Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-mlc7c Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-10-30T03:10:58.010Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Part VIII - Psychological effects on color perception

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 April 2016

Andrew J. Elliot
Affiliation:
University of Rochester, New York
Mark D. Fairchild
Affiliation:
Rochester Institute of Technology, New York
Anna Franklin
Affiliation:
University of Sussex
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2015

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

References

Adams, G. K. (1923). An experimental study of memory color and related phenomena. American Journal of Psychology, 34(3), 359407.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baker, K. E., and Mackintosh, I. (1955). The influence of past associations upon attributive color judgments. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49(4), 281–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bannert, M. M., and Bartels, A. (2013). Decoding the yellow of a gray banana. Current Biology, 23(22), 2268–72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barsalou, L. W. (2008). Grounded cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 617–45.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bartleson, C. J. (1960). Memory colors of familiar objects. Journal of the Optical Society of America, 50, 73–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bolles, R. C., Hulicka, I. M., and Hanly, B. (1959). Color judgment as a function of stimulus conditions and memory color. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 13, 175–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bramao, I., Reis, A., Petersson, K. M., and Faisca, L. (2011). The role of color information on object recognition: a review and meta-analysis. Acta Psychologica, 138(1), 244–53.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Broackes, J. (1992). The autonomy of color. In Lennon, K. and Charles, D. (eds.), Reduction, Explanation and Realism (pp. 421–65). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Bruner, J. S., Postman, L., and Rodrigues, J. (1951). Expectation and the perception of color. American Journal of Psychology, 64(2), 216–27.Google ScholarPubMed
Byrne, A., and Hilbert, D. R. (2003). Color realism and color science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 26(1), 321; discussion 2263.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chao, L. L., and Martin, A. (1999). Cortical regions associated with perceiving, naming, and knowing about colors. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 11(1), 2535.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cheung, O. S., and Bar, M. (2012). Visual prediction and perceptual expertise. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 83(2), 156–63.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, J. (2006). Color and perceptual variation revisited: unknown facts, alien modalities, and perfect psychosemantics. Dialectica, 60(3), 307–19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Collins, J. A., and Olson, I. R. (2014). Knowledge is power: how conceptual knowledge transforms visual cognition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 21(4), 843–60.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Delk, J. L., and Fillenbaum, S. (1965). Differences in perceived color as a function of characteristic color. American Journal of Psychology, 78, 290–3.Google ScholarPubMed
Deroy, O. (2013). Object-sensitivity versus cognitive penetrability of perception. Philosophical Studies, 162(1), 87107.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dorffner, G. (1998). Flexible features, connectionism, and computational learning theory. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 21, 24–5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duncker, K. (1939). The influence of past experience upon perceptual properties. American Journal of Psychology, 52(2), 255–65.Google Scholar
Emmerson, P. G., and Ross, H. E. (1987). Variation in color constancy with visual information in the underwater environment. Acta Psychologica, 65(2), 101–13.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fahle, M. (2009). Perceptual learning and sensomotor flexibility: cortical plasticity under attentional control? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 364(1515), 313–19.Google ScholarPubMed
Fisher, S. C., Hull, C., and Holtz, P. (1956). Past experience and perception: memory color. American Journal of Psychology, 69(4), 546–60.Google ScholarPubMed
