Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-wq484 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-26T05:51:11.630Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

2 - The social systems of the guenons

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  30 July 2009

W. Scott McGraw
Affiliation:
Ohio State University
Klaus Zuberbühler
Affiliation:
University of St Andrews, Scotland
Ronald Noë
Affiliation:
Université Louis Pasteur, Strasbourg
Get access

Summary

Introduction

The social system of a species includes the nature of the interactions of individuals between and within social units and the spatial distribution of different age/sex classes. For primates, theories concerning the evolution of social systems are typically based on field data from a restricted number of species, with a clear bias towards species living in more open habitats (Sterck et al. 1997). This is problematic because the forest is a major primate habitat, housing a large number of primate species. The social behavior of most forest-living primates is not well described, primarily due to the difficulties in accessibility and observation conditions. In this respect the forest guenons (Cercopithecus spp.) are of particular interest for evolutionary theories because they represent a major group of Old World primates.

It is theorized that female primates live in social groups because of anti-predation benefits (van Schaik 1983, van Schaik & van Hoof 1983) and because group-living improves their capacity to defend resources against other groups of conspecifics (Wrangham 1980). Across species, female primates differ in the types of social relations they maintain with one another to achieve these goals. It has been proposed that the relative strengths of inter- and intra-group competition are the two main factors that determine the nature of the females' social relationships and their social system (Sterck et al. 1997, Table 2.1).

Across primate species, inter-group encounters can vary from friendly intermingling to hostile fights.

Type
Chapter
Information
Monkeys of the Taï Forest
An African Primate Community
, pp. 51 - 71
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2007

