Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-pftt2 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-03T23:45:34.012Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

8 - The social context of chimpanzee tool use

from Part III - Ecology and culture

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 March 2013

Crickette M. Sanz
Affiliation:
Washington University, Department of Anthropology
David B. Morgan
Affiliation:
Lester E. Fisher Center for the Study and Conservation of Apes, Lincoln Park Zoo Wildlife Conservation Society, Congo
Crickette M. Sanz
Affiliation:
Washington University, St Louis
Josep Call
Affiliation:
Max-Planck-Institut für Evolutionäre Anthropologie, Germany
Christophe Boesch
Affiliation:
Max-Planck-Institut für Evolutionäre Anthropologie, Germany
Get access

Summary

Although several animal species exhibit some form of tool use (Shumaker et al., 2011), there are relatively few animals which flexibly use a diverse repertoire of implements on a regular basis within their natural environments. As shown in this volume, hominins, chimpanzees, orangutans, some capuchins and corvids are the exceptions. Van Schaik et al. (1999) have suggested that the evolution of material culture in primates is dependent upon the intersection of four primary factors: manipulative skills, cognitive abilities, suitable ecological niches and social tolerance. Although one cannot entirely dismiss the possibility of differences in manipulative skills and cognitive abilities within species, there are intriguing differences in diversity and types of tool use among populations of wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) which are not entirely explicable by environmental circumstances, and so have been attributed to social influences (Whiten et al., 1999, 2001; Möbius et al., 2008). With the exception of a few developmental studies, the social context of tool use remains largely unexplored in these apes.

Primates show differing degrees of social cohesion, and varying responses to fluctuations in the availability of resources in their environment. The abundance and distribution of important food resources dictates not only population density, but also social tolerance. Social tolerance varies across species, but may also differ within species (among populations, groups and even individuals). Resource scarcity may incite feeding competition among conspecifics, which could cause primates to avoid spending time in close proximity, whereas bountiful resources may attract conspecifics to forage at the same site. Social tolerance in gregarious foraging could enhance social learning by allowing primates to forage in close proximity to each other, providing a relaxed social atmosphere in which attention may be focused on a task, and enabling subordinate individuals to participate in close proximity foraging without risk of theft or aggression by conspecifics (van Schaik, 2003). The “opportunities for social learning hypothesis” predicts that higher degrees of social tolerance should result in a larger number of customary technical skills exhibited by primates (van Schaik, 2003). An extension of the opportunities for social learning hypothesis is that higher degrees of social tolerance could be associated with the transmission of more complex tool behaviors. Pradhan et al. (2012) have proposed that variation in sociability accounts for intraspecific and interspecific differences in the simple and cumulative technology of chimpanzees and orangutans.

Type
Chapter
Information
Tool Use in Animals
Cognition and Ecology
, pp. 161 - 175
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2013

