Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-qxdb6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-26T13:22:37.134Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

6 - Langerian Mindfulness and Liminal Performing Spaces

from I - INTRODUCTION TO MINDFULNESS: HISTORY AND THEORETICAL UNDERSTANDING

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 January 2016

Sayyed Mohsen Fatemi
Affiliation:
Harvard University, U.S.A.
Amy L. Baltzell
Affiliation:
Boston University
Get access

Summary

One's action or performance in a specific domain may be analyzed by virtue of a wide variety of influencing factors, including social, cultural, physiological, and psychological contexts. Perhaps Lewin's Ecological Model may move in line with the same sort of analysis whereby behavior is considered to be the function of the person and the environment. In looking into the dynamics of the action, one may scrutinize the underlying conscious and unconscious structural components in espousing specific manifestation of a performance.

The hot perspective, which dates back to 1950s and 1960s, was merely interested in the unconscious elements that produced certain types of performance. Thus, any performance, based on the hot perspective, was ultimately embedded within the unconscious interactive process with idiosyncratic emotional, cognitive, and behavioral categories. It was the realization of the clandestine, cryptic, and latently unconscious elements that contributed to the configuration of specific behavioral manifestation (see, for instance, Zajonc, 1984).

An alternative perspective known as the cold approach was inspired by computer-oriented discoveries and focused on the cognitive interplay of the influencing factors of a performance. The focus from the impulsive and unconscious sedimentation of a behavior was here replaced with an interest in the analytical, computational, and serial processing of information and their implications for decision making in a performance. A search for the computational analysis of the performance and its original elements was encouraged in the cold perspective with the intention of identifying the systematic generative constituents of a performance (see, for instance, De Jaegher, Di Paolo, and Gallagher, 2010).

Both perspectives were challenged by critical approaches that indicated how each of the hot and cold outlooks overlooked some of the significant influential factors of a performance. The result was the combination of both perspectives into what was later called the warm look (see, for instance, Sorrentino, 2003; Sorrentino & Higgins, 1986). Dual process theories of social cognition examined two types of information processing that considered both hot and cold perspectives. One led to an effortful and reflective type of thinking, and the other one studied an automatic form of thinking, with each type having implications for behavior and performance (Kruglanski & Orehek, 2007; Petty, 2004).

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2016

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Dale, G. A., and Wrisberg, C. A. (1996). The use of a performance profiling technique in a team setting: Getting athletes and coaches on the “same page.” Sport Psychologist, 10, 261–277.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Jaegher, H., Di Paolo, E., and Gallagher, S. (2010). Can social interaction constitute social cognition?Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14, 441–447. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2010.06.009CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Diberardinis, J., Barwind, J., Flaningam, R. R., and Jenkins, V. (1983). Enhanced interpersonal relation as predictor of athletic performance. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 14, 243–251.Google Scholar
Gilovich, T., and Savitsky, K. (2002). Like goes with like: The role of representativeness in erroneous and pseudo-scientific belies. In Gilovich, T., Griffin, D., and Kahneman, D. (Eds.), Heuristics and biases. The psychology of intuitive judgment (pp. 617–624). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kahneman, D., and Frederick, S. (2002). Representativeness revised: Attribute substitution in intuitive judgment. In Gilovich, T., Griffin, D., and Kahneman, D. (Eds.), Heuristic and biases: The psychology of intuitive judgement (pp. 49–81). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Kruglanski, A. W., and Orehek, E. (2007). Partitioning the domain of social influence: Dual mode and system models and their alternatives. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 291–316. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085629CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Langer, E. J. (1989). Mindfulness. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.Google Scholar
Langer, E. J. (1997). The power of mindful learning. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.Google Scholar
Langer, E. J. (2000). Mindful learning. Journal of the American Psychological Society, 9, 220–223. doi: 10.1111/1467-8721.00099Google Scholar
Langer, E. J. (2005). On becoming an artist: Reinventing yourself through mindful creativity. New York, NY: Ballantine.Google Scholar
Langer, E. J. (2009). Counterclockwise: Mindful health and the power of possibility. New York, NY: Ballantine.Google Scholar
Langer, E. J., Bashner, R. S., and Chanowitz, B. (1985). Decreasing prejudice by increasing discrimination. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49, 113–120. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022–3514.49.1.113CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Langer, E. J., and Piper, A. I. (1987). The prevention of mindlessness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 280–287.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lausic, D., Tennebaum, G., Eccles, D., Jeong, A., and Johnson, T. (2009). Intrateam communication and performance in double tennis. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 80, 281–290. doi: 10.1080/02701367.2009.10599563CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Petty, R. E. (2004). Multi-process model in social psychology provide a more balanced view of social thought and action. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27, 353–354.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schneider, F. W., Gruman, J. A. and Coutts, L. M. (Eds.). (2012). Applied social psychology, understanding and addressing social and practical problems. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Sorrentino, R. M. (2003). Motivated perception and the warm look: Current perspectives and future directions. In Spencer, S. J.., Fein, S., Zanna, M. P., and Olson, J. M. (Eds.), Motivated social perception: The Ontario symposium (Vol. 9., pp. 299–316). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Sorrentino, R. M., and Higgins, E. T. (1986). Motivation and cognition: Warming to synergism. In Sorrentino, R. M. and Higgins, E. T. (Eds.), The handbook of motivation and cognition: Foundations of social behavior (pp. 3–19). New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Sullivan, P. J. (1995). The relationship between communication and cohesion in inter-collegiate rugby players (Unpublished master's thesis). University of Windsor:Ontario, Canada.Google Scholar
Sullivan, P., and Feltz, D. L. (2003). The preliminary development of the Scale for Effective Communication in Sport Teams (SECTS). Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 33, 1693–1715. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.2003.tb01970.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Topolski, D., and Robinson, P. (1989). True blue: The Oxford boat race mutiny. London, UK: Doubleday.Google Scholar
Widmeyer, W. N., and Williams, J. M. (1991). Predicting cohesion in a coaching sport. Small Group Research, 22, 548–570. doi: 10.1177/1046496491224007CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zajonc, R. B. (1984). On the primacy of affect. American Psychologist, 39, 117–123. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.39.2.117CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×