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Altmann, J. (1974). Observational study of behavior: sampling methods. Behaviour, 49, 227–67.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barton, R. A. (1993). Sociospatial mechanisms of feeding competition in female olive baboons, Papio anubis. Animal Behaviour, 46, 791–802.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barton, R. A., Byrne, R. W. and Whiten, A. (1996). Ecology, feeding competition and social structure in baboons. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 38, 321–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bernstein, I. S. (1991). The correlation between kinship and behaviour in non-human primates. In Kin Recognition, ed. Hepper, P. G.. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 6–29.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bourlière, F., Hunkeler, C. and Bertrand, M. (1970). Ecology and behaviour of Lowe's guenon (Cercopithecus campbelli lowei) in the Ivory Coast. In Old World Monkeys: Evolution, Systematics and Behaviour, ed. Napier, J. R. and Napier, P. H.. New York: Academic Press, pp. 367–405.Google Scholar
Butynski, T. (1990). Comparative ecology of blue monkeys (Cercopithecus mitis) in high and low density subpopulations. Ecological Monographs, 60, 1–26.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Butynski, T. (2002). The guenons: an overview of diversity and taxonomy. In The Guenons: Diversity and Adaptation in African Monkeys, ed. Glenn, M. E. and Cords, M.. New York: Kluwer, pp. 3–13.Google Scholar
Buzzard, P. J. (2004). Interspecific competition among Cercopithecus campbelli, C. petaurista, and C. diana. Ph.D. Thesis. Columbia University, New York.Google Scholar
Buzzard, P. J. (2006). Ecological partitioning of Cercopithecus campbelli, C. petaurista, and C. diana in the Taï Forest. International Journal of Primatology, 27(2), 529–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Byrne, R. W., Conning, A. M. and Young, J. (1983). Social relationships in a captive group of Diana monkeys (Cercopithecus diana). Primates, 24, 360–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chadwick-Jones, J. K. (1989). Presenting and mounting in non-human primates: theoretical developments. Journal of Social and Biological Stress, 12, 319–33.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chism, J. and Cords, M. (1998). De Brazza's monkeys in the Kisere National Reserve, Kenya. African Primates, 3, 18–22.Google Scholar
Chism, J. and Rogers, W. (2002). Grooming and social cohesion in patas monkeys and other guenons. In The Guenons: Diversity and Adaptation in African Monkeys, ed. Glenn, M. E. and Cords, M.. New York: Kluwer, pp. 233–44.Google Scholar
Clutton-Brock, T. H. and Harvey, P. H. (1977). Primate ecology and social organization. Journal of Zoology, London, 183, 1–39.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cords, M. (1987). Mixed species associations of Cercopithecus monkeys in the Kakamega Forest. University of California Publications in Zoology, 117, 1–109.Google Scholar
Cords, M. (1988). Mating systems of forest guenons: a preliminary review. In A Primate Radiation: Evolutionary Biology of the African Guenons, ed. Gautier-Hion, A., Bourlière, F., Gautier, J.-P. and Kingdon, J.. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 323–39.Google Scholar
Cords, M. (2000a). The agonistic and affiliative relationships of adult females in a blue monkey group. In Old World Monkeys, ed. Jolly, C. and Whitehead, P.. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 453–79.Google Scholar
Cords, M. (2000b). The number of males in guenon groups. In Primate Males: Causes and Consequences of Variation in Group Composition, ed. Kappeler, P. M.. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 84–96.Google Scholar
Cords, M. (2002a). Friendship among adult blue monkeys (Cercopithecus mitis). Behaviour, 139, 291–314.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cords, M. (2002b). When are there influxes in blue monkey groups? In The Guenons: Diversity and Adaptation in African Monkeys, ed. Glenn, M. E. and Cords, M.. New York: Kluwer, pp 189–201.Google Scholar
Eckardt, W. and Zuberbühler, K. (2004). Cooperation and competition in forest monkeys. Behavioural Ecology, 15: 400–12.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fashing, P. and Cords, M. (2000). Diurnal primate densities and biomass in the Kakamega Forest: an evaluation of census methods and a comparison with other forests. American Journal of Primatology, 50, 139–52.3.0.CO;2-N>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fimbel, C. (1994). The relative use of abandoned farm clearings and old forest habitats by primates and a forest antelope at Tiwai, Sierra Leone, West Africa. Biological Conservation, 70, 277–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gautier, J.-P. and Gautier, A. (1977). Communication in Old World monkeys. In How Animals Communicate, ed. Seboek, T. A.. Bloomington, IN: Indiana University Press, pp. 890–964.Google Scholar
Gautier, J.-P. and Gautier-Hion, A. (1969). Les associations chez les Cercopithecidae du Gabon. Terre et Vie, 23, 164–201.Google Scholar
Gautier-Hion, A. (1988). Polyspecific associations among forest guenons: ecological, behavioural and evolutionary aspects. In A Primate Radiation: Evolutionary Biology of the African Guenons, ed. Gautier-Hion, A., Bourlière, F., Gautier, J.-P. and Kingdon, J.. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 452–76.Google Scholar