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Anderson, D. P., Nordheim, E. V., Boesch, C. & Moermond, T. C. (2002). Factors influencing fission-fusion grouping in chimpanzees in the Taï National Park, Cote d’Ivoire. In Boesch, C., Hohmann, G. & Marchant, L. F. (eds.) Behavioral Diversity in Chimpanzees and Bonobos (pp. 90–101). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barthélemy, M., Barrat, A., Pastor-Satorras, R. & Vespignani, A. (2005). Characterization and modeling of weighted networks. Physica A, 346, 34–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boccaletti, S., Latora, V., Moreno, Y., Chavez, M. & Hwang, D. U. (2006). Complex networks: structure and dynamics. Physics Reports, 424, 175–308.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boesch, C. (1991). Teaching among wild chimpanzees. Animal Behaviour, 41, 530–532.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boesch, C. (1996). Social grouping in Taï chimpanzees. In McGrew, W. C., Marchant, L. F. & Nishida, T. (eds.) Great Ape Societies (pp.101–113). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boesch, C. (2003). Is culture a golden barrier between human and chimpanzee?Evolutionary Anthropology, 12, 82–91.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boesch, C. and Boesch, H. (1984). Mental map in wild chimpanzees: an analysis of hammer transports for nut cracking. Primates, 25, 160–170.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boesch, C. and Boesch-Achermann, H. (2000). The Chimpanzees of the Taï Forest: Behavioural Ecology and Evolution. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Boesch, C., Head, J. & Robbins, M. M. (2009). Complex tool sets for honey extraction among chimpanzees in Loango National Park, Gabon. Journal of Human Evolution, 56(6), 560–569.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cairns, S. J. & Schwager, S. J. (1987). A comparison of association indices. Animal Behaviour, 35, 1454–1469.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caro, T. M. & Hauser, M. D. (1992). Is there teaching in nonhuman animals?Quarterly Review of Biology, 67(2), 151–174.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chapman, C. A., Chapman, L. J. & Wrangham, R. W. (1995). Ecological constraints on group size: an analysis of spider monkey and chimpanzee subgroups. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 36, 59–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coussi-Korbel, S. & Fragaszy, D. M. (1995). On the relation between social dynamics and social learning. Animal Behaviour, 50, 1441–1453.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Franks, N. R. & Richardson, T. (2006). Teaching in tandem-running in ants. Nature, 439, 153.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ginsberg, J. R. & Young, T. P. (1992). Measuring association between individuals or groups in behavioural studies. Animal Behaviour, 44, 377–379.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodall, J. (1986). The Chimpanzees of Gombe: Patterns of Behavior. Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press.Google Scholar
Halperin, S. (1979). Temporary association patterns in free ranging chimpanzees: an assessment of individual grouping preferences. In Hamburg, D. A. & McCown, E. (eds.) The Great Apes (pp. 491–499). Menlo Park, CA: Benjamin/Cummings.Google Scholar
Holme, P., Park, S. M., Kim, B. J. & Edling, C. R. (2007). Korean university life in a network perspective: dynamics of a large affiliation network. Physica a-Statistical Mechanics and Its Applications, 373, 821–830.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hopper, L. M., Spiteri, A., Lambeth, S. P., et al. (2007). Experimental studies of traditions and underlying transmission processes in chimpanzees. Animal Behaviour, 73, 1021–1032.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoppitt, W. J. E., Brown, G. R., Kendal, R., et al. (2008). Lessons from animal teaching. Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 23(9), 486–493.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Horner, V. (2010). The cultural mind of chimpanzees: how social tolerance can shape the transmission of culture. In Lonsdorf, E. V., Ross, S. R. & Matsuzawa, T. (eds.) The Mind of the Chimpanzee: Ecological and Experimental Perspectives (pp. 116–126). Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Humle, T., Snowdon, C. T. & Matsuzawa, T. (2009). Social influences on ant-dipping acquisition in the wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes verus) of Bossou, Guinea, West Africa. Animal Cognition, 12, S37–S48.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kitahara-Frisch, J. & Norikoshi, K. (1982). Spontaneous sponge-making in captive chimpanzees. Journal of Human Evolution, 11(1), 41–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lonsdorf, E. V. (2006). What is the role of mothers in the acquisition of termite-fishing behaviors in wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii)?Animal Cognition, 9, 36–46.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lusseau, D. (2003). The emergent properties of a dolphin social network. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 270, S186–S188.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lusseau, D., Wilson, B., Hammond, P. S., et al. (2006). Quantifying the influence of sociality on population structure in bottlenose dolphins. Journal of Animal Ecology, 75(1), 14–24.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McGrew, W. C. (1977). Socialization and object-manipulation of wild chimpanzees. In Chevalier-Skolnikoff, S. & Poirier, F. E. (eds.) Primate Bio-social Development: Biological, Social, and Ecological Determinants (pp. 261–288). New York: Garland Publishing.Google Scholar
