Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-v5vhk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-06-14T17:03:32.517Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

10 - Spatial language addressed to children

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 December 2010

Pierre R. Dasen
Affiliation:
Université de Genève
Ramesh C. Mishra
Affiliation:
Banaras Hindu University, Varanasi, India
Get access

Summary

The first language children hear in this world is the language of their parents, and generally that is the language they first acquire as their mother tongue. Language is largely a product of parent–child interaction, particularly in early years (Bates, Dale, & Thal, 1995; Harris, 1992; Snow, 1995). In later years other influences (e.g. peers, schooling) also shape language development (Bates, 1975). Much research in developmental linguistics has described the manner in which children acquire language and the way in which they grasp the meaning of concepts (Bowerman & Levinson, 2001). While the existence of “motherese” is no doubt universal, there are wide cultural as well as individual differences in its forms (Masataka, 2008).

One way to understand the processes underlying the development of a spatial frame of reference is to analyze the spatial language addressed by adults, especially mothers, to children in early years of life. Such analyses may permit an understanding of the language transmission processes. Given a situation in which mothers find an opportunity to talk to young children about the spatial location of objects or spatial events, it may be assumed that they would use the mode of spatial description predominant in the local context in ways that would be understandable to children. In other words, we assume that mothers will use their preferred spatial FoR in describing objects or situations by tailoring it to the level of understanding of the particular child.

Type
Chapter
Information
Development of Geocentric Spatial Language and Cognition
An Eco-cultural Perspective
, pp. 233 - 241
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2010

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×