Gautier-Hion, A. and Gautier, J.-P. (1974). Les associations polyspecifique des Cercopitheques du plateau de M'passa, Gabon. Folia Primatologica, 22, 134–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Glenn, M. E. (1996). The natural history and ecology of the mona monkey (Cercopithecus mona Schreber 1774) on the island of Grenada, West Indies. Ph.D. Thesis, Northwestern University, Evansville, IL, USA.Google Scholar
Glenn, M. (1997). Group size and group composition of the mona monkey (Cercopithecus mona) on the island of Grenada, West Indies. American Journal of Primatology, 43, 167–73.3.0.CO;2-V>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Glenn, M., Matsuda, R. and Bensen, K. (2002). Unique behavior of the Mona Monkey (Cercopithecus mona): all-male groups and copulation calls. In The Guenons: Diversity and Adaptation in African Monkeys, ed. Glenn, M. E. and Cords, M.. New York: Kluwer Academic Publishers, pp. 133–45.Google Scholar
Hill, C. M. (1994). The role of female Diana monkeys (Cercopithecus diana) in territorial defence. Animal Behaviour, 47, 425–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Höner, O. P., Leumann, L. and Noë, R. (1997). Dyadic associations of red colobus and Diana monkeys in the Taï National Park, Ivory Coast. Primates, 38, 281–91.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hunkeler, C., Bourlière, F. and Bertrand, M. (1972). Le comportement social de la mone de Lowe (Cercopithecus campbelli lowei). Folia Primatologica, 17, 218–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Janson, C. H. and Goldsmith, M. L. (1995). Predicting group size in primates: foraging costs and predation risks. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 5, 326–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jolly, A. (1985). The Evolution of Primate Behaviour, 2nd ed. New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Kingdon, J. (1997). The Kingdon Field Guide to African Mammals. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Korstjens, A. H. (2001). The Mob, the Secret Sorority, and the Phantoms: an Analysis of the SocioEcological Strategies of the Three Colobines of Taï. Ph.D. Thesis, Utrecht University, Utrecht, The Netherlands.Google Scholar
Lawes, M. J. and Henzi, S. P. (1995). Inter-group encounters in blue monkeys: how territorial must a territorial species be?Animal Behaviour, 49, 240–3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lemasson, A., Gautier, J.-P. and Hausberger, M. (2003). Vocal similarities and social bonds in Campbell's monkeys (Cercopithecus campbelli campbelli). Comptes Rendus Biologies, 326, 185–93.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lemasson, A. and Hausberger, M. (2004). Patterns of vocal sharing and social dynamics in a captive group of Campbell's monkeys (Cercopithecus campbelli campbelli). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 118, 347–59.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Macleod, M. C. (2000). The Reproductive Strategies of Samango Monkeys (Cercopithecus mitis erythrarchus). Ph.D. Thesis, University of Surrey, Roehampton, UK.Google Scholar
Macleod, M. C., Ross, C. and Lawes, M. J. (2002). Costs and benefits of alternative mating strategies in samango monkey males. In The Guenons: Diversity and Adaptation in African Monkeys, ed. Glenn, M. E. and Cords, M.. New York: Kluwer, pp. 203–16.Google Scholar
McGraw, W. S. (1996). The Positional Behavior and Habitat Use of Six Monkeys in the Taï Forest, Ivory Coast. Ph.D. Thesis, State University of New York, Stony Brook, USA.Google Scholar
McGraw, W. S., Plavcan, M. J. and Adachi-Kanawaza, K. (2002). Adult female Cercopithecus diana employ canine teeth to kill another C. diana female. International Journal Primatology, 23, 1301–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mitani, M. (1991). Niche overlap and polyspecific associations among sympatric cercopithecids in the Campo Animal Reserve, southwestern Cameroon. Primates, 32, 137–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Noë, R. and Bshary, R. (1997). The formation of red colobus-Diana monkey associations under predation pressure from chimpanzees. Proceedings Royal Society London B, 264, 253–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oates, J. F. (1988). The distribution of Cercopithecus monkeys in West African forests. In A Primate Radiation: Evolutionary Biology of the African Guenons, ed. Gautier-Hion, A., Bourlière, F., Gautier, J.-P. and Kingdon, J.. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 79–103.Google Scholar
Oates, J. F. and Whitesides, G. H. (1990). Association between olive colobus (Procolobus verus), Diana guenons (Cercopithecus diana), and other forest monkeys in Sierra Leone. American Journal of Primatology, 21, 129–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oates, J. F., Whitesides, G. H., Davies, A. G.et al. (1990). Determinants of variation in tropical forest primate biomass: new evidence from West Africa. Ecology, 71, 328–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Payne, H. F. P., Lawes, M. J. and Henzi, S. P. (2003). Competition and the exchange of grooming among female samango monkeys (Cercopithecus mitis erythrarchus). Behaviour, 140, 453–72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pazol, K. (2001). Social, ecological and endocrine influences on female relationships in blue monkeys (Cercopithecus mitis stuhlmanni). Ph.D. Thesis, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, USA.