Fuster, J. M. (1997). Network memory. Trends in Neurosciences, 20(10), 451–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gegenfurtner, K. R. (2003). Cortical mechanisms of color vision. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4, 563–72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gegenfurtner, K. R., and Kiper, D. C. (2003). Color vision. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 26(1), 181206.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gegenfurtner, K. R., and Rieger, J. (2000). Sensory and cognitive contributions of color to the recognition of natural scenes. Current Biology, 10(13), 805–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goffaux, V., Jacques, C., Mouraux, A., Oliva, A., and Schyns, P. G. (2005). Diagnostic colours contribute to early stages of scene categorization: behavioural and neurophysiological evidence. Visual Cognition, 12(6), 878–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldstone, R. L. (1998). Perceptual learning. Annual Review of Psychology, 49, 585612.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gosselin, F., and Schyns, P. G. (2003). Superstitious perceptions reveal properties of internal representations. Psychological Science, 14(5), 505–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Granzier, J. J. M., and Gegenfurtner, K. R. (2012). Effects of memory color on color constancy for unknown colored objects. i-Perception, 3(3), 190215.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hansen, T., and Gegenfurtner, K. R. (2006). Color scaling of discs and natural objects at different luminance levels. Visual Neuroscience, 23(3–4), 603–10.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hansen, T., Giesel, M., and Gegenfurtner, K. R. (2008). Chromatic discrimination of natural objects. Journal of Vision, 8(1), 2, 119.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hansen, T., Olkkonen, M., Walter, S., and Gegenfurtner, K. R. (2006). Memory modulates color appearance. Nature Neuroscience, 9(11), 1367–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hansen, T., Walter, S., and Gegenfurtner, K. R. (2007). Effects of spatial and temporal context on color categories and color constancy. Journal of Vision, 7(4), 115.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hardin, C. L. (1988). Color for Philosophers: Unweaving the Rainbow. Indianapolis, IN: Hackett.Google Scholar
Harper, R. S. (1953). The perceptual modification of colored figures. American Journal of Psychology, 66(1), 86–9.Google ScholarPubMed
Harper, R. S. (1972). Human Senses in Action. Edinburgh: Churchill Livingstone.Google Scholar
Hering, E. (1964/1878). Outlines of a Theory of the Light Sense, trans. Hurvich, M. and Jameson, D.. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Herring, B. S., and Bryden, M. P. (1970). Memory color effects as a function of viewing time. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 24, 127–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ho-Phuoc, T., Guyader, N., Landragin, F., and Guérin-Dugué, A. (2012). When viewing natural scenes, do abnormal colors impact on spatial or temporal parameters of eye movements? Journal of Vision, 12(2).CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hsu, N. S., Frankland, S. M., and Thompson-Schill, S. L. (2012). Chromaticity of color perception and object color knowledge. Neuropsychologia, 50(2), 327–33.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huettig, F., and Altmann, G. T. (2011). Looking at anything that is green when hearing “frog”: how object surface color and stored object color knowledge influence language-mediated overt attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64(1), 122–45.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hurlbert, A. C., and Ling, Y. (2005). If it’s a banana, it must be yellow: the role of memory colors in color constancy. Journal of Vision, 5(8), 787–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hyman, J. (2005). What, if anything, are colors relative to? Philosophy, 80(04), 475–94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jameson, D., and Hurvich, L. M. (1989). Essay concerning color constancy. Annual Review of Psychology, 40, 122.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnson, E. K., and Huettig, F. (2011). Eye movements during language-mediated visual search reveal a strong link between overt visual attention and lexical processing in 36-month-olds. Psychological Research, 75(1), 3542.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, E. K., McQueen, J. M., and Huettig, F. (2011). Toddlers’ language-mediated visual search: they need not have the words for it. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64(9), 1672–82.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Joseph, J. E., and Proffitt, D. R. (1996). Semantic versus perceptual influences of color in object recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22(2), 407–29.Google ScholarPubMed
Kanematsu, E., and Brainard, D. H. (2014). No measured effect of a familiar contextual object on color constancy. Color Research & Application, 39(4), 347–59.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kimura, A., Wada, Y., Masuda, T., Goto, S., Tsuzuki, D., Hibino, H., and Dan, I. (2013). Memory color effect induced by familiarity of brand logos. PLoS ONE, 8(7), e68474.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
King, A. (2000). Situated cognition. In Kazdin, A. E. (ed.), Encyclopedia of Psychology (vol. VII, pp. 289–91). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Leibovich, B., and Paolera, M. B. (1970). Research on color perception and after-image in relation to significant images. Annales Médico-Psychologiques, 128(5), 735–42.Google ScholarPubMed
Lewis, D. E., Pearson, J., and Khuu, S. K. (2013). The color “fruit”: object memories defined by color. PLoS ONE, 8(5), e64960.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ling, Y., Allen-Clarke, L., Vurro, M., and Hurlbert, A. C. (2008). The effect of object familiarity and changing illumination on color categorization. Perception, 37(ECVP Abstract Suppl.), 149.Google Scholar
Ling, Y., and Hurlbert, A. C. (2004). Color and size interactions in a real 3D object similarity task. Journal of Vision, 4(9), 721–34.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ling, Y., and Hurlbert, A. C. (2006). Color-memory-dependent color constancy: 2D vs 3D real surfaces. Paper presented at the Third European Conference on Color in Graphics, Imaging, and Vision (CGIV).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lotto, R. B., and Purves, D. (2002). The empirical basis of color perception. Consciousness and Cognition, 11(4), 609–29.Google ScholarPubMed
Lu, Z. L., Hua, T., Huang, C. B., Zhou, Y., and Dosher, B. A. (2011). Visual perceptual learning. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 95(2), 145–51.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lupyan, G. (2013). Semantic effects on color afterimages. Journal of Vision, 13(9), 466.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lyons, J. (2011). Circularity, reliability, and the cognitive penetrability of perception. Philosophical Issues, 21(1), 289311.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacPherson, F. (2012). Cognitive penetration of color experience. Philosophy and Phenomenological Research, 84(1), 2462.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miceli, G., Fouch, E., Capasso, R., Shelton, J. R., Tomaiuolo, F., and Caramazza, A. (2001). The dissociation of color from form and function knowledge. Nature Neuroscience, 4(6), 662–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mitterer, H., and de Ruiter, J. P. (2008). Recalibrating color categories using world knowledge. Psychological Science, 19(7), 629–34.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mitterer, H., Horschig, J. M., Musseler, J., and Majid, A. (2009). The influence of memory on perception: it’s not what things look like, it’s what you call them. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35(6), 1557–62.Google ScholarPubMed
Moser, S., Muller, T., Ebert, M. O., Jockusch, S., Turro, N. J., and Krautler, B. (2008). Blue luminescence of ripening bananas. Angewandte Chemie International Edition, 47(46), 8954–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nagai, J.-I., and Yokosawa, K. (2003). What regulates the surface color effect in object recognition: color diagnosticity or category? Technical Report on Attention and Cognition, 28.Google Scholar
Naor-Raz, G., Tarr, M. J., and Kersten, D. (2003). Is color an intrinsic property of object representation? Perception, 32(6), 667–80.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Newhall, S. M., Burnham, R. W., and Clark, J. R. (1957). Comparison of successive with simultaneous color matching. Journal of the Optical Society of America, 47, 4354.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nicholson, K. G., and Humphrey, G. K. (2004). The effect of color congruency on shape discriminations of novel objects. Perception, 33(3), 339–53.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nijboer, T. C., van Zandvoort, M. J., and de Haan, E. H. (2006). Seeing red primes tomato: evidence for comparable priming from color and color name primes to semantically related word targets. Cognitive Processing, 7(4), 269–74.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O’Connor, K., and Glenberg, A. M. (2003). Situated cognition. In Nadel, L. (ed.), Encyclopedia of Cognitive Science (vol. IV, pp. 1925). London: Nature Publishing Group.Google Scholar
