Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-wg55d Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-20T22:14:43.478Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Music, bonding, and human evolution: A critique

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  30 September 2021

Bjorn Merker*
Affiliation:
Independent Scholar, SE-29194Kristianstad, Sweden. bjornmerker@gmail.com

Abstract

Savage et al. propose that music filled a hypothetical “bonding gap” in human sociality by Baldwinian gene-culture coevolution (or protracted cognitive niche construction). Both these stepping stones to an evolutionary account of the function and origin of music are problematic. They are scrutinized in this commentary, and an alternative is proposed.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Darwin, C. (1871). The descent of man and selection in relation to sex. D. Appleton & Co.Google Scholar
Dunbar, R. I. M. (1992). Neocortex size as a constraint on group size in primates. Journal of Human Evolution, 20, 469493.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunbar, R. I. M. (1993). Coevolution of neocortical size, group size and language in humans. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 16, 681735.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunbar, R. I. M. (1998). The social brain hypothesis. Evolutionary Anthropology, 6, 178190.3.0.CO;2-8>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunbar, R. I. M. (2012a). On the evolutionary function of song and dance. In Bannan, N. (Ed.), Music, language, and human evolution (pp. 201214). Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunbar, R. I. M. (2012b). Bridging the bonding gap: The transition from primates to humans. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Science, 367, 18371846.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ember, C. R. (1978). Myths about hunter-gatherers. Ethnology, 17, 439448. doi: 10.2307/3773193.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Enquist, M., Arak, A., Ghirlanda, S., & Wachtmeister, C.-A. (2002). Spectacular phenomena and limits to rationality in genetic and cultural evolution. Transactions of the Royal Society of London B, 357, 15851594.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fisher, R. A. (1930). The genetical theory of natural selection. Clarendon Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ghiglieri, M. P. (1984). The chimpanzees of Kibale forest. Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
Ghiglieri, M. P. (1987). Sociobiology of the great apes and the hominid ancestor. Journal of Human Evolution, 16, 319357. doi: 10.1016/0047-2484(87)90065-0.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Janik, V. M., & Slater, P. J. B. (1997). Vocal learning in mammals. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 26, 5999.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Janik, V. M., & Slater, P. J. B. (2000). The different roles of social learning in vocal communication. Animal Behaviour, 60, 111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Launay, J., Tarr, B., & Dunbar, R. I. M. (2016). Synchrony as an adaptive mechanism for large-scale human social bonding. Ethology, 122, 779789.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lehmann, J., Korstjens, A. H., & Dunbar, R. I. M. (2007). Group size, grooming and social cohesion in primates. Animal Behavior, 74, 16171629. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2006.10.025.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lovejoy, C. O. (2009). Reexamining human origins in light of Ardipithecus ramidus. Science, 326(5949), 74e174e8. doi: 10.1126/science.1175834.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Merker, B. (1999). Synchronous chorusing and the origins of music. Musicae Scientiae, Special Issue, Rhythm, Musical narrative, and Origins of Human Communication, pp. 59–74. [these special issues have no volume number!!!].CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Merker, B. (2000). Synchronous chorusing and human origins. In Wallin, N. L., Merker, B. & Brown, S. (Eds.), The origins of music (pp. 315328). The M.I.T. Press.Google Scholar
Merker, B. (2019). When extravagance impresses: Recasting esthetics in evolutionary terms. In Thaut, M. & Hodges, D. (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of music and the Brain (pp. 6686). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Merker, B., Madison, G., & Eckerdal, P. (2009). On the role and origin of isochrony in human rhythmic entrainment. Cortex, 45, 417.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Merker, B., Morley, I., & Zuidema, W. (2018). Five fundamental constraints on theories of the origins of music. In Honing, H. (Ed.), The origins of musicality (pp. 4980). The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Miller, G. F. (1998). How mate choice shaped human nature: A review of sexual selection and human evolution. In Crawford, C. & Krebs, D. (Eds.), Handbook of evolutionary psychology: Ideas, issues, and applications (pp. 87130). Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Nottebohm, F. (1976). Discussion paper. Vocal tract and brain: A search for evolutionary bottlenecks. In Harnad, S. R., Steklis, H. D. & Lancaster, J. (Eds.), Origins and evolution of language and speech. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 280 (pp. 643649). New York Academy of Sciences.Google Scholar
Patel, A. D. (2018). Music as a transformative technology of the mind: An update. In Honing, H. (Ed.), The origins of musicality (pp. 113126). The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Podlipniak, P. (2017). The role of the Baldwin effect in the evolution of human musicality. Frontiers in Neuroscience 11(542), 112. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2017.00542.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reynolds, V., & Reynolds, R. (1965). Chimpanzees of the Budongo forest. In Devore, I. (Ed.), Primate behavior: Field studies of monkeys and apes (pp. 368424). Holt, Rinehart & Winston.Google Scholar
Snyder-Mackler, N., Beehner, J. C., & Bergman, T. J. (2012). Defining higher levels in the multilevel societies of geladas (Theropithecus gelada). International Journal of Primatology 33, 10541068. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10764-012-9584-5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sugiyama, Y. (1969). Social behavior of chimpanzees in the Budongo Forest, Uganda. Primates, 9, 225258. doi:10.1007/bf01730343.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sugiyama, Y. (1972). Social characteristics and socialization of wild chimpanzees. In Poirer, F. E. (Ed.), Primate socialization (pp. 145163). Random House.Google Scholar
Tarr, B., Launay, J., & Dunbar, R. I. M. (2014). Music and social bonding: “Self-other” merging and neurohormonal mechanisms. Frontiers in Psychology 5, article 1096, 110. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01096.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Turchin, P., Currie, T. E., Whitehouse, H., François, P., Feeney, K., Mullins, D., … Spencer, C. (2018). Quantitative historical analysis uncovers a single dimension of complexity that structures global variation in human social organization. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 115, E144E151 https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1708800115.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wachtmeister, C.-A. (2001). Display in monogamous pairs: A review of empirical data and evolutionary explanations. Animal Behaviour, 61, 861868.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wachtmeister, C.-A., & Enquist, M. (2000). The evolution of courtship rituals in monogamous species. Behavioral Ecology, 11, 405410.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wrangham, R. W. (1975). The behavioural ecology of chimpanzees in Gombe National Park, Tanzania. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK.Google Scholar
Wrangham, R. W. (1979). On the evolution of ape social systems. Social Science International, 18, 335368.Google Scholar
Zahavi, A. (1975). Mate selection – a selection for a handicap. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 53, 205214.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed