Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-wg55d Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-15T02:22:53.900Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

4 - The behavioural activation system: challenges and opportunities

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  31 January 2011

Alan D. Pickering
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of London
Luke D. Smillie
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of London
Philip J. Corr
Affiliation:
University of Wales, Swansea
Get access

Summary

The influence of Reinforcement Sensitivity Theory (RST) on personality psychology has been far greater than Jeffrey Gray would ever have conceived. This is especially the case for the Behavioural Activation System (or Behavioural Approach System) (BAS), which was introduced to his model on a somewhat arbitrary basis, only to be embraced by others as a major organizing framework for trait explanation. In the last few years, RST has been substantially revised in a number of aspects (as discussed in other chapters of this book), particularly centring on the role of the Behavioural Inhibition System (BIS) and Fight-Flight-Freeze System (FFFS). This might give the impression that the changes in thinking about the BAS have been relatively minor. We would argue that this impression is misleading and that an equally radical review of the BAS is called for (as we have been arguing in a series of articles: Pickering and Gray 1999, 2001; Pickering 2004; Smillie, Pickering and Jackson 2006). In this chapter we highlight the opportunities ahead at this time of theoretical flux, sketch some of the challenges to be met by future research, and suggest what we believe may be some promising initial steps in this process.

A time for rethinking the BAS within RST

Like Gray's other brain-behavioural systems, such as the BIS, the BAS was originally defined in ethological terms (see Gray 1987c).

