Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-x4r87 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-29T15:33:17.303Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

10 - Mental Models and Deductive Reasoning

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 June 2012

Philip N. Johnson-Laird
Affiliation:
Princeton University
Jonathan E. Adler
Affiliation:
Brooklyn College, City University of New York
Lance J. Rips
Affiliation:
Northwestern University, Illinois
Get access

Summary

How do people reason? The view that I learned at my mother's knee was that they rely on logic. During the 1960s and 1970s when the study of thinking had become respectable again after the Dark Ages of Behaviorism, psychologists – including the present author – took this view for granted. The idea that logic provided the norms of reasoning can be traced back to the rise of modern logic, and was defended in the nineteenth century by both Boole (1854) and Mill (1874). In the twentieth century, the Swiss psychologist, Jean Piaget, and his colleagues argued that the construction of a formal logic in the mind was the last great step in children's intellectual development, and that it occurred at about the age of twelve (see, e.g., Inhelder and Piaget, 1958). And so thirty years ago the task for psychologists appeared to be to determine which particular formal logic was laid down in the mind and which particular rules of inference were used in its mental formulation. That, at least, was how several like-minded authors conceived their research (see, e.g., Osherson 1974–1976; Johnson-Laird 1975; Braine 1978; Rips 1983). In the parallel “universe” of artificial intelligence, researchers were similarly developing computer programs that proved theorems relying on formal rules of inference (e.g., Bledsoe 1977). The main skeptics were those engaged in trying to analyze everyday arguments. They discovered that it was extraordinarily difficult to translate such arguments into formal logic.

Type
Chapter
Information
Reasoning
Studies of Human Inference and its Foundations
, pp. 206 - 222
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2008