Pusey, A. E. and Packer, C. (1987). Dispersal and philopatry. In Primate Societies, ed. Smuts, B. B., Cheney, D. L., Seyfarth, R. M., Wrangham, R. W. and Struhsaker, T. T.. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, pp. 250–66.Google Scholar
Rowell, T. E. (1988). The social system of guenons compared with baboons, macaques, and mangabeys. In A Primate Radiation: Evolutionary Biology of the African Guenons, ed. Gautier-Hion, A., Bourlière, F., Gautier, J.-P. and Kingdon, J.. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 439–51.Google Scholar
Rowell, T. E., Wilson, C. and Cords, M. (1991). Reciprocity and partner preference in grooming of female blue monkeys. International Journal of Primatology, 12, 319–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seyfarth, R. M. (1976). Social relationships among adult female baboons. Animal Behaviour, 24, 917–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Siegel, S. and Castellan, N. J. Jr. (1988). Nonparametric Statistics for the Behavioral Sciences, 2nd ed. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Smuts, B. B., Cheney, D. L., Seyfarth, R. M., Wrangham, R. W. and Struhsaker, T. T. (1987). Primate Societies, Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Srivastava, A., Borries, C. and Sommer, V. (1991). Homosexual mounting in free-ranging female Hanuman langurs (Presbytis entellus). Archives Sexual Behavior, 20, 487–512.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sterck, E. H. M. and Steenbeeck, R. (1997). Female dominance relationships and food competition in the sympatric Thomas langur and long-tailed macaques. Behaviour, 134, 749–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sterck, E. H. M., Watts, D. P. and Schaik, C. P. (1997). The evolution of female social relationships in nonhuman primates. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 41, 291–309.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Struhsaker, T. T. (1969). Correlates of ecology and social organization among African cercopithecines. Folia Primatologica, 11, 80–118.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Struhsaker, T. T. (1981). Polyspecific associations among tropical rain forest primates. Zietschrift für Tierpsychchologie, 57, 268–304.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schaik, C. P. (1983). Why are diurnal primates living in groups?Behaviour, 87, 120–44.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Schaik, C. P. (1989). The ecology of social relationships amongst female primates. In Comparative Socioecology: The Behavioural Ecology of Humans and Other Mammals, ed. Standen, V. and Foley, R.. Oxford: Blackwell, pp. 195–218.Google Scholar
Schaik, C. P. and Hoof, J. (1983). On the ultimate causes of primate social systems. Behaviour, 85, 91–117.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schaik, C. P. and Horstermann, M. (1994). Predation risk and the number of adult males in a primate group: a comparative test. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 35, 273–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schaik, C. P. and Noordwijk, M. A. (1985). Evolutionary effect of the absence of felids on the social organisation of macaques on the island of Simeuleu (Macaca fascicularis). Folia Primatologica, 44, 138–44.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tsingalia, H. M. and Rowell, T. E. (1984). The behaviour of adult male blue monkeys. Zietschrift für Tierpsychchologie, 64, 253–68.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
White, L. J. T. (1994). Biomass of rain forest mammals in the Lope reserve, Gabon. Journal of Animal Ecology, 63, 499–512.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whitesides, G. H. (1981). Community and population ecology of non-human primates in the Douala-Edea forest reserve. M.Sc. Thesis, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, USA.
Whitesides, G. H., Oates, J. F., Green, S. M. and Kluberdanz, R. P. (1988). Estimating primate densities from transects in a West African rain forest: a comparison of techniques. Journal of Animal Ecology, 57, 345–67.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wolters, S. and Zuberbühler, K. (2003). Mixed-species associations of Diana and Campbell's monkeys: the costs and benefits of a forest phenomenon. Behaviour, 140, 371–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wrangham, R. W. (1980). An ecological model of female-bonded primate groups. Behaviour, 75, 262–300.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wrangham, R. W., Gittleman, J. L. and Chapman, C. A. (1993). Constraints on group size in primates and carnivores: population density and day range as assays of exploitation competition. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 32, 199–209.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zuberbühler, K. (2001). Predator-specific alarm calls in Campbell's guenons. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 50, 414–22.Google Scholar
Zuberbühler, K. and Jenny, D. (2002). Leopard predation and primate evolution. Journal of Human Evolution, 43, 873–86.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zuberbühler, K., Noë, R. and Seyfarth, R. M. (1997). Diana monkey long-distance calls: messages for conspecifics and predators. Animal Behaviour, 53, 589–604.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×