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2008

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Ashby, F. G., Alfonso-Reese, L. A., Tuken, A. U. and Wladron, E. M. (1998), A neuropsychological theory of multiple systems in category learning, Psychological Review, 105, 442–481CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ashby, F. G., Maddox, W. T. and Bohil, C. J. (2002), Observational versus feedback training in rule-based and information-integration category learning, Memory and Cognition, 30, 666–677CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ball, S. A. and Zuckerman, M. (1990), Sensation seeking, Eysenck's personality dimensions and reinforcement sensitivity in concept formation, Personality and Individual Differences, 11, 343–353CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barratt, E. S. (1985), Impulsiveness subtraits: arousal and information processing in Spence, J. T. and Izard, C. E. (eds), Motivation, Emotion, and Personality (New York: Elsevier Science Publishers), pp. 137–146Google Scholar
Barratt, E. S. (1994), Impulsiveness and aggression in Monohan, J. and Steadman, H. J. (eds), Violence and Mental Disorder (Chicago: University of Chicago Press), pp. 61–79Google Scholar
Bartussek, D., Diedrich, O., Naumann, E. and Collet, W. (1993), Introversion-extraversion and event-related potentials (event-related potential): a test of J. A. Gray's theory, Personality and Individual Differences, 14, 565–574CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Block, J. (1995), A contrarian view of the five-factor approach to personality description, Psychological Bulletin, 117, 187–215CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boddy, J., Carver, A. and Rowley, K. (1986), Effects of positive and negative verbal reinforcement on performance as a function of extraversion-introversion: some tests of Gray's theory, Personality and Individual Differences, 7, 81–88CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, J., Bullock, D. and Grossberg, S. (1999), How the basal ganglia use parallel excitatory and inhibitory learning pathways to selectively respond to unexpected rewarding cues, Journal of Neuroscience, 19, 10502–10511CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, R. G. and Marsden, C. D. (1988), Internal versus external cues and the control of attention in Parkinson's disease, Brain, 111, 323–345CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carver, C. S. (2004), Negative affects deriving from the behavioural approach system, Emotion, 4, 3–22CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carver, C. S. and White, T. L. (1994), Behavioural inhibition, behavioural activation, and affective responses to impending reward and punishment: the Behavioural Inhibition System/Behavioural Approach System scales, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 319–333CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caseras, X., Ávila, C. and Torrubia, R. (2003), The measurement of individual differences in behavioural inhibition and behavioural activation systems: a comparison of personality scales, Personality and Individual Differences, 34, 999–1013CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cattell, R. B. (1950), Personality: a Systematic Theoretical and Factual Study, (New York, NY: McGraw-Hill)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chico, E., Tous, J. M., Lorenzo-Seva, U. and Vigil-Colet, A. (2003), Spanish adaptation of Dickman's impulsivity inventory: its relationship to Eysenck's personality questionnaire, Personality and Individual Differences, 35, 1883–1892CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cloninger, C. R. (1987), A systematic method for clinical description and classification of personality variants, Archives of General Psychiatry, 44, 573–588CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cloninger, C. R., Svrakic, D. M. and Przybeck, T. R. (1993), A psychobiological model of temperament and character, Archives of General Psychiatry, 50, 975–990CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, M. X., Young, J., Baek, J. M., Kessler, C. and Ranganath, C. (2005), Individual differences in extraversion and dopamine genetics reflect reactivity of neural reward circuitry, Cognitive Brain Research, 25, 851–861CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Corr, P. J. (2002), J. A. Gray's reinforcement sensitivity theory and frustrative nonreward: a theoretical note on expectancies in reactions to rewarding stimuli, Personality and Individual Differences, 32, 1247–1253CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Corr, P. J., Pickering, A. D. and Gray, J. A. (1995), Personality and reinforcement in associative and instrumental learning, Personality and Individual Differences, 19, 47–71CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coscina, D. V. (1997), The biopsychology of impulsivity: focus on brain serotonin in , C. D. Webster and , M. A. Jackson (eds), Impulsivity: Theory, Assessment and Treatment (London: Guilford Press), pp. 95–115Google Scholar