Oliva, A., and Schyns, P. G. (2000). Diagnostic colors mediate scene recognition. Cognitive Psychology, 41(2), 176210.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olkkonen, M., Hansen, T., and Gegenfurtner, K. R. (2008). Color appearance of familiar objects: effects of object shape, texture, and illumination changes. Journal of Vision, 8(5), 116.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olkkonen, M., Hansen, T., and Gegenfurtner, K. R. (2012). High-level perceptual influences on color appearance. In Hatfield, G. and Sarah, A. (eds.), Visual Experience: Sensation, Cognition, and Constancy (pp. 179–98). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Panichello, M. F., Cheung, O. S., and Bar, M. (2012). Predictive feedback and conscious visual experience. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 620.Google ScholarPubMed
Pearce, B., Crichton, S., Mackiewicz, M., Finlayson, G. D., and Hurlbert, A. (2014). Chromatic illumination discrimination ability reveals that human color constancy is optimised for blue daylight illuminations. PLoS ONE, 9(2), e87989.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pérez-Carpinell, J., Fez, M. D., Baldoví, R., and Soriano, J. C. (1998). Familiar objects and memory color. Color Research & Application, 23(6), 416–27.3.0.CO;2-N>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Prevor, M. B., and Diamond, A. (2005). Color-object interference in young children: a Stroop effect in children 3(1/2)–6(1/2) years old. Cognitive Development, 20(2), 256–78.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ratner, C., and McCarthy, J. (1990). Ecologically relevant stimuli and color memory. Journal of General Psychology, 117(4), 369–77.Google ScholarPubMed
Ross, P. W. (2001). The location problem for color subjectivism. Consciousness and Cognition, 10(1), 4258.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rossion, B., and Pourtois, G. (2004). Revisiting Snodgrass and Vanderwart’s object pictorial set: the role of surface detail in basic-level object recognition. Perception, 33(2), 217–36.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rumelhart, D. E., and Zipser, D. (1985). Feature discovery by competitive learning. Cognitive Science, 9, 75112.Google Scholar
Schyns, P. G., Goldstone, R. L., and Thibaut, J.-P. (1998). The development of features in object concepts. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 21, 117.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Siegel, S. (2012). Cognitive penetrability and perceptual justification. Noûs, 46(2), 201–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Simmons, W. K., Ramjee, V., Beauchamp, M. S., McRae, K., Martin, A., and Barsalou, L. W. (2007). A common neural substrate for perceiving and knowing about color. Neuropsychologia, 45(12), 2802–10.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Siple, P., and Springer, R. M. (1983). Memory and preference for the colors of objects. Perception and Psychophysics, 34(4), 363–70.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sternheim, C. E., and Boynton, R. M. (1966). Uniqueness of perceived hues investigated with a continuous judgmental technique. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 72(5), 770–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
St. Julien, J. (1997). Explaining learning: the research trajectory of situated cognition and the implications of connectionism. In Kirshner, D. and Whitson, J. A. (eds.), Situated Cognition: Social, Semiotic, and Psychological Perspectives (pp. 261–80). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Stokes, D. (2013). Cognitive penetrability of perception. Philosophy Compass, 8(7), 646–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tanaka, J. W., and Presnell, L. M. (1999). Color diagnosticity in object recognition. Perception and Psychophysics, 61(6), 1140–53.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tanaka, J. W., Weiskopf, D., and Williams, P. (2001). The role of color in high-level vision. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5(5), 211–15.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Therriault, D. J., Yaxley, R. H., and Zwaan, R. A. (2009). The role of color diagnosticity in object recognition and representation. Cognitive Processing, 10(4), 335–42.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Toribio, J. (2014). Nonconceptualism and the cognitive impenetrability of early vision. Philosophical Psychology, 27(5), 621–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Uttl, B., Graf, P., and Santacruz, P. (2006). Object color affects identification and repetition priming. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 47(5), 313–25.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van Gulick, A. E., and Tarr, M. J. (2010). Is object color memory categorical? Journal of Vision, 10(7), 407.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vandenbroucke, Fahrenfort, Meuwese, Scholte, and Lamme, . (in press). Prior knowledge about objects determines neural color representation in human visual cortex. Cerebral Cortex.Google Scholar
Versace, R., Riou, B., Vallet, G. T., Lesourd, M., Labeye, É., and Brunel, L. (2014). ACT-IN: an integrated view of memory mechanisms. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 26, 280306.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
