Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-2lccl Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-27T20:20:53.028Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

6 - Socialization in the family

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  31 December 2009

Nancy Eisenberg
Affiliation:
Arizona State University
Paul Henry Mussen
Affiliation:
University of California, Irvine
Get access

Summary

As we have seen, children generally acquire the patterns of behavior, motives, personal characteristics, predispositions, values, and social responses that are adaptive and expected in their own cultures. Most children reared in Mexican villages, Hopi children on reservations in the Southwest, and youngsters on Israeli kibbutzim are more considerate, kind, and cooperative than their “typical” middle-class American counterparts.

Nevertheless, cultural factors alone cannot account for the wide range of prosocial actions within each culture; some children are consistently cooperative and generous, but others are not. These individual differences are attributable primarily to variations in the ways children are socialized in their own families, where they acquire many of their personal attributes, motives, and values.

Many agents contribute to the child's socialization and to the development of prosocial tendencies. In the early years, the family is the principal agent of socialization. In this chapter, attention is centered on how the family promotes (or inhibits) prosocial behavior and, most important, strengthens (or weakens) children's internalized motivations for altruism, helpfulness, generosity, and consideration of others.

Socialization involves many processes. Parents often self-consciously attempt to teach their children prosocial responses by praising and approving altruism and generosity and punishing selfish and uncooperative responses. They may instruct their children, reason with them, and explain rules or standards of prosocial behavior. But many of the child's prosocial actions and motivations are the products of more subtle processes, such as imitation or identification; these responses appear to emerge spontaneously, without direct training or reward - usually without anyone intending to “teach” and without the child intending to learn.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 1989

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×