Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-5nwft Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-04T15:58:14.412Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

8 - Monkey alarm calls

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  30 July 2009

W. Scott McGraw
Affiliation:
Ohio State University
Klaus Zuberbühler
Affiliation:
University of St Andrews, Scotland
Ronald Noë
Affiliation:
Université Louis Pasteur, Strasbourg
Get access

Summary

Introduction

Most primates vocalize when threatened by a predator. These signals, usually termed alarm calls (from old Italian “all arme” = “to arms” on the approach of an enemy), are interesting for a number of reasons. First, they are relatively uncomplicated to examine. Alarm calls are typically highly discrete signals, and it is thus often not difficult to study both causes and consequences of this behavior. Second, alarm calls are interesting because they pose a problem for evolutionary theory. Paradoxically, they are often amongst the most prominent and noticeable signals in a species' repertoire. But why should an individual behave conspicuously in the presence of a predator, hereby revealing its presence and location? Finally, alarm calls have obtained some significance for the empirical study of pre-linguistic abilities. Alarm calls are unique because they are well suited for experimental work and so provide a unique tool for empirically accessing the cognitive mechanisms underlying an individual's behavior.

The purpose of this chapter is to review some key findings in the study of primate alarm calling behavior. The first section is devoted to the problem of why primates produce seemingly maladaptive behavior in the presence of a predator. Which evolutionary processes could have provided a selective advantage for individuals to behave in this counter-intuitive way? A second section is concerned with the cognitive processes that underlie call production and perception.

Type
Chapter
Information
Monkeys of the Taï Forest
An African Primate Community
, pp. 194 - 220
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2007

