, 75–84Google Scholar
Matthews, K, Robbins, T W, Everitt, B J and Caine, S B (1999). Repeated neonatal maternal separation alters intravenous cocaine self-administration in adult rats. Psychopharmacology, 141, 123–34Google Scholar
Matthews, K, Dalley, J W, Matthews, C A, Tsai, T H and Robbins, T W (2001). Periodic maternal separation of neonatal rats produces region and gender-specific effects on biogenic amine content in post-mortem adult brain. Synapse, 40, 1–10Google Scholar
McKinney, W T and Bunney, W E (1969). Animal model of depression: review of evidence and implications for research. Arch Gen Psychiatry, 21, 240–8Google Scholar
Mineka, S and Suomi, S J (1978). Social separation in monkeys. Psychol Bull, 85, 1376–400Google Scholar
Munro, A (1966). Parental deprivation in depressive patients. Br J Psychiatry, 112, 443–57Google Scholar
Nemeroff, C B (1998). The neurobiology of depression. Sci Am, 278, 42–9Google Scholar
O'Donnell, D, Larocque, S, Seckl, J R and Meaney, M J (1994). Postnatal handling alters glucocorticoid but not mineralocorticoid mRNA expression in adult rats. Mol Brain Res, 26, 242–8Google Scholar
Ongur, D, Drevets, W C and Price, J L (1998). Glial reduction in the subgenual prefrontal cortex in mood disorders. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA, 95, 13290–5Google Scholar
Paykel, E S and Hollyman, J A (1984). Life events and depression – a psychiatric view. Trends Neurosci, 12, 478–81Google Scholar
Phillips, G D, Willner, P and Muscat, R (1991). Suppression or facilitation of operant behaviour by raclopride dependent on concentration of sucrose reward. Psychopharmacology, 105, 239–46Google Scholar
Porsolt, R D, LePichon, M and Jalfre, M (1977). Depression: a new animal model sensitive to antidepressant treatment. Nature, 266, 730–2Google Scholar
Rajkowska, G, Miguel-Hidalgo, J J, Wei, J et al. (1999). Morphometric evidence for neuronal and glial prefrontal cell pathology in major depression. Biol Psychiatry, 45, 1085–98Google Scholar
Reite M and Short R (1983). Maternal separation studies: rationale and methodological considerations. In Ethopharmacology: Primate Models of Neuropsychiatric Disorders, ed. K A Miczek, pp. 219–53. New York: Liss
Ribble M A (1944). Infantile experience in relation to personality development. In Personality and Behavior Disorders, ed. J M Hunt, pp. 621–51. New York: Ronald Press
Robertson, M M and Trimble, M R (1981). Neuroleptics as antidepressants. Neuropsychopharmacology, 21, 1335–6Google Scholar
Rosenblum, L and Kaufman, I C (1968). Variations in infant development and response to mother loss in monkeys. Am J Orthopsychiatr, 38, 418–26Google Scholar
Rots, N, Jong, J, Workel, J O, Levine, S, Cools, A R and Kloet, E R (1996). Neonatal maternally deprived rats have as adults elevated basal pituitary adrenal activity and enhanced susceptibility to apomorphine. J Neuroendocrinol, 8, 501–6Google Scholar
Sackeim, H A (2001). Functional brain circuits in major depression and remission. Arch Gen Psychiatry, 58, 649–50Google Scholar
Sapolsky, R M and Meaney, M J (1986). Maturation of the adrenal stress response: neuroendocrine control mechanisms and the stress-hyporesponsive period. Brain Res Rev, 11, 65–76Google Scholar
Seay, B M and Harlow, H (1965). Maternal separation in the rhesus monkey. J Nerv Ment Dis, 140, 434–41Google Scholar
Seay, B M, Hansen, E W and Harlow, H (1962). Mother–infant separation in monkeys. J Child Psychol Psychiatry, 3, 123–32Google Scholar
Seitz, P F D (1959). Infantile experience and adult behaviour in animal subjects: age of separation from the mother and adult behavior in the cat. Psychosom Med, 21, 353–78Google Scholar
Shah, P J, Ogilvie, A D, Goodwin, G M and Ebmeier, K P (1997). Clinical and psychometric correlates of dopamine D2 binding in depression. Psychol Med, 27, 1247–56Google Scholar
Sheline, Y L, Wang, P W, Gado, M H, Csernansky, J G, and Vannier, M W (1996). Hippocampal atrophy in recurrent major depression. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA, 93, 3908–13Google Scholar
Smith A L and Weissman M M (1992). Epidemiology. In Handbook of Affective Disorders, 2nd edn, ed. E S Paykel, pp. 111–31. London: Churchill Livingstone
Spitz, R A (1945). Hospitalism. Psychoanal Study Child, 1, 53–74Google Scholar
Spitz, R A and Wolf, K A (1946). Anaclitic depression: an inquiry into the genesis of psychiatric conditions in early childhood. Psychoanal Study Child, 2, 313–42Google Scholar
Styron W (1992). Darkness Visible. London: Picador
Suchecki, D, Rosenfeld, P and Levine, S (1993). Maternal regulation of hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis in the infant rat: the role of feeding and stroking. Dev Brain Res, 75, 185–92Google Scholar
Suomi, S J, Harlow, H and Domek, C J (1970). Effect of repetitive mother–infant separation of young monkeys. J Abnorm Psychol, 76, 161–72Google Scholar
Suomi, S J, Mineka, S and DeLizio, R D (1973). Short and long-term effects of repetitive mother–infant separations on social development in rhesus monkeys. Dev Psychol, 19, 770–86Google Scholar
Suomi, S J, Seaman, S F, Lewis, J K, DeLizio, R D, and McKinney, W T (1978). Effects of imipramine treatment of separation-induced social disorders in rhesus monkeys. Arch Gen Psych, 35, 321–5Google Scholar
Tanda, G, Frau, R and Di Chiara, G (1997). Chronic desipramine and fluoxetine differentially affect extracellular dopamine in the rat prefrontal cortex. Psychopharmacology, 127, 83–7Google Scholar
Wehr, T A, Muscettola, G and Goodwin, F K (1980). 3-methoxy-4-hydroxyphenolglycol circadian rhythm: early timing (phase advance) in manic depressives compared with normal subjects. Arch Gen Psychiatry, 37, 257–63Google Scholar
Willner, P (1984). The validity of animal models of depression. Psychopharmacology, 83, 1–16Google Scholar
Willner P (1995). Dopaminergic machanisms in depression and mania. In Psychopharmacology: the Fourth Generation of Progress, ed. F E Bloom and D J Kupfer, pp. 921–32. New York: Raven Press
Willner, P, Towell, A, Sampson, D, Sophokleous, S and Muscat, R (1987). Reduction of sucrose preference by chronic unpredictable mild stress, and its restoration by a tricyclic antidepressant. Psychopharmacology, 93, 358–64Google Scholar
Wit, H, Ulenhath, E H and Johanson, C E (1987). The reinforcing properties of amphetamine in overweight subjects and subjects with depression. Clin Pharm Therap, 42, 127–36Google Scholar
Wolpert L (1999). Malignant Sadness: The Anatomy of Depression. London: Faber and Faber
World Health Organization (1992). The ICD-10 Classification of Mental and Behavioural Disorders: Clinical Descriptions and Diagnostic Guidelines. Geneva: WHO
Yassa, R, Camille, Y and Belzile, L (1987). Tardive dyskinesia in the course of antidepressant therapy: a prevalence study and review of the literature. J Clin Psychopharmacol, 7, 243–6Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×