Skip to main content
×
Home
    • Aa
    • Aa
  • Get access
    Check if you have access via personal or institutional login
  • Cited by 42
  • Cited by
    This article has been cited by the following publications. This list is generated based on data provided by CrossRef.

    Bringsjord, Selmer Licato, John and Bringsjord, Alexander 2016. Integrating Cognitive Architectures into Virtual Character Design.


    Gonzalez, Cleotilde and Mehlhorn, Katja 2016. Framing From Experience: Cognitive Processes and Predictions of Risky Choice. Cognitive Science, Vol. 40, Issue. 5, p. 1163.


    Ferhat, Ensar 2015. Schema-based text comprehension. Educational Research and Reviews, Vol. 10, Issue. 18, p. 2568.


    Frey, Renato Mata, Rui and Hertwig, Ralph 2015. The role of cognitive abilities in decisions from experience: Age differences emerge as a function of choice set size. Cognition, Vol. 142, p. 60.


    Glodek, Michael Honold, Frank Geier, Thomas Krell, Gerald Nothdurft, Florian Reuter, Stephan Schüssel, Felix Hörnle, Thilo Dietmayer, Klaus Minker, Wolfgang Biundo, Susanne Weber, Michael Palm, Günther and Schwenker, Friedhelm 2015. Fusion paradigms in cognitive technical systems for human–computer interaction. Neurocomputing, Vol. 161, p. 17.


    Gonzalez, Cleotilde Ben-Asher, Noam Martin, Jolie M. and Dutt, Varun 2015. A Cognitive Model of Dynamic Cooperation With Varied Interdependency Information. Cognitive Science, Vol. 39, Issue. 3, p. 457.


    Hertwig, Ralph 2015. The Wiley Blackwell Handbook of Judgment and Decision Making.


    Plechawska-Wojcik, Malgorzata 2015. 2015 8th International Conference on Human System Interaction (HSI). p. 68.

    Walsh, Matthew M. and Gluck, Kevin A. 2015. Mechanisms for Robust Cognition. Cognitive Science, Vol. 39, Issue. 6, p. 1131.


    Franklin, Stan Madl, Tamas D'Mello, Sidney and Snaider, Javier 2014. LIDA: A Systems-level Architecture for Cognition, Emotion, and Learning. IEEE Transactions on Autonomous Mental Development, Vol. 6, Issue. 1, p. 19.


    Grotto, Rosapia Lauro 2014. 2014 IEEE 13th International Conference on Cognitive Informatics and Cognitive Computing. p. 190.

    Hélie, Sébastien and Sun, Ron 2014. Autonomous learning in psychologically-oriented cognitive architectures: A survey. New Ideas in Psychology, Vol. 34, p. 37.


    Oltramari, Alessandro Ben-Asher, Noam Cranor, Lorrie Bauer, Lujo and Christin, Nicolas 2014. 2014 IEEE Military Communications Conference. p. 129.

    Schultheis, Holger Bertel, Sven and Barkowsky, Thomas 2014. Modeling Mental Spatial Reasoning About Cardinal Directions. Cognitive Science, Vol. 38, Issue. 8, p. 1521.


    Spraragen, Marc and Madni, Azad M. 2014. Modeling of Emotional Effects on Decision-making by Game Agents. Procedia Computer Science, Vol. 28, p. 736.


    Taatgen, Niels A. 2014. Between architecture and model: Strategies for cognitive control. Biologically Inspired Cognitive Architectures, Vol. 8, p. 132.


    Gonzalez, Cleotilde Dutt, Varun and Lebiere, Christian 2013. Validating instance-based learning mechanisms outside of ACT-R. Journal of Computational Science, Vol. 4, Issue. 4, p. 262.


    Rachkovskij, Dmitri A. Kussul, Ernst M. and Baidyk, Tatiana N. 2013. Building a world model with structure-sensitive sparse binary distributed representations. Biologically Inspired Cognitive Architectures, Vol. 3, p. 64.


    Reddy, R. Kanaparthi, B. R. and Dutt, V. 2013. 2013 3rd IEEE International Advance Computing Conference (IACC). p. 1369.

    Byrne, Michael D. 2012. Unified theories of cognition. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, Vol. 3, Issue. 4, p. 431.


    ×

The Newell Test for a theory of cognition

  • John R. Anderson (a1) and Christian Lebiere (a2)
  • DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X0300013X
  • Published online: 01 October 2003
Abstract

Newell (1980; 1990) proposed that cognitive theories be developed in an effort to satisfy multiple criteria and to avoid theoretical myopia. He provided two overlapping lists of 13 criteria that the human cognitive architecture would have to satisfy in order to be functional. We have distilled these into 12 criteria: flexible behavior, real-time performance, adaptive behavior, vast knowledge base, dynamic behavior, knowledge integration, natural language, learning, development, evolution, and brain realization. There would be greater theoretical progress if we evaluated theories by a broad set of criteria such as these and attended to the weaknesses such evaluations revealed. To illustrate how theories can be evaluated we apply these criteria to both classical connectionism (McClelland & Rumelhart 1986; Rumelhart & McClelland 1986b) and the ACT-R theory (Anderson & Lebiere 1998). The strengths of classical connectionism on this test derive from its intense effort in addressing empirical phenomena in such domains as language and cognitive development. Its weaknesses derive from its failure to acknowledge a symbolic level to thought. In contrast, ACT-R includes both symbolic and subsymbolic components. The strengths of the ACT-R theory derive from its tight integration of the symbolic component with the subsymbolic component. Its weaknesses largely derive from its failure, as yet, to adequately engage in intensive analyses of issues related to certain criteria on Newell's list.

Copyright
Recommend this journal

Email your librarian or administrator to recommend adding this journal to your organisation's collection.

Behavioral and Brain Sciences
  • ISSN: 0140-525X
  • EISSN: 1469-1825
  • URL: /core/journals/behavioral-and-brain-sciences
Please enter your name
Please enter a valid email address
Who would you like to send this to? *
×

Keywords: