Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-ttngx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-06-08T14:56:59.937Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

31 - Propensities and Counterfactuals: The Loser That Almost Won

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 June 2012

Daniel Kahneman
Affiliation:
Princeton University
Carol A. Varey
Affiliation:
University of British Columbia
Jonathan E. Adler
Affiliation:
Brooklyn College, City University of New York
Lance J. Rips
Affiliation:
Northwestern University, Illinois
Get access

Summary

The question of how people think of things that could have happened but did not has attracted increasing interest among psychologists in recent years (J. T. Johnson, 1986; Kahneman & Miller, 1986; Kahneman & Tversky, 1982a; Landman, 1987; D. T. Miller, Turnbull, & MacFarland, 1990; Wells & Gavanski, 1989; Wells, Taylor, & Turtle, 1987). As philosophers have long known, the study of counterfactuals cannot be separated from a conception of causality, and an understanding of causality requires a conception of possibility and conditional probability. Counterfactual assertions rest on causal beliefs, and causal attributions invoke counterfactual beliefs, for example, about what would have happened in the absence of a putative cause. Some counterfactual assertions assign degrees of probability or plausibility to unrealized outcomes, many causal beliefs are probabilistic, and judgments of probability often draw on impressions of causal tendencies or propensities. The present article is concerned with a psychological analysis of this nexus of issues.

Our study began with an attempt to understand the psychology of assertions of the form “X almost happened,” which we call close counterfactuals. An important characteristic of such assertions is that they are not expressed as a conditional with a specified antecedent, as counterfactual conditionals are. The close counterfactual does not invoke an alternative possible world, but states a fact about the history of this world – namely, that things were close to turning out differently than they did.

Type
Chapter
Information
Reasoning
Studies of Human Inference and its Foundations
, pp. 639 - 651
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2008