von Helmholtz, H. (1924/1867). Treatise on Physiological Optics, vol. II (Southall, J. P. C. ed.). Menasha, WI: Optical Society of America.Google Scholar
von Helmholtz, H. (1925/1867). Treatise on Physiological Optics, vol. III (Southall, J. P. C., ed.). Menasha, WI: Optical Society of America.Google Scholar
Vurro, M., Ling, Y., and Hurlbert, A. C. (2013). Memory color of natural familiar objects: effects of surface texture and 3-D shape. Journal of Vision, 13(7), 20.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wandell, B. A. (1995). Foundations of Vision. Sunderland, MA: Sinaur Associates.Google Scholar
Watanabe, T., Náñez, J. E., and Sasaki, Y. (2001). Perceptual learning without perception. Nature, 413, 844–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Watzlawick, P. (ed.) (1984). The Invented Reality: How Do We Know What We Believe We Know? Contributions to Constructivism. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
White, C. W., and Montgomery, D. A. (1976). Memory colors in afterimages: a bicentennial demonstration. Perception and Psychophysics, 19(4), 371–4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wichmann, F. A., Sharpe, L. T., and Gegenfurtner, K. R. (2002). The contributions of color to recognition memory for natural scenes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 28(3), 509–20.Google ScholarPubMed
Wittgenstein, L. (1953). Philosophical Investigations, 3rd edn. Trans. Anscombe, G. E. M.. New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Witzel, C. (2011). Unterschiede in der Farbwahrnehmung. In Groh, A. (ed.), Was ist Farbe? – Bunte Beiträge aus der Wissenschaft (pp. 3962). Berlin: Weidler.Google Scholar
Witzel, C. (2012). Colours’ Appearance in the Light of Language and Experience. Doctoral dissertation, Justus-Liebig-Universität, Gießen.Google Scholar
Witzel, C., and Gegenfurtner, K. R. (2013). Memory color. In Luo, R. (ed.), Encyclopedia of Color Science and Technology (http://link.springer.com/referenceworkentry/10.1007/978-3-642-27851-8_58-8). Heidelberg: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
Witzel, C., Valkova, H., Hansen, T., and Gegenfurtner, K. R. (2011). Object knowledge modulates color appearance. i-Perception, 2(1), 1349.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wright, W. (2010). Perception, color, and realism. Erkenntnis, 73, 1940.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wurm, L. H., Legge, G. E., Isenberg, L. M., and Luebker, A. (1993). Color improves object recognition in normal and low vision. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 19(4), 899911.Google ScholarPubMed
Yee, E., Ahmed, S. Z., and Thompson-Schill, S. L. (2012). Colorless green ideas (can) prime furiously. Psychological Science, 23(4), 364–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yendrikhovskij, S. N., Blommaert, F. J. J., and Ridder, H. D. (1999). Representation of memory prototype for an object color. Color Research & Application, 24(6), 393410.3.0.CO;2-Z>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yoonessi, A., and Zaidi, Q. (2010). The role of color in recognizing material changes. Ophthalmic and Physiological Optics, 30(5), 626–31.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zeimbekis, J. (2013). Color and cognitive penetrability. Philosophical Studies, 165, 167–75.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

References

Abramov, I. (1997). Physiological mechanisms of color vision. In Hardin, C. L. and Maffi, L. (eds.), Color Categories in Thought and Language (pp. 89117). New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Adams, F. M., and Osgood, C. E. (1973). A cross-cultural study of the affective meanings of color. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 4, 135–56.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Attrill, M. J., Gresty, K. A., Hill, R. A., and Barton, R. A. (2008). Red shirt colour is associated with long-term team success in English football. Journal of Sports Sciences, 26, 577–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ault, J. T., and Barney, S. T. (2007). Construct validity and reliability of Hartman’s Color Code Personality Profile. International Journal of Selection and Assessment, 15, 7281.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Balcetis, E., and Lassiter, G. D. (2010). Social Psychology of Visual Perception. New York: Psychology Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Banerjee, P., Chatterjee, P., and Sinha, J. (2012). Is it dark or light? Recalling moral behavior changes perception of brightness. Psychological Science, 23, 407–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barrick, C. B., Taylor, D., and Correa, E. I. (2002). Color sensitivity and mood disorders: biology or metaphor? Journal of Affective Disorders, 68, 6771.