Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-4rdrl Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-06-17T13:03:16.725Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

18 - Phonosyntactics

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 August 2010

Leanne Hinton
Affiliation:
University of California, Berkeley
Johanna Nichols
Affiliation:
University of California, Berkeley
John J. Ohala
Affiliation:
University of California, Berkeley
Get access

Summary

Introduction

Although the search for linguistic universals has been successful in discovering a wide variety of similarities among the languages of the world (Greenberg 1966, 1978), few generalities about the semantic properties of languages have been uncovered (Weinreich 1963). Some of the most striking examples of a structured semantic domain are to be found in the connection between sound and meaning. In principle, resemblances between meaning and sound should not exist. As Greenberg (1957) notes, the connection between sound and meaning is essentially arbitrary. A meaning can theoretically be represented by almost any set of sounds in a language.

Despite claims that this principle applies to all languages, the articles in this volume show that connections between the meanings and the sounds of language do exist. Two universal and well-documented cases of sound symbolism are relevant to the present study. First, Ultan (1978) found evidence for distance symbolism. The notion of distance symbolism must be considered within the broader framework of deixis. Deixis refers to those features of the language which reflect the spatio-temporal coordinates of the relative situation of the utterance. Distinctions are made between things that are near in space and/or time versus things that are far in space and/or time. In an analysis of 136 languages, Ultan found that 33.1% of the sample exhibited distance (spatial) symbolism in their demonstrative system. More importantly, the languages that overtly symbolized distance relationships predominantly used front or high front vowels to represent proximity to the speaker. Ultan also found universal correspondences for size symbolism in language. Some languages overtly mark words expressing diminution by changing the phonological features of the sound in the root.

Type
Chapter
Information
Sound Symbolism , pp. 263 - 275
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 1995

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×