Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-x24gv Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-18T11:44:26.946Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

17 - Developing word-level literacy skills in children with and without typical communication skills

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 April 2011

Kenn Apel
Affiliation:
Florida State University
Elizabeth B. Wilson-Fowler
Affiliation:
Florida State University
Julie J. Masterson
Affiliation:
Missouri State University
Sue Ellis
Affiliation:
University of Strathclyde
Elspeth McCartney
Affiliation:
University of Strathclyde
Get access

Summary

Introduction

Words have power. Spoken words convey meaning and intent. Without an understanding and facility with spoken words, face-to-face communication can be impeded. Written words are equally powerful. Successful reading and writing require strong knowledge of the written word. Children must know how to read words to access meaning at the word, sentence and text level. Equally important, children also must be able to spell words correctly to convey accurate and appropriate information to their readers.

In the USA, teachers provide instruction in word-level reading (i.e. decoding and recognition abilities) during the early primary years (e.g. Darling-Hammond 2000) although the amount of time focused specifically on decoding may be variable (e.g. Juel and Minden-Cupp 2000). Even less time is spent on spelling instruction. Graham et al. (2008) reported that 90 per cent of the primary-grade teachers they surveyed taught spelling. However, there was quite a bit of variability in teachers' responses, with some spending less than twenty minutes per week and a few not teaching spelling at all. It seems educators assume that children should quickly develop good word-level reading and spelling skills, and, therefore, provide little focused instruction in these areas, especially beyond the early primary years. While the majority of children acquire word-level reading with minimal instruction, a substantial number do not (Lyon 1998). Estimates are that almost 18 per cent of children in the USA encounter significant reading problems in the first three years of school (NAEP 2005).

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2011

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×