Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-94d59 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-27T02:29:27.324Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Ancestral human mother–infant interaction was an adaptation that gave rise to music and dance

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  30 September 2021

Ellen Dissanayake*
Affiliation:
School of Music, University of Washington, Seattle, WA98195-3450, USAed3@uw.edu

Abstract

Human infants are born ready to respond to affiliative signals of a caretaker's face, body, and voice. This ritualized behavior in ancestral mothers and infants was an adaptation that gave rise to music and dance as exaptations for promoting group ritual and other social bonding behaviors, arguing for an evolutionary relationship between mother and infant bonding and both music and dance.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Aitken, K., & Trevarthen, C. (1997). Self-other organization in human psychological development. Development and Psychopathology 9, 653677.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beebe, B. (1982). Micro-timing in mother–infant communication. In Key, M. R. (Ed.), Nonverbal communication today (pp. 168195). Mouton.Google Scholar
Dissanayake, E. (2018). From play and ritualization to ritual and its arts: Sources of Upper Pleistocene ritual practices in Lower Middle Pleistocene ritualized and play behaviors in ancestral hominins. In Renfrew, C., Morley, I. & Boyd, M. (Eds.), Ritual, play and belief, in evolution and early human societies (pp. 8798). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. (1989). Human ethology. Aldine de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Field, T. M., Sostek, A. M., Vietze, , & Leidermann, P. H. (Eds.) (1981). Culture and early interactions. Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Hrdy, S. B. (2009). Mothers and others: The evolutionary origin of mutual understanding. Harvard.Google Scholar
Jaffe, J., Beebe, B., Feldstein, S., Crown, C. L., & Jasnow, M. D. (2001). Rhythms of dialogue in infancy. Blackwell.Google ScholarPubMed
Koulomzin, M., Beebe, B., Anderson, S., Jaffe, J., Feldstein, A., & Crown, C. (2002). Infant gaze, head, face and self-touch at 4 months differentiate secure vs. avoidant attachment at 1 year: A microanalytic approach. Attachment and Human Development, 4(1), 324.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leiderman, P. H., Tuckerman, S. R., & Rosenfeld, A. (Eds.) (1977) Culture and infancy. Academic Press.Google Scholar
Murray, L., & Trevarthen, C. (1985) Emotional regulation of interactions between two-month-olds and their mothers. In Field, T. M. & Fox, N. A. (Eds.), Social perception in infants (pp. 177–197). Ablex.Google Scholar
Nadel, J., Carchon, I., Kervella, C., Marcelli, D., & Reserbet-Planty, D. (1999) Expectancies for social contingency in 2-month-olds. Developmental Science, 2, 164173.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schore, A. N. (1994) Affect regulation and the origin of the self: The neurobiology of emotional development. Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Stern, D. (1985) The interpersonal world of the infant. Basic Books.Google Scholar
Trevarthen, C. (1999) Musicality and the intrinsic motive pulse: Evidence from human psychobiology and infant communication. Musicæ Scientiæ [Special Issue 1999–2000], 155215.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trevarthen, C., & Aitken, K. (1994) Brain development, infant communication, and empathy disorders: Intrinsic factors in child mental health. Developmental Psychopathology, 6, 597633.CrossRefGoogle Scholar