Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-p2v8j Total loading time: 0.001 Render date: 2024-05-16T18:59:00.385Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Commentary

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 May 2010

Carol M. Worthman
Affiliation:
Emory University, Atlanta
Paul M. Plotsky
Affiliation:
Emory University, Atlanta
Daniel S. Schechter
Affiliation:
Hôpitaux Universitaires de Genève
Constance A. Cummings
Affiliation:
Foundation for Psychocultural Research, California
Get access

Summary

INTRODUCTION

The case study on infant abuse in rhesus macaques by M. Mar Sánchez, Kai McCormack, and Dario Maestripieri is particularly germane to a work about the effects of early life experiences on later outcomes. As the authors point out, behavioral similarities between nonhuman primate abuse and human abuse are very close, if not identical, given what we know at this time. Furthermore, studying the neuroendocrine, neurochemical, and neuroanatomical consequences in nonhuman primates, although challenging, can be especially helpful.

The work of Sánchez and colleagues has particular resonance and meaning for my field and my research. Three aspects in particular are intriguing points of convergence: (1) the nicely documented multi-system consequences (that is, the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal [HPA], serotonergic, and neuroimmune systems) and interactions of abuse and/or rejection, including intergenerational transmission; (2) the dimensional continuum approach to quality of early maternal care seen in natural observations; and (3) the age or developmental specificity of the time course of abuse and rejection. I will discuss each of these further in this commentary.

My connections with this case study intersect both at a general and a specific level. On a general level, my “field” is the very young subspecialty of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics. For this field, the influences of early life experiences on later functioning are absolutely central (Barr, 2004). However, in these exciting days of “consilience” among the sciences (Wilson, 1998), this issue is even more general, and transcends many disciplinary boundaries.

Type
Chapter
Information
Formative Experiences
The Interaction of Caregiving, Culture, and Developmental Psychobiology
, pp. 238 - 247
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2010

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Agran, P. F., Anderson, C., Winn, D., Trent, R., Walton-Haynes, L., & Thayer, S. (2003). Rates of pediatric injuries by 3-month intervals for children 0 to 3 years of age. Pediatrics, 111(6), e683–e692.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bard, K. A. (2000). Crying in infant primates: insights into the development of crying in chimpanzees. In Barr, R. G., Hopkins, B., & Green, J. A. (Eds.), Crying as a sign, a symptom, and a signal: Clinical, emotional and developmental aspects of infant and toddler crying (pp. 157–175). London: Mac Keith Press.Google Scholar
Barr, R. G. (1990). The normal crying curve: What do we really know?Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 32(4), 356–362.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barr, R. G. (2004). From infant crying to political action: What is the basic science of developmental-behavioral pediatrics?Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 25(2), 123–130.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barr, R. G. (2006). Crying behavior and its importance for psychosocial development in children. In Tremblay, R. E., Peters, R. D., Boivin, M., & Barr, R. G. (Eds.), Encyclopedia on early childhood development [Internet] (pp. 1–10). Montreal: Centre of Excellence for Early Childhood Development. Retrieved from http://www.child-encyclopedia.com/en-ca/home.htmlGoogle Scholar
Barr, R. G., Kobor, M., Boyce, W. T., & Robinson, W. (2008). Hours of contact in a sample of mothers and infants during the fifth week of life. Unpublished raw data.
Barr, R. G., Trent, R. B., & Cross, J. (2006). Age-related incidence curve of hospitalized shaken baby syndrome cases: convergent evidence for crying as a trigger to shaking. Child Abuse & Neglect, 30(1), 7–16.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barr, R. G., Warfield, J. J., & Catherine, N. L. A. (2005). Is there an ‘early distress peak’ in free ranging rhesus macaque monkeys? Poster presented at the Society for Research in Child Development, Atlanta, GA.
Keating, D. P., & Hertzman, C. (1999). Developmental health and the wealth of nations: Social, biological, and educational dynamics. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Lee, C., Barr, R. G., Catherine, N., & Wicks, A. (2007). Age-related incidence curve of publicly reported shaken baby syndrome cases: Is crying a trigger for shaking?Journal of Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics 28(4), 288–293.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maestripieri, D., Higley, J. D., Lindell, S. G., McCormack, K., Sanchez, M. M., & Newman, T. K. (2006). Early maternal rejection affects the development of monoaminergic systems and adult abusive parenting in rhesus macaques. Behavioral Neuroscience, 120(5), 1017–1024.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Runyan, D. K. (2008). The challenges of assessing the incidence of inflicted traumatic brain injury: A world perspective. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 34(4), S112–S115.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weaver, I. C. G., Cervoni, N., Champagne, F. A., D'Alessio, A. C., Sharma, S., Seckl, J. R., et al. (2004). Epigenetic programming by maternal behavior. Nature Neuroscience, 7(8), 847–854.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilson, E. O. (1998). Consilience: The unity of knowledge. New York: Knopf.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×