Skip to main content
×
×
Home

In your face: Emotional expressivity as a predictor of ideology

  • Johnathan Caleb Peterson (a1), Carly Jacobs (a2), John Hibbing (a3) and Kevin Smith (a4)
Abstract

Research suggests that people can accurately predict the political affiliations of others using only information extracted from the face. It is less clear from this research, however, what particular facial physiological processes or features communicate such information. Using a model of emotion developed in psychology that treats emotional expressivity as an individual-level trait, this article provides a theoretical account of why emotional expressivity may provide reliable signals of political orientation, and it tests the theory in four empirical studies. We find statistically significant liberal/conservative differences in self-reported emotional expressivity, in facial emotional expressivity measured physiologically, in the perceived emotional expressivity and ideology of political elites, and in an experiment that finds that more emotionally expressive faces are perceived as more liberal.

Copyright
Corresponding author
Correspondence: Kevin Smith, Political Science, University of Nebraska-Lincoln, Lincoln, Nebraska, United States, 68588. Email: ksmith1@unl.edu
References
Hide All
1 Friedman, S. H., Mertz, T. I., and DiMatteo, M. R., “Perceived bias in the facial expression of television news broadcasters,” Journal of Communication , 1980, 30(4): 103111.
2 Jahoda, G., “Political attitudes and judgements of other people,” Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology , 1954, 49: 331334.
3 Koppensteiner, M. and Grammer, K., “Motion patterns in political speech and their influence on personality ratings,” Journal of Research in Personality , 2010, 44(3): 374379.
4 Laustsen, L., “Decomposing the relationship between candidates’ facial appearance and electoral success,” Political Behavior , 2014, 36(4): 777791.
5 McHugo, G. J. and Smith, C. A., “The power of faces: A review of John T. Lanzetta’s research on facial expression and emotion,” Motivation & Emotion , 1996, 2(2): 85120.
6 Olivola, C. and Todorov, A., “Elected in 100 milliseconds: Appearance-based trait inferences and voting,” Journal of Nonverbal Behavior , 2010a, 34(2): 83100.
7 Olivola, C. and Todorov, A., “Fooled by first impressions? Reexamining the diagnostic value of appearance-based inferences,” Journal of Experimental Psychology , 2010b, 46(2): 315324.
8 Olivola, C., Sussman, A. B., Tsetsos, K., Kang, O. E., and Todorov, A., “Republicans prefer Republican-looking leaders: Political facial stereotypes predict candidate electoral success among right-leaning voters,” Social Psychological and Personality Science , 2012, 3(5): 605613.
9 Sullivan, D. G. and Masters, R. D., “‘Happy warriors’: Leaders’ facial displays, viewers’ emotions, and political support,” American Journal of Political Science , 1988, 32(2): 345368.
10 Stewart, P. and Dowe, P. K., “Interpreting President Barack Obama’s facial displays of emotion: Revisiting the Dartmouth Group,” Political Psychology , 2013, 34(3): 369385.
11 Todorov, A., Said, C. P., Engel, A. D., and Oosterhof, N., “Understanding evaluation of faces on social dimensions,” Trends in Cognitive Sciences , 2008, 12(12): 455460.
12 Ambady, N., Bernieri, F. J., and Richeson, J. A., “Toward a histology of social behavior: Judgmental accuracy from thin slices of the behavioral system,” in Advances in Social Psychology, Zanna, M. P., ed. (San Diego, CA: Academic Press, 2000), pp. 211271.
13 Olivola and Todorov, 2010b.
14 Tskhay, K. and Rule, N., “Accuracy in categorizing perceptually ambiguous groups: A review and meta-analysis,” Personality and Social Psychology Review , 2013, 17(1): 7286.
15 Bull, R. and Hawkes, C., “Judging politicians by their faces,” Political Studies , 1982, 30(1): 95101.
16 Carpinella, C. M. and Johnson, K. L., “Appearance-based politics: Sex-type facial cues communicate political party affiliation,” Journal of Experimental Social Psychology , 2013, 49: 156160.
17 Olivola and Todorov, 2010a.
18 Olivola and Todorov, 2010b.
19 Roberts, T., Griffin, H., McOwan, P. W., and Johnston, A., “Judging political affiliation from faces of UK MPs,” Perception , 2011, 40(8): 949952.
20 Rule, N. and Ambady, N., “Democrats and Republicans can be differentiated from their faces,” PLOS ONE , 2010, 5: e8733.
21 Samochowiec, J., Wanke, M., and Fiedler, K., “Political ideology at face value,” Social Psychological and Personality Science , 2010, 1(3): 206213.
22 Herrmann, M. and Shikano, S., “Attractiveness and facial competence bias face-based inferences of candidate ideology,” Political Psychology , 2016, 