Crowe, E. and Higgins, E. T. (1997), Regulatory focus and strategic inclinations: promotion and prevention in decision-making, Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 69(2), 117–132CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dawe, S., Gullo, M. and Loxton, N. J. (2004), Reward drive and rash impulsiveness as dimensions of impulsivity: implications for substance misuse, Addictive Behaviours, 29, 1389–1406CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dawe, S. and Loxton, N. J. (2004), The role of impulsivity in the development of substance use and eating disorders, Neuroscience and Biobehavioural Reviews, 28, 343–351CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Depue, R. A. and Collins, P. F. (1999), Neurobiology of the structure of personality: dopamine, facilitation of incentive motivation, and extraversion, Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 22, 491–569CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Depue, R. and Iacono, W. (1989), Neurobehavioral aspects of affective disorders, Annual Review of Psychology, 40, 457–492CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Depue, R. A. and Morrone-Strupinsky, J. V. (2005), A neurobehavioral model of affiliative bonding: implications for conceptualizing a human trait of affiliation, Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28, 313–395CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Derryberry, D. and Tucker, D. M. (1991), The adaptive base of the neural hierarchy: elementary motivational controls on network function in Dienstbier, R. (ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, vol. 38, Perspectives on Motivation (Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press), pp. 289–342Google Scholar
Dickman, S. J. (1990), Functional and dysfunctional impulsivity: personality and cognitive correlates, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 95–102CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dickman, S. J.(2000), Impulsivity, arousal and attention, Personality and Individual Differences, 28, 563–581CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elliot, A. J. and Thrash, T. M. (2002), Approach-avoidance motivation in personality: approach avoidance temperaments and goals, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 804–818CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Evenden, J. L. (1999), Varieties of impulsivity, Psychopharmacology, 146, 348–361CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eysenck, H. J. (1967), The Biological Basis of Personality (Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas)Google Scholar
Eysenck, H. J., Barrett, P., Wilson, G. D. and Jackson, C. J. (1992), Primary trait measurement of the 21 components of the P-E-N System, European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 8, 109–117Google Scholar
Eysenck, S. B. G., Pearson, P. R., Easting, G. and Allsopp, J. F. (1985), Age norms for impulsiveness, venturesomeness and empathy in adults, Personality and Individual Differences, 6, 613–619CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fowles, D. C. (1993), Biological variables in psychopathology: a psychobiological perspective in Sutker, P. B. and Adams, H. E. (eds), Comprehensive Handbook of Psychopathology (2nd edn, New York: Plenum Press), p. 5782CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldberg, L. R. (1982), From ace to zombie: some explorations in the language of personality in , C. D. Spielberger and , J. N. Butcher (eds), Advances in Personality Assessment (Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum), vol. 1, pp. 203–234Google Scholar
Gomez, R. and McLaren, S. (1997), The effects of reward and punishment on response disinhibition, moods, heart rate and skin conductance level during instrumental learning, Personality and Individual Differences, 23, 305–316CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gray, J. A. (1970), The psychophysiological basis of introversion-extraversion, Behaviour Research and Therapy, 8, 249–266CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gray, J. A.(1973), Causal models of personality and how to test them in Royce, J. R. (ed.), Multivariate Analysis and Psychological Theory (London: Academic Press), pp. 409–463Google Scholar
Gray, J. A.(1975), Elements of a Two-Process Theory of Learning (London: Academic Press)Google Scholar
Gray, J. A.(1981), A critique of Eysenck's theory of personality in Eysenck, H. J. (ed.), A Model for Personality (Berlin: Springer), pp. 246–276Google Scholar
Gray, J. A.(1987a), Perspectives on anxiety and impulsivity: a commentary, Journal of Research in Personality, 21, 493–509CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gray, J. A. (1987b), The neuropsychology of emotion and personality in Stahl, S. M., Iversen, S. D. and Goodman, E. G. (eds), Cognitive Neurochemistry (Oxford: Oxford University Press), pp. 171–190Google Scholar
Gray, J. A.(1987c), The Psychology of Fear and Stress (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press)Google Scholar
Gray, J. A. (1994), Framework for a taxonomy of psychiatric disorder in Goozen, S. H. M., Poll, N. E. and Sergeant, J. A. (eds), Essays on Emotion Theory (Hove: Lawrence Erlbaum), pp. 29–59Google Scholar
Gray, J. A. and McNaughton, N. (2000), The Neuropsychology of Anxiety (Oxford: Oxford University Press)Google Scholar
Gray, J. A., Owen, S., Davis, N. and Tsaltas, E. (1983), Psychological and physiological relations between anxiety and impulsivity in Zuckerman, M. (ed.), The Biological Bases of Sensation Seeking, Impulsivity, and Anxiety (Hillsdale: Earlbaum), pp. 189–217Google Scholar
Gray, J. A. and Smith, P. T. (1969), An arousal-decision model for partial reinforcement and discrimination learning in Gilbert, R. M. and Sutherland, N. S. (eds), Animal Discrimination Learning (London: Academic Press), pp. 243–272Google Scholar