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Birren, F. (1973). Color preference as a clue to personality. Art Psychotherapy, 1, 1316.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bowmaker, J. K. (2012). Evolution of the vertebrate eye. In Lazareva, O. F., Shimizu, T., and Wasserman, E. A. (eds.), How Animals See the World: Comparative Behavior, Biology, and the Evolution of Vision (pp. 441–72). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Burley, N., Krantzberg, G., and Radman, P. (1982). Influence of colour-banding on the conspecific preferences of zebra finches (Poephilia guttata). Animal Behaviour, 30, 444–55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bushman, B. J., and Anderson, C. A. (1998). Methodology in the study of aggression: integrating experimental and nonexperimental findings. In Geen, R. G. and Donnerstein, E. (eds.), Human Aggression: Theories, Research, and Implications for Social Policy (pp. 2348). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Changizi, M. (2009). The Vision Revolution: How the Latest Research Overturns Everything We Thought We Knew About Human Vision. Dallas, TX: BenBella Books.Google Scholar
Changizi, M. A., Zhang, Q., and Shimojo, S. (2006). Bare skin, blood and the evolution of primate colour vision. Biology Letters, 2, 217–21.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clarke, T., and Costall, A. (2008). The emotional connotations of color: a qualitative investigation. Color Research & Application, 33, 406–10.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
D’Andrade, R., and Egan, M. (1974). The colors of emotion. American Ethnologist, 1, 4963.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dixson, A. F. (1998). Primate Sexuality: Comparative Studies of the Prosimians, Monkeys, Apes, and Human Beings. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Drummond, P. D., and Quah, S. H. (2001). The effect of expressing anger on cardiovascular reactivity and facial blood flow in Chinese and Caucasians. Psychophysiology, 38, 190–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elliot, A. J., and Maier, M. A. (2014). Color psychology: effects of perceiving color on psychological functioning in humans. Annual Review of Psychology, 65, 95120.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ellis, H. (1900). The psychology of red. Popular Science Monthly, 57, 365–75.Google Scholar
Epstein, S. (1994). Integration of the cognitive and psychodynamic unconscious. American Psychologist, 49, 709–24.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eysenck, H. J. (1941). A critical and experimental study of colour preferences. American Journal of Psychology, 54, 385–94.Google Scholar
Fairchild, M. D. (2005). Color Appearance Models, 2nd edn. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Farrelly, D., Slater, R., Walden, H. R., and Wetherell, M. A. (2013). Competitors who choose to be red have higher testosterone levels. Psychological Science, 24, 2122–4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Feltman, R., and Elliot, A. J. (2011). The influence of red on perceptions of relative dominance and threat in a competitive context. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 33, 308–14.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fetterman, A. K., Liu, T., and Robinson, M. D. (2014). Extending color psychology to the personality realm: interpersonal hostility varies by red preferences and perceptual biases. Journal of Personality, 83(1).Google Scholar
Fetterman, A. K., Robinson, M. D., Gordon, R. D., and Elliot, A. J. (2011). Anger as seeing red: perceptual sources of evidence. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 2, 311–16.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fetterman, A. K., Robinson, M. D., and Meier, B. P. (2012). Anger as ‘seeing red’: evidence for a perceptual association. Cognition and Emotion, 26, 1445–58.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gage, J. (1999). Colour and Meaning: Art, Science and Symbolism. London: Thames and Hudson.Google Scholar
Goldstein, K. (1942). Some experimental observations concerning the influence of colors on the function of the organism. Occupational Therapy and Rehabilitation, 21, 147–51.Google Scholar
Gorn, G. J., Chattopadhyay, A., Sengupta, J., and Tripathi, S. (2004). Waiting for the Web: how screen color affects time perception. Journal of Marketing Research, 41, 215–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greenwald, A. G., and Banaji, M. R. (1995). Implicit social cognition: attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes. Psychological Review, 102, 427.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hagemann, N., Strauss, B., and Leissing, J. (2008). When the referee sees red. Psychological Science, 19, 769–71.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
