Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-r6qrq Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-27T19:00:28.534Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

2 - Designing the study

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 April 2013

Natalie Schilling
Affiliation:
Georgetown University, Washington DC
Get access

Summary

As with any project, completing a successful sociolinguistic research project involves a good deal of pre-planning. Fieldwork is time-consuming, emotionally demanding, and often expensive, and there is nothing worse than spending time and energy in the field only to return home realizing that you should have delimited your population differently, obtained data from different groups or individuals, or perhaps more or fewer speakers, or obtained better quality data, whether in terms of recording quality or in terms of what people said or how they said it. I have taught courses in sociolinguistic field methods and supervised students’ sociolinguistic research projects for a number of years, and I cannot count how many times students have included in their field reports the phrase, “If only I had . . .”

In this chapter, we take the first steps toward eliminating the “if onlys” by stressing the importance of carefully selecting one’s community of study, choosing which speakers to study from within that community, and appropriately categorizing the selected speakers. We also discuss the extra care that must go into planning a re-study of a community previously subjected to sociolinguistic study, for the purposes of obtaining data on language change in real time. While seemingly a re-study would involve a simple replication of previous methods, in actuality replication is by no means “simple”; indeed, exact replication is not really even possible, since even if one manages to record all the same speakers (or the same groupings of speakers, including age, gender, ethnic, and social class groups), using the same interview questionnaire or other survey instrument, the same recording locations, etc., the data-gathering situation will necessarily be different, since the times have changed, and the interviewer and speakers are either different people who were not a part of the original study, or they have aged and changed; hence interviewer and speakers will necessarily interrelate in different ways than in the original study. Obtaining good quality language data are the subjects of Chapters 3 and 4, while good recordings – and record-keeping – are discussed in Chapter 6.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2013

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Patrick, Peter 2002. The Speech Community. In Chambers, J. K., Trudgill, Peter, and Schilling-Estes, Natalie (eds.), The Handbook of Language Variation and Change. Malden, Ma: Blackwell, 573–597.Google Scholar
Bucholtz, Mary 1999. “Why Be Normal?” Language and Identity Practices in a Community of Nerd Girls. Language in Society 28: 203–223.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bailey, Guy, Wikle, Tom, and Tillery, Jan 1997b. The Effects of Methods on Results in Dialectology. English World-Wide 18: 35–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cukor-Avila, Patricia and Bailey, Guy 2013. Real and Apparent Time. In Chambers, J. K. and Schilling, Natalie (eds.), The Handbook of Language Variation and Change. 2nd edn. Malden, MA: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Sankoff, Gillian 2006. Age: Apparent Time and Real Time. Elsevier Encyclopedia of Language and Linguistics. 2nd edn. Article Number: LALI: 01479.
Trudgill, Peter 1988. Norwich revisited: Recent Linguistic Changes in an English Urban Dialect. English World-Wide 9(3): 33–49.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

  • Designing the study
  • Natalie Schilling, Georgetown University, Washington DC
  • Book: Sociolinguistic Fieldwork
  • Online publication: 05 April 2013
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511980541.002
Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

  • Designing the study
  • Natalie Schilling, Georgetown University, Washington DC
  • Book: Sociolinguistic Fieldwork
  • Online publication: 05 April 2013
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511980541.002
Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

  • Designing the study
  • Natalie Schilling, Georgetown University, Washington DC
  • Book: Sociolinguistic Fieldwork
  • Online publication: 05 April 2013
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511980541.002
Available formats
×