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Anderson, M. (1994). Sexual Selection. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Arnold, K. and Zuberbühler, K. (2006a). The alarm calling system of putty- nosed monkeys, Cercopithecus nictitans martini. Animal Behaviour, 72, 643–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arnold, K. and Zuberbühler, K. (2006b). Language evolution: semantic combinations in primate calls. Nature, 441, 303.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bergstrom, C. T. and Lachmann, M. (2001). Alarm calls as costly signals of antipredators vigilance: the watchful babbler game. Animal Behaviour, 61, 535–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blumstein, D. T., Steinmetz, J., Armitage, K. B. and Daniel, J. C. (1997). Alarm calling in yellow-bellied marmots: 2. The importance of direct fitness. Animal Behaviour, 53, 173–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boesch, C. and Boesch, H. (1989). Hunting behavior of wild chimpanzees in the Taï National Park. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 78, 547–73.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chapman, C. A., Chapman, L. J. and Lefebvre, L. (1990). Spider monkey alarm calls: honest advertisement of warning kin. Animal Behaviour, 39, 197–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Charnov, E. L. and Krebs, J. R. (1975). Evolution of alarm calls: altruism or manipulation? American Naturalist, 109, 107–12.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cheney, D. L. and Seyfarth, R. M. (1985). Vervet monkey alarm calls: manipulation through shared information?Behaviour, 94, 739–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cheney, D. L. and Seyfarth, R. M. (1988). Assessment of meaning and the detection of unreliable signals by vervet monkeys. Animal Behaviour, 36, 477–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cheney, D. L. and Seyfarth, R. M. (1990). Attending to behaviour versus attending to knowledge: examining monkeys' attribution of mental states. Animal Behaviour, 40, 742–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clutton-Brock, T. M. and Albon, S. D. (1979). The roaring of red deer and the evolution of honest advertisement. Behaviour, 69, 145–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eckardt, W. and Zuberbühler, K. (2004). Cooperation and competition in two forest monkeys. Behavioral Ecology, 15, 400–11.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eimas, P. D., Siqueland, P., Jusczyk, P. and Vigorito, J. (1971). Speech perception in infants. Science, 212, 303–6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fant, G. (1960). Acoustic Theory of Speech Production. The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
Gautier, J.-P. and Gautier, A. (1977). Communication in Old World monkeys. In How Animals Communicate, ed. Sebeok, T. A.. Bloomington, IN: Indiana University Press, pp. 890–964.Google Scholar
Geissmann, T. (1984). Inheritance of song parameters in the gibbon song, analysed in 2 hybrid gibbons (Hylobates pileatus x H. lar). Folia Primatologica, 24, 216–35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ghazanfar, A. A. and Hauser, M. D. (1999). The neuroethology of primate vocal communication: substrates for the evolution of speech. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3, 377–85.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodall, J. (1986). The Chimpanzees of Gombe: Patterns of Behaviour. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Gouzoules, H. and Gouzoules, S. (1989). Design features and developmental modification of pigtail macaque, Macaca nemestrina, agonistic screams. Animal Behaviour, 32, 182–93.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Green, S. (1975). Variation of vocal pattern with social situation in the Japanese monkey (Macaca fuscata): a field study. In Primate Behavior Developments in Field and Laboratory Research, ed. Rosenblum, L. A.. New York: Academic Press, pp. 1–102.Google Scholar
Gyger, M., Marler, P. and Pickert, R. (1987). Semantics of an avian alarm call system: the male domestic fowl, G. domesticus. Behaviour, 102, 15–40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hailman, J. P. and Ficken, M. S. (1987). Combinatorial animal communication with computable syntax: chick-a-dee calling qualifies as “language” by structural linguistics. Animal Behaviour, 34, 1899–901.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hammerschmidt, K., Freudenstein, T. and Jurgens, U. (2001). Vocal development in squirrel monkeys. Behaviour, 138, 1179–204.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hauser, M. D., Evans, C. S. and Marler, P. (1993). The role of articulation in the production of rhesus monkey, Macaca mulatta, vocalizations. Animal Behaviour, 45, 423–33.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hauser, M. D. and Ybarra, M. S. (1994). The role of lip configuration in monkey vocalizations – experiments using Xylocaine as a nerve block. Brain and Language, 46, 232–44.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Henton, C. and Bladon, A. (1988). Creak as a sociophonetic marker. In Language, Speech, and Mind: Studies in Honour of Victoria A. Fromkin, ed. Hyman, L. M. and Li, C. N.. London: Routledge, pp. 3–29.Google Scholar
Herbinger, I., Boesch, C. and Rothe, H. (2001). Territory characteristics among three neighbouring chimpanzee communities in the Taï National Park, Côte d'Ivoire. International Journal of Primatology, 22, 143–67.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Janik, V. M., Dehnhardt, G. and Todt, D. (1994). Signature whistle variations in a bottlenosed dolphin, Tursiops truncatus. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 35, 243–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leger, D. W., Owings, D. H. and Gelfand, D. L. (1980). Single note vocalizations of California ground squirrels: graded signals and situation – specificity of predator and socially evoked calls. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 52, 227–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lemasson, A. and Hausberger, M. (2004). Patterns of vocal sharing and social dynamics in a captive group of Campbell's monkeys (Cercopithecus campbelli campbelli). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 118, 347–59.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lemasson, A., Hausberger, M. and Zuberbühler, K. (2005). Socially meaningful vocal plasticity in adult Campbell's monkeys (Cercopithecus campbelli). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 112, 220–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lemasson, A., Richard, J. P. and Hausberger, M. (2004). A new methodological approach to context analysis of call production. Bioacoustics – the International Journal of Animal Sound and Its Recording, 14, 111–25.Google Scholar