He, W., Zhang, Y., Zhu, J., Xu, Y., Yu, W., Chen, W., et al. (2011). Could sex difference in color preference and its personality correlates fit into social theories? Let Chinese university students tell you. Personality and Individual Differences, 51, 154–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hill, R. A., and Barton, R. A. (2005). Red enhances human performance in contests. Nature, 435, 293.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Humphrey, N. (1976). The colour currency of nature. In Porter, T. and Mikellides, B. (eds.), Colour for Architecture (pp. 95–8). London: Studio-Vista.Google Scholar
Hurlbert, C. H., and Ling, Y. (2007). Biological components of sex differences in color preference. Current Biology, 17, R623–5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kay, P., Berlin, B., Maffi, L., and Merrifield, W. (1997). Color naming across languages. In Hardin, C. L. and Maffi, L. (eds.), Color Categories in Thought and Language (pp. 2156). New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaya, N., and Epps, H. H. (2004). Relationship between color and emotion: a study of college students. College Student Journal, 38, 396405.Google Scholar
Koole, S. L., and Pelham, B. W. (2003). On the nature of implicit self-esteem: the case of the name letter effect. In Spencer, S. J., Fein, S., Zanna, M. P., and Olson, J. M. (eds.), Motivated Social Perception: The Ontario Symposium (vol. IX, pp. 93116). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Kövecses, Z. (2005). Metaphor in Culture: Universality and Variation. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Labrecque, L. I., and Milne, G. R. (2012). Exciting red and competent blue: the importance of color in marketing. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 40, 711–27.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lakoff, G., and Johnson, M. (1999). Philosophy in the Flesh. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Landau, M. J., Robinson, M. D., and Meier, B. P. (2014). Introduction. In Landau, M. J., Robinson, M. D., and Meier, B. P. (eds.), The Power of Metaphor: Examining Its Influence on Social Life (pp. 316). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lang, P. J. (1979). A bio-informational theory of emotional imagery. Psychophysiology, 16, 495512.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, J. L., Andrade, E. B., and Palmer, S. (2014). How emotions influence color perception. Paper submitted for publication.Google Scholar
Lerner, J. S., and Keltner, D. (2001). Fear, anger, and risk. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 146–59.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lichtenfeld, S., Elliot, A. J., Maier, M. A., and Pekrun, R. (2012). Fertile green: green facilitates creative performance. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 38, 784–97.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ligon, J. D., Thornhill, R., Zuk, M., and Johnson, K. (1990). Male–male competition in red jungle fowl and the multiple roles of testosterone in sexual selection. Animal Behaviour, 40, 367–73.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lüscher, M., and Scott, I. (1971). The Lüscher Color Test. New York: Pocket Books.Google Scholar
Mahnke, F. H. (1996). Color, Environment, and Human Response. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Mauss, I. B., and Robinson, M. D. (2009). Measures of emotion: a review. Cognition and Emotion, 23, 209–37.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mehta, R., and Zhu, R. (2009). Blue or red? Exploring the effect of color on cognitive task performances. Science, 323, 1226–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meier, B. P., and Robinson, M. D. (2005). The metaphorical representation of affect. Metaphor and Symbol, 20, 239–57.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meier, B. P., Robinson, M. D., and Clore, G. L. (2004). Why good guys wear white: automatic inferences about stimulus valence based on brightness. Psychological Science, 15, 82–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meier, B. P., Robinson, M. D., Crawford, L. E., and Ahlvers, W. J. (2007). When ‘light’ and ‘dark’ thoughts become light and dark responses: affect biases brightness judgments. Emotion, 7, 366–76.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moller, A. C., Elliot, A. J., and Maier, M. A. (2009). Basic hue-meaning associations. Emotion, 9, 898902.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moskowitz, D. S. (2010). Quarrelsomeness in daily life. Journal of Personality, 78, 3966.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ou, L., Luo, M. R., Woodcock, A., and Wright, A. (2004). A study of colour emotion and colour preference. I. Colour emotions for single colours. Color Research & Application, 29, 232–40.Google Scholar