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Adams, E. W. (1976). Prior probabilities and counterfactual conditionals. In Harper, W. L. and Hooker, C. A. (Eds.), Foundations of probability theory, statistical inference, and statistical theories of science (Vol. 1, pp. 1–21). Dordrecht, Holland: Reidel.Google Scholar
Ajzen, I. (1977). Intuitive theories of events and the effects of base-rate information on prediction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 35, 303–314.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bar-Hillel, M. (1990). Back to base rates. In Hogarth, R. M. (Ed.), Insights in decision making: A tribute to Hillel J. Einhoorn (pp. 200–216). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Black, J. B., and Bower, G. H. (1979). Episodes as chunks in narrative memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 18, 187–198.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Braine, M. D. S. (1978). On the relation between the natural logic of reasoning and standard logic. Psychological Review, 85, 1–21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ducasse, C. J. (1969). Causation and the types of necessity. New York: Dover.Google Scholar
Einhorn, H., and Hogarth, R. (1986). Judging probable cause. Psychological Bulletin, 99, 3–19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fielding, H. (1975). The History of Tom Jones, a foundling (Vol. 2). Oxford, England: Wesleyan University Press/Oxford University Press. (Original work published 1749.)Google Scholar
Fischhoff, B. (1975). Hindsight ≉ foresight: The effect of outcome knowledge on judgment under uncertainty. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 1, 288–299.Google Scholar
Fischhoff, B. (1982). For those condemned to study the past. In Kahneman, D., Slovic, P., and Tversky, A. (Eds.), Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases (pp. 422–444). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Freyd, J., and Finke, R. (1984). Representational momentum. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 10, 126–132.Google Scholar
Gigerenzer, G., Hell, W., and Blank, H. (1988). Presentation and content: The use of base rates as a continuous variable. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 14, 513–525.Google Scholar
Gilovich, T., Vallone, R., and Tversky, A. (1985). The hot hand in basketball: On the misperception of random sequences. Cognitive Psychology, 17, 295–314.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodman, N. (1954). Fact, fiction and forecast (1st ed.). London: Athlone Press.Google Scholar
Goodman, N. (1983). Fact, fiction, and forecast (4th ed.). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Hart, H. L. A., and Honore, A. M. (1959). Causation in the law. London: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Heider, F. (1958). The psychology of interpersonal relations. New York: Wiley.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heider, F., and Simmel, M. (1944). An experimental study of apparent behavior. American Journal of Psychology, 57, 243–259.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hofstadter, D. (1979). Gödel, Escher, Bach: An eternal golden braid. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Johnson, J. T. (1986). The knowledge of what might have been: Affective and attributional consequences of near outcomes. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 12, 51–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, J. T., Ogawa, K. H., Delforge, A., and Early, D. (1989). Causal primacy and comparative fault: The effect of position in a causal chain on judgments of legal responsibility. Personality and Social Psychology, 15, 161–174.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, M. K. (1988). Discriminating the origin of information. In Oltmanns, T. F. and Maher, B. A. (Eds.), Delusional beliefs: Interdisciplinary perspectives (pp. 34–65). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1983). Mental models: Towards a cognitive science of language, inference, and consciousness. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Kahneman, D ., and Miller, D. T. (1986). Norm theory: Comparing reality to its alternatives. Psychological Review, 93, 136–153.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kahnemen, D., Slovic, P., and Tversky, A. (Eds.). (1982). Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kahneman, D., and Tversky, A. (1973). On the psychology of prediction. Psychological Review, 80, 237–251.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kahneman, D., and Tversky, A. (1982a). The simulation heuristic. In Kahneman, D., Slovic, P., and Tversky, A. (Eds.), Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases (pp. 201–208). New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kahneman, D., and Tversky, A. (1982b). Variants of uncertainty. Cognition, 11, 143–157.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kelley, H. H. (1967). Attribution theory in social psychology. In Levine, D. (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation (Vol. 15, pp. 192–240). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Keren, G., and Wagenaar, W. A. (1985). On the psychology of playing blackjack: Normative and descriptive considerations with implications for decision theory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 114, 133–158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kintsch, W., and Dijk, T. A. (1978). Toward a model of text comprehension and production. Psychological Review, 85, 363–384.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kvart, I. (1986). A theory of counterfactuals. Indianapolis: Hackett.Google Scholar
Kvart, I. (1989). Causal independence. Unpublished manuscript.Google Scholar
Lakoff, G. (1987). Women, fire, and dangerous things: What categories reveal about the mind. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.CrossRef
Lakoff, G., and Johnson, M. (1980). Metaphors we live by. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Landman, J. (1987). Regret and elation following action and inaction. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 13, 524–536.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewin, K. (1936). Principles of topological psychology (F. Heider & G. M. Heider, Trans). New York: McGraw-Hill.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewis, D. (1973). Counterfactuals. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Lewis, D. (1979). Counterfactual dependence and time's arrow. Noûs, 13, 455–476.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lyons, J. (1977). Semantics, Volume 2. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Mackie, J. L. (1974). The cement of the universe: A study of causation. Oxford, England: Clarendon Press (Oxford University Press).Google Scholar
Michotte, A. E. (1946). La perception de la causalité [The perception of causality]. Louvain, Belgium: University of Louvain Publications.Google Scholar
Miller, D. T., and Gunasegaram, S. (1990). Temporal order and the perceived mutability of events: Implications for blame assignment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 1111–1118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, D. T., Turnbull, W., and McFarland, C. (1990). Counterfactual thinking and social perception: Thinking about what might have been. In Zanna, M. (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 23, pp. 305–331). Orlando, FL: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Miller, G. A., and Johnson-Laird, P. L. (1976). Language and perception. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nute, D. (1980). Topics in conditional logic. Boston: Reidel.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pennington, N., and Hastie, R. (1988). Explanation-based decision making: Effects of memory structure on judgment. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 14, 521–533.Google Scholar
Pollock, J. L. (1976). Subjunctive reasoning. Dordrecht, Holland: Reidel.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Read, S. J. (1987). Constructing causal scenarios: A knowledge structure approach to causal reasoning. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 288–302.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rips, L. (1990). Reasoning. Annual Review of Psychology, 41, 321–353.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rumelhart, D. E. (1977). Understanding and summarizing brief stories. In Laberge, D. and Samuels, J. (Eds.), Basic processes in reading, perception and comprehension (pp. 265–303). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Schank, R. C. (1975). The structure of episodes in memory. In Bobrow, D. G. and Collins, A. (Eds.), Representation and understanding: Studies in cognitive science (pp. 237–272). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Schank, R. C., and Abelson, R. P. (1977). Scripts, plans, goals, and understanding. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Shafer, G. (1976). A mathematical theory of evidence. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Skyrms, B. (1980). The prior propensity account of subjunctive conditionals. In Harper, W. L., Stalnaker, R., and Pearce, G. (Eds.), Ifs: Conditionals, belief, decision, chance and time (Vol. 1, pp. 125–162). Dordrecht, Holland: Reidel.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stalnaker, R. C. (1968). A theory of conditionals. In Rescher, N. (Ed.), Studies in logical theory: American Philosophical Quarterly monograph series 2 (pp. 98–112). Oxford, England: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Talmy, L. (1981, May). Force images. Paper presented at the Conference on Language and Mental Imagery, University of California, Berkeley.Google Scholar
Talmy, L. (1983). How language structures space. In Pick, H. & Acredolo, L. (Eds.), Spatial orientation: Theory, research, and application (pp. 219–282). New York: Plenum Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trabasso, T., Secco, T., and van den Broek, P. (1984). Causal cohesion and story coherence. In Mandl, H., Stein, N. L., and Trabasso, T. (Eds.), Learning and comprehension of text (pp. 83–111). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Tversky, A., and Kahneman, D. (1980). Causal schemas in judgments under uncertainty. In Fishbein, M. (Ed.), Progress in social psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 153–160). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Tversky, A., and Kahneman, D. (1982). Evidential impact of base rates. In Kahneman, D., Slovic, P., and Tversky, A. (Eds.), Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases (pp. 201–208). New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vendler, Z. (1967). Linguistics in philosophy. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press.Google Scholar
Wells, G. L., and Gavanski, I. (1989). Mental simulation of causality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 161–169.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wells, G. L, Taylor, B. R., and Turtle, J. W. (1987). The undoing of scenarios. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 421–430.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×