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Baddeley, A. D. (1986) Working Memory. Oxford: Clarendon Press.Google ScholarPubMed
Barres, P., and Johnson-Laird, P. N. (2003) On imagining what is true (and what is false). Thinking & Reasoning, 9, 1–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barrouillet, P., Grosset, N., and Leças, J. F. (2000) Conditional reasoning by mental models: Chronometric and developmental evidence. Cognition, 75, 237–266.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bauer, M. I., and Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1993) How diagrams can improve reasoning. Psychological Science, 4, 372–378.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bledsoe, W. W. (1977) Non-resolution theorem proving. Artificial Intelligence, 9, 1–35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boole, G. (1854) An Investigation of the Laws of Thought on Which are Founded the Mathematical Theories of Logic and Probabilities. London: Macmillan.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Braine, M. D. S. (1978) On the relation between the natural logic of reasoning and standard logic. Psychological Review, 85, 1–21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bucciarelli, M., and Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1999) Strategies in syllogistic reasoning. Cognitive Science, 23, 247–303.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bucciarelli, M., and Johnson-Laird, P. N. (2005) Naïve deontics: A theory of meaning, representation, and reasoning. Cognitive Psychology, 50, 159–193.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Byrne, R. M. J. (2005) The Rational Imagination: How People Create Alternatives to Reality. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Byrne, R. M. J., and Handley, S. J. (1997) Reasoning strategies for suppositional deductions. Cognition, 62, 1–49.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carreiras, M., and Santamaría, C. (1997) Reasoning about relations: Spatial and nonspatial problems. Thinking & Reasoning, 3, 191–208.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cheng, P., and Holyoak, K. J. (1985) Pragmatic reasoning schemas. Cognitive Psychology, 17, 391–416.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cosmides, L. (1989) The logic of social exchange: Has natural selection shaped how humans reason?Cognition, 31, 187–276.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Evans, J.St.B.T., Over, D. E., and Handley, S. J. (2005) Suppositions, extensionality, and conditionals: A critique of the mental model theory of Johnson-Laird & Byrne (2002). Psychological Review, 112, 1040–1052.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
García-Madruga, J. A., Moreno, S., Carriedo, N., Gutiérrez, F., and Johnson-Laird, P. N. (2001) Are conjunctive inferences easier than disjunctive inferences? A comparison of rules and models. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54A, 613–632.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Girotto, V., Mazzocco, A., and Tasso, A. (1997) The effect of premise order in conditional reasoning: A test of the mental model theory. Cognition, 63, 1–28.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldvarg, Y., and Johnson-Laird, P. N. (2001) Naïve causality: A mental model theory of causal meaning and reasoning. Cognitive Science, 25, 565–610.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodwin, G., and Johnson-Laird, P. N. (2005a) Reasoning about relations. Psychological Review, 112, 468–493.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodwin, G., and Johnson-Laird, P. N. (2005b) Reasoning about the relations between relations. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59, 1–23.Google Scholar
Goodwin, G., and Johnson-Laird, P. N. (2005c) Diagnosis of ambiguous faults in simple networks. Proceeding of the Twenty-Seventh Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Stresa, Italy. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. 791–796.Google Scholar
Inhelder, B., and Piaget, J. (1958) The Growth of Logical Thinking from Childhood to Adolescence. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jeffrey, R. (1981) Formal Logic: Its Scope and Limits. Second edition. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1975) Models of deduction. In Falmagne, R. J. (Ed.) Reasoning: Representation and Process in Children and Adults. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. pp. 7–54.Google Scholar
Johnson-Laird, P. N. (2001) Mental models and deduction. Trends in Cognitive Science, 5, 434–442.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnson-Laird, P. N. (2004) The history of mental models. In Manktelow, K., and Chung, M. C. (Eds.) Psychology of Reasoning: Theoretical and Historical Perspectives. New York: Psychology Press. pp. 179–212.Google Scholar
Johnson-Laird P. N. (2006) Mental models, sentential reasoning, and illusory inferences. In Held, C., Knauff, M. and Vosgerau, G. (Eds.) Mental Models: A Conception in the Intersection of Cognitive Psychology, Neuroscience and Philosophy. Berlin: Elsevier. pp. 27–52.Google Scholar
Johnson-Laird, P. N., and Byrne, R. M. J. (1991) Deduction. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Johnson-Laird, P. N., and Byrne, R. M. J. (2002) Conditionals: A theory of meaning, pragmatics, and inference. Psychological Review, 109, 646–678.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnson-Laird, P. N., and Hasson, U. (2003) Counterexamples in sentential reasoning. Memory & Cognition, 31, 1105–1113.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnson-Laird, P. N., Legrenzi, P., Girotto, P., and Legrenzi, M. S. (2000) Illusions in reasoning about consistency. Science, 288, 531–532.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnson-Laird, P. N., Legrenzi, P., Girotto, V., Legrenzi, M., and Caverni, J-P. (1999) Naive probability: A mental model theory of extensional reasoning. Psychological Review, 106, 62–88.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnson-Laird, , Legrenzi, P., and Sonino Legrenzi, M. (1972) Reasoning and a sense of reality. British Journal of Psychology, 63, 395–400.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson-Laird, P. N., and Savary, F. (1999) Illusory inferences: A novel class of erroneous deductions. Cognition, 71, 191–229.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, N. G. L., and Johnson-Laird, P. N. (2005a) Strategies in problem solving. Under submission.
Lee, N. G. L., and Johnson-Laird, P. N. (2005b) Synthetic reasoning and the reverse engineering of Boolean circuits. Proceeding of the Twenty-Seventh Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Stresa, Italy. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. 1260–1265.Google Scholar
Manktelow, K. I. and Over, D. E. (1995) Deontic reasoning. In Newstead, S. E., and Evans, J.St.B. T. (Eds.) Perspectives on Thinking and Reasoning: Essays in Honour of Peter Wason. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. pp. 91–114.Google Scholar
Mill, J. S. (1874) A System of Logic, Ratiocinative and Inductive: Being a Connected View of the Principles of Evidence and the Methods of Scientific Evidence. Eighth edition. New York: Harper. (First edition published 1843.)Google Scholar
Oaksford, M., and Chater, N. (1998) A revised rational analysis of the selection task: Exceptions and sequential sampling. In Oaksford, M. and Chater, N. (Eds.) Rational Models of Cognition. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Ormerod, T. C., and Richardson, J. (2003) On the generation and evaluation of inferences from single premises. Memory & Cognition, 31, 467–478.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Osherson, D. N. (1974–6) Logical Abilities in Children, Vols. 1–4. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Peirce, C. S. (1931–1958) Collected Papers of Charles Sanders Peirce. 8 vols. Hartshorne, C., Weiss, P., and Burks, A. (Eds.) Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Popper, K. (1959) The Logic of Scientific Discovery. London: Hutchinson.Google Scholar
Rips, L. J. (1983). Cognitive processes in propositional reasoning. Psychological Review, 90, 38–71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rips, L. (1994) The Psychology of Proof. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Schaeken, W. S., Johnson-Laird, P. N., Ydewalle, G. (1996) Mental models and temporal reasoning. Cognition, 60, 205–234.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scriven, M. (1976) Reasoning. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Stenning, K., and Yule, P. (1997) Image and language in human reasoning: A syllogistic illustration. Cognitive Psychology, 34, 109–159.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Toulmin, S. E. (1958) The Uses of Argument. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Henst, J.-B., Yang, Y., and Johnson-Laird, P. N. (2002) Strategies in sentential reasoning. Cognitive Science, 26, 425–468.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vandierendonck, A., Dierckx, V., and Vooght, G. (2004) Mental model construction in linear reasoning: Evidence for the construction of initial annotated models. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 57A, 1369–1391.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Walsh, C., and Johnson-Laird, P. N. (2004) Co-reference and reasoning. Memory & Cognition, 32, 96–106.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wason, P. C. (1966) Reasoning. In Foss, B. M. (Ed.) New Horizons in Psychology. Harmondsworth, Middx: Penguin.Google Scholar
Wason, P. C., and Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1972) The Psychology of Deduction: Structure and Content. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. London: Batsford.Google Scholar
Wason, P. C., and Shapiro, D. (1971) Natural and contrived experience in a reasoning problem. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 23, 63–71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×