Palmer, S. E., and Schloss, K. B. (2010). An ecological theory of human color preference. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 107, 8877–82.Google ScholarPubMed
Palmer, S. E., Schloss, K. B., Xu, Z., and Prado-León, L. R. (2013). Music-color associations are mediated by emotion. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 110, 8836–41.Google ScholarPubMed
Pelham, B. W., Carvallo, M., and Jones, J. T. (2005). Implicit egotism. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14, 106–10.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pravossoudovitch, K., Cury, F., Young, S. G., and Elliot, A. J. (2014). Is red the colour of danger? Testing an implicit red–danger association. Ergonomics, 57, 503–10.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Regan, B. C., Julliot, C., Simmen, B., Vienot, F., Charles-Dominique, P., and Mollon, J. D. (2001). Fruits, foliage and the evolution of primate color vision. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 356, 229–83.Google Scholar
Regan, C. L., and Horn, S. A. (2005). To nature or not to nature: associations between environmental preferences, mood states and demographic factors. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 25, 5766.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Robinson, M. D., and Fetterman, A. K. (2014). Toward a metaphor-enriched personality psychology. In Landau, M., Robinson, M. D., and Meier, B. P. (eds.), The Power of Metaphor: Examining Its Influence on Social Life (pp. 133–52). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Robinson, M. D., Fetterman, A. K., and Liu, T. (2014). Light–dark metaphors and personality. Paper in preparation.Google Scholar
Röderer, K., and Cervinka, R. (2012). Mental representations of nature: the importance of well-being. In Kabisch, S. K., Kunath, A. K., Schweizer-Ries, P., and Steinführer, A. S. (eds.), Vulnerability, Risks, and Complexity: Impacts of Global Change on Human Habitats (pp. 243–54). Cambridge, MA: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Russell, J. A. (2003). Core affect and the psychological construction of emotion. Psychological Review, 110, 145–72.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schloss, K. B., Poggesi, R. M., and Palmer, S. E. (2011). Effects of university affiliation and ‘school spirit’ on color preferences: Berkeley versus Stanford. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 18, 498504.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schwarz, N., and Clore, G. L. (2007). Feelings and phenomenal experiences. In Kruglanski, A. W. and Higgins, E. T. (eds.), Social Psychology: Handbook of Basic Principles, 2nd edn. (pp. 385407). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Setchell, J. M., and Wickings, E. J. (2005). Dominance, status signals and coloration in male mandrills (Mandrillus sphinx). Ethology, 111, 2550.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sherman, G. D., and Clore, G. L. (2009). The color of sin: white and black are perceptual symbols of moral purity and pollution. Psychological Science, 20, 1019–25.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Song, H., Vonasch, A. J., Meier, B. P., and Bargh, J. A. (2012). Brighten up: smiles facilitate perceptual judgments of facial lightness. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48, 450–2.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stefanucci, J. K., Gagnon, K. T., and Lessard, D. A. (2011). Follow your heart: emotion adaptively influences perception. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 5, 296308.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stephan, I. D., Smith, M. J. L., Stirrat, M. R., and Perrett, D. I. (2009). Facial skin coloration affects perceived health of human faces. International Journal of Primatology, 30, 845–57.Google Scholar
Valdez, P., and Mehrabian, A. (1994). Effects of color on emotions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 123, 394409.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wade, N. J., and Swanston, M. T. (2013). Visual Perception: An Introduction, 3rd edn. New York: Psychology Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Watson, D. (2000). Mood and Temperament. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Wexner, L. B. (1954). The degree to which colors (hues) are associated with mood-tones. Journal of Applied Psychology, 38, 432–5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Young, S. G., Elliot, A. J., Feltman, R., and Ambady, N. (2013). Red enhances the processing of facial expressions of anger. Emotion, 13, 380–4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhong, C., Bohns, V. K., and Gino, F. (2010). Good lamps are the best police: darkness increases dishonesty and self-interested behavior. Psychological Science, 21, 311–14.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×