Gray, N. S., Pickering, A. D. and Gray, J. A. (1994), Psychoticism and dopamine D2 binding in the basal ganglia using single photon emission tomography, Personality and Individual Differences, 17, 431–434CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gupta, S. (1990), Impulsivity sociability and reinforcement in verbal operant conditioning: a replication, Personality and Individual Differences, 11, 585–589CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Higgins, E. T. (1987), Self-discrepancy: a theory relating self and affect, Psychological Review, 94, 319–340CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Higgins, E. T.(1997), Beyond pleasure and pain, American Psychologist, 52, 1280–1300CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jackson, C. J. and Smillie, L. D.(2000), Making a good decision: value from fit, American Psychologist, 55(11), 1217–1230Google Scholar
Jackson, C. J. (2001), Comparison between Eysenck and Gray's models of personality in the prediction of motivational work criteria, Personality and Individual Differences, 31, 129–144CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jackson, C. J. and Smillie, L. D. (2004), Appetitive motivation predicts the majority of personality and an ability measure: a comparison of Behavioural Approach System measures and a re-evaluation of the importance of RST, Personality and Individual Differences, 36, 1627–1636CrossRefGoogle Scholar
John, O. P., Donahue, E. M. and Kentle, R. L. (1991), The Big Five Inventory: Version 4a and 5 (Technical report, Berkeley, CA: Institute of Personality and Social Research, University of California)Google Scholar
Knyazev, G. G., Slobodskaya, H. R. and Wilson, G. D. (2004), Comparison of the construct validity of the Gray-Wilson personality questionnaire and the Behavioural Inhibition System/Behavioural Approach System scales, Personality and Individual Differences, 37, 1565–1582CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Knowlton, B. J., Mangels, J. A. and Squire, L. R. (1996), A neostriatal habit learning system in humans, Science, 273, 1399–1402CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kruschke, J. K. (2005), Category learning in , K. Lamberts and , R. L. Goldstone (eds), The Handbook of Cognition (London: Sage), pp. 183–201Google Scholar
Lessiter, J. (1999), A Dopaminergic Model of Reward Motivation: A Test of Implications for Personality and Schizophrenia (Doctoral thesis, London: University of London)Google Scholar
Markman, A. B., Baldwin, G. C. and Maddox, W. T. (2005), The interaction of payoff structure and regulatory focus in classification, Psychological Science, 16, 852–855CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martin, L. E. and Potts, G. F. (2004), Reward sensitivity in impulsivity, Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuropsychology, 15, 1519–1522Google ScholarPubMed
Mason, O., Claridge, G., and Jackson, M. (1995), New scales for the assessment of schizotypy, Personality and Individual Differences, 18, 7–13CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Parker, J. D. A. and Bagby, R. M. (1997), Impulsivity in adults: a critical review of measurement approaches in Webster, C. D. and Jackson, M. A. (eds), Impulsivity: Theory, Assessment and Treatment (London: Guilford Press), pp. 142–157Google Scholar
Patton, J. H., Stanford, M. S. and Barratt, E. S. (1995), Factor structure of the Barratt impulsiveness scale, Journal of Clinical Psychology, 51, 768–7743.0.CO;2-1>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pickering, A. D. (1999), Personality correlates of the dopaminergic facilitation of incentive motivation: impulsive sensation seeking rather than extraversion?, Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 22, 534–535CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pickering, A. D.(2004), The neuropsychology of Impulsive antisocial sensation seeking personality traits: from dopamine to hippocampal function? in , R. M. Stelmack (ed.), On the Psychobiology of Personality: Essays in Honour of Marvin Zuckerman (Oxford: Elsevier Ltd), pp. 453–476Google Scholar
Pickering, A. D., Corr, P. J., Powell, J. H., Kumari, V., Thornton, J. C. and Gray, J. A. (1997), Individual differences in reactions to reinforcing stimuli are neither black nor white: to what extent are they Gray? in , H. Nyborg (ed.), The Scientific Study of Human Nature: Tribute to Hans J. Eysenck at Eighty (London: Elsevier Sciences), pp. 36–67Google Scholar
Pickering, A. D., Díaz, A. and Gray, J. A. (1995), Personality and reinforcement: an exploration using a maze-learning task, Personality and Individual Differences, 18, 541–558CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pickering, A. D. and Gray, J. A. (1999), The neuroscience of personality in , L. Pervin and , O. John (eds), Handbook of Personality (2nd edn, New York: Guilford Press), pp. 277–299Google Scholar
Pickering, A. D. and Gray, J. A. (2001), Dopamine, appetitive reinforcement, and the neuropsychology of human learning: an individual differences approach in , A. Eliasz and , A. Angleitner (eds), Advances in Individual Differences Research (Lengerich, Germany: PABST Science Publishers), pp. 113–149Google Scholar
Quilty, L. C. and Oakman, J. M. (2004), The assessment of behavioural activation: the relationship between impulsivity and behaviour activation, Personality and Individual Differences, 37, 429–442CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reuter, M., Schmitz, A., Corr, P. and Hennig, J. (2005), Molecular genetics support Gray's personality theory: the interaction of COMT and DRD2 polymorphisms predicts the behavioural approach system, International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology, 1, 1–12Google Scholar