Marshall, A., Wrangham, R. and Clark Arcadi, A. (1999). Does learning affect the structure of vocalizations in chimpanzees?Animal Behaviour, 58, 825–30.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maynard-Smith, J. (1965). The evolution of alarm calls. American Naturalist, 99, 59–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nagel, T. (1974). What is it like to be a bat?Philosophical Review, 83, 435–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nagel, T. (1995). Other Minds. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Owings, D. H. and Leger, D. W. (1980). Chatter vocalizations of California ground squirrels: predator- and social-role specificity. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 54, 163–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Owings, D. H. and Virginia, R. A. (1978). Alarm calls of California ground squirrels (Spermophilus beecheyi). Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 46, 58–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Owren, M. J. (1990a). Acoustic classification of alarm calls by vervet monkeys (Cercopithecus aethiops) and humans (Homo sapiens). 1. Natural calls. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 104, 20–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Owren, M. J. (1990b). Acoustic classification of alarm calls by vervet monkeys (Cercopithecus aethiops) and humans (Homo sapiens). 2. Synthetic calls. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 104, 29–40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Owren, M. J. and Bernacki, R. H. (1998). Applying Linear Predictive Coding (LPC) to frequency-spectrum analysis of animal acoustic signals. In Animals Acoustic Communication, ed. Hopp, S. L., Owren, M. J. and Evans, C. S.. Berlin: Springer, pp. 129–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Owren, M. J., Dieter, J. A., Seyfarth, R. M. and Cheney, D. L. (1993). Vocalizations of rhesus (Macaca mulatta) and Japanese (M. fuscata) macaques cross-fostered between species show evidence of only limited modification. Developmental Psychobiology, 26, 389–406.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rendall, D., Owren, M. J. and Rodman, R. S. (1998). The role of vocal tract filtering in identity cueing in rhesus monkey (Macaca mulatta) vocalizations. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 103, 602–14.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Riede, T., Bronson, E., Hatzikirou, B. and Zuberbühler, K. (2005). The production mechanisms of Diana monkey alarm calls: morphological data and a model. Journal of Human Evolution, 48, 85–96.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Riede, T., Bronson, E., Hatzikirou, B. and Zuberbühler, K. (2006). Multiple discontinuities in nonhuman vocal tracts – A response to Lieberman (2006). Journal of Human Evolution, 50, 222.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Riede, T. and Zuberbühler, K. (2003a). Pulse register phonation in Diana monkey alarm calls. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 113, 2919–26.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Riede, T. and Zuberbühler, K. (2003b). The relationship between acoustic structure and semantic information in Diana monkey alarm calls. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 114, 1132–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sakura, O. (1989). Variability in contact calls between troops of Japanese macaques: a possible case of neutral evolution of animal culture. Animal Behaviour, 38, 900–2.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seavy, E. A. (2001). Associations of crested guinea fowl Guttera pucherani and monkeys in Kibale national park, Uganda. Ibis, 143, 310–12.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seyfarth, R. M. and Cheney, D. L. (1980). The ontogeny of vervet monkey alarm calling behavior: a preliminary report. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 54, 37–56.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seyfarth, R. and Cheney, D. L. (1997). Behavioral mechanisms underlying vocal communication in nonhuman primates. Animal Learning and Behavior, 25, 249–67.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seyfarth, R. M., Cheney, D. L. and Marler, P. (1980). Monkey responses to three different alarm calls: evidence of predator classification and semantic communication. Science, 210, 801–3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Slocombe, K. and Zuberbühler, K. (2005a). Functionally referential communication in a chimpanzee. Current Biology, 15, 1779–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Slocombe, K. E. and Zuberbühler, K. (2005b). Agonistic screams in wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) vary as a function of social role. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 119, 67–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Snowdon, C. T. and Elowson, A. M. (1999). Pygmy marmosets modify call structure when paired. Ethology, 105, 893–908.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Struhsaker, T. T. (1967). Auditory communication among vervet monkeys (Cercopithecus aethiops). In Social Communication Among Primates, ed. Altmann, S. A.. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, pp. 281–324.Google Scholar
Tenaza, R. R. and Tilson, R. L. (1977). Evolution of long-distance alarm calls in Kloss's gibbon. Nature, 268, 233–5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M. and Zuberbühler, K. (2002). Primate vocal and gestural communication. In The Cognitive Animal, ed. Bekoff, M., Allen, C. and Burghardt, G. M.. Cambridge, MA: Massachusetts Institute of Technology Press, pp. 293–9.Google Scholar
Uster, D. and Zuberbühler, K. (2001). The functional significance of Diana monkey “clear” calls. Behaviour, 138, 741–56.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frisch, K. (1973). Decoding the language of the bee. In Nobel Lecture.Google Scholar
Wachter, B., Schabel, M. and Noë, R. (1997). Diet overlap and polyspecific associations of red colobus and Diana monkeys in the Taï National Park, Ivory Coast. Ethology, 103, 514–26.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wolters, S. and Zuberbühler, K. (2003). Mixed-species associations of Diana and Campbell's monkeys: the costs and benefits of a forest phenomenon. Behaviour, 140, 371–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yates, J. and Tule, N. (1979). Perceiving surprising words in an unattended auditory channel. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 31, 281–6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zuberbühler, K. (2000a). Causal cognition in a non-human primate: field playback experiments with Diana monkeys. Cognition, 76, 195–207.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zuberbühler, K. (2000b). Causal knowledge of predators' behaviour in wild Diana monkeys. Animal Behaviour, 59, 209–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zuberbühler, K. (2000c). Interspecific semantic communication in two forest monkeys. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, B, 267, 713–18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zuberbühler, K. (2000d). Referential labelling in Diana monkeys. Animal Behaviour, 59, 917–27.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zuberbühler, K. (2001). Predator-specific alarm calls in Campbell's guenons. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 50, 414–22.Google Scholar
Zuberbühler, K. (2002a). Effects of natural and sexual selection on the evolution of guenon loud calls. In The Guenons: Diversity and Adaptation in African Monkeys, ed. Glenn, M. E. and Cords, M.. New York: Plenum, pp. 289–306.Google Scholar
Zuberbühler, K. (2002b). A syntactic rule in forest monkey communication. Animal Behaviour, 63, 293–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zuberbühler, K., Cheney, D. L. and Seyfarth, R. M. (1999). Conceptual semantics in a nonhuman primate. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 113, 33–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zuberbühler, K. and Jenny, D. (2002). Leopard predation and primate evolution. Journal of Human Evolution, 43, 873–86.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zuberbühler, K., Jenny, D. and Bshary, R. (1999). The predator deterrence function of primate alarm calls. Ethology, 105, 477–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×