McGrew, W. C. (1992). Chimpanzee Material Culture: Implications for Human Evolution. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGrew, W. C. & Collins, D. A. (1985). Tool use by wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) to obtain termites (Macrotermes herus) in the Mahale Mountains, Tanzania. American Journal of Primatology, 9, 47–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mitani, J. C. (2006). Demographic influences on the behavior of chimpanzees. Primates, 47(1), 6–13.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mitani, J., Watts, D. & Lwanga, J. (2002). Ecological and social correlates of chimpanzee party size and composition. In Boesch, C., Hohmann, G. & Marchant, L. F. (eds.) Behavioural Diversity in Chimpanzees and Bonobos (pp. 102–111). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Möbius, Y., Boesch, C., Koops, K., Matsuzawa, T. & Humle, T. (2008). Cultural differences in army ant predation by West African chimpanzees? A comparative study of microecological variables. Animal Behaviour, 76, 37–45.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Newman, M. E. J. (2001). The structure of scientific collaboration networks. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 98, 404–409.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Newman, M. E. J. (2003). The structure and function of complex networks. Society for Industrial and Applied Mathematics, 45, 167–256.Google Scholar
Newman, M. E. J. (2004). Analysis of weighted networks. Physical Review E, 70, 056131.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Newman, M. E. J., Watts, D. J. & Strogatz, S. H. (2002). Random graph models of social networks. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 99, 2566–2572.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Potts, R. (2004). Paleoenvironmental basis of cognitive evolution in great apes. American Journal of Primatology, 62(3), 209–228.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pradhan, G. R., Tennie, C. & van Schaik, C. P. (2012). Social organization and the evolution of cumulative technology of apes and hominins. Journal of Human Evolution, 63(1), 180–190.CrossRef
Sanz, C. M. & Morgan, D. B. (2007). Chimpanzee tool technology in the Goualougo Triangle, Republic of Congo. Journal of Human Evolution, 52(4), 420–433.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sanz, C. M. & Morgan, D. B. (2009). Flexible and persistent tool-using strategies in honey-gathering by wild chimpanzees. International Journal of Primatology, 30(3), 411–427.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sanz, C. & Morgan, D. (2010). Complexity of chimpanzee tool using behaviors. In Lonsdorf, E. V., Ross, S. R. & Matsuzawa, T. (eds.) The Mind of the Chimpanzee: Ecological and Experimental Perspectives (pp. 127–140). Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Sanz, C., Morgan, D. & Gulick, S. (2004). New insights into chimpanzees, tools, and termites from the Congo basin. American Naturalist, 164(5), 567–581.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sanz, C., Call, J. & Morgan, D. (2009). Design complexity in termite-fishing tools of chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Biology Letters, 5(3), 293–296.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sanz, C. M., Schöning, C. & Morgan, D. B. (2010). Chimpanzees prey on army ants with specialized tool set. American Journal of Primatology, 72(1), 17–24.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shumaker, R. W., Walkup, K. R. & Beck, B. B. (2011). Animal Tool Behavior: The Use and Manufacture of Tools by Animals. Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University Press.Google Scholar
Sugiyama, J. (1984). Population dynamics of wild chimpanzees at Bossou, Guinea, between 1976–1983. Primates, 25, 391–400.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thornton, A. & McAuliffe, K. (2006). Teaching in wild meerkats. Science, 313, 227–229.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Schaik, C. P. (2001). Fragility of traditions: the disturbance hypothesis for the loss of local traditions in orangutans. International Journal of Primatology, 23(3), 527–538.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Schaik, C. P. (2003). Local traditions in orangutans and chimpanzees: social learning and social tolerance. In Fragaszy, D. M. & Perry, S. (eds.) The Biology of Traditions: Models and Evidence (pp. 297–328). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Schaik, C. P., Deaner, R. O. & Merrill, M. Y. (1999). The conditions for tool use in primates: implications for the evolution of material culture. Journal of Human Evolution, 36, 719–741.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Visalberghi, E., Fragaszy, D., Ottoni, E., et al. (2007). Characteristics of hammer stones and anvils used by wild bearded capuchin monkeys (Cebus libidinosus) to crack open palm nuts. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 132(3), 426–444.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whitehead, H. (2006). SOCPROG 2.3.
Whiten, A., Goodall, J., McGrew, W. C., et al. (1999). Cultures in chimpanzees. Nature, 399, 682–685.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whiten, A., Goodall, J., McGrew, W. C., et al. (2001). Charting cultural variation in chimpanzees. Behaviour, 138, 1481–1516.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yamamoto, S., Humle, T. & Tanaka, M. (2009). Chimpanzees help each other upon request. PLoS ONE, 4(10).Google ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×