Revelle, W. (1995), Personality processes, Annual Review of Psychology, 6, 295–328CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Revelle, W.(1997), Extraversion and impulsivity: the last dimension in Nyborg, H. (ed.), The Scientific Study of Human Nature: Tribute to Hans J. Eysenck at Eighty (Elsevier Science Press), pp. 189–212Google Scholar
Revelle, W., Humphreys, M. S., Simon, L. and Gilliland, K. (1980), The interactive effect of personality, time of day, and caffeine: a test of the arousal model, Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 109, 1–31CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schultz, W. (1998), Predictive reward signal of dopamine neurons, Journal of Neurophysiology, 80, 1–27CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Senuath, O. M. (1975), Personality, reinforcement and learning, Percept Motor Skills, 41, 459–463CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smillie, L. D. (2005), Trait Representation of J. A. Gray's Behavioural Activation System: Distinguishing between Reward-Reactivity and Impulsivity (Doctoral Thesis, University of Queensland)Google Scholar
Smillie, L. D., Dalgleish, L. I. and Jackson, C. J. (2007), Distinguishing between learning and motivation in behavioural tests of the reinforcement sensitivity theory of personality, Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33, 476–489CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smillie, L. D. and Jackson, C. J. (2005), The appetitive motivation scale and other Behavioural Approach System measures in the prediction of approach and active avoidance, Personality and Individual Differences, 38, 981–994CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smillie, L. D. and Jackson, C. J.(2006), Functional impulsivity and reinforcement sensitivity theory, Journal of Personality, 74, 47–83CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smillie, L. D., Jackson, C. J. and Dalgleish, L. I. (2006), Conceptual distinctions among Carver and White's (1994), Behavioural Approach System scales: a reward-reactivity versus trait impulsivity perspective, Personality and Individual Differences, 40, 1039–1050CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smillie, L. D., Pickering, A. D. and Jackson, C. J. (2006), The new reinforcement sensitivity theory: implications for personality measurement, Personality and Social Psychology Review, 10, 320–335CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Squire, L. R. (1992), Memory and the hippocampus: a synthesis from findings with rats, monkeys, and humans, Psychological Review, 99, 195–231CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stewart, G. L. (1996), Reward structure as a moderator of the relationship between extraversion and sales performance, Journal of Applied Psychology, 81, 619–627CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thayer, R. E. (1989), The Biopsychology of Mood and Arousal (New York: Oxford University Press)
Torrubia, R., Ávila, C., Caseras, X. and Moltó, J. (2001), The sensitivity to punishment and sensitivity to reward questionnaire (SPSRQ) as a measure of Gray's anxiety and impulsivity dimensions, Personality and Individual Differences, 31, 837–862CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Venables, P. H., Wilkins, S., Mitchell, D. A., Raine, A. and Bailes, K. (1990), A scale for the measurement of schizotypy, Personality and Individual Differences, 11, 481–495CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wacker, J., Chavanon, M. and Stemmler, G. (2006), Investigating the dopaminergic basis of extraversion in humans: a multilevel approach, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 171–187CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Watson, D. and Clark, L. A. (eds) (1994), Personality and Psychopathology, Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103 [Special Issue].
Wickens, J. and Kotter, R. (1995), Cellular models of reinforcement in , J. C. Houk, , J. L. Davis, , D. G. Beiser (eds), Models of Information Processing in the Basal Ganglia (London: MIT Press), pp. 189–214Google Scholar
Wilson, G. D., Barrett, P. T. and Gray, J. A. (1989), Human reactions to reward and punishment: a questionnaire examination of Gray's personality theory, British Journal of Psychology, 80, 509–515CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilson, G. D., Gray, J. A. and Barrett, P. T. (1990), A factor analysis of the Gray-Wilson personality questionnaire, Personality and Individual Differences, 11, 1037–1045CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zelenski, J. M. and Larsen, R. J. (1999), Susceptibility to affect: a comparison of three personality taxonomies, Journal of Personality, 67, 761–791CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zinbarg, R. and Mohlman, J. (1998), Individual differences in the acquisition of affectively valenced associations, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1024–1040CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zinbarg, R. and Revelle, W. (1989), Personality and conditioning: a test of four models, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 301–314CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zuckerman, M. (1979), Sensation Seeking: Beyond the Optimal Level of Arousal (Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum)Google Scholar
Zuckerman, M.(1991), Psychobiology of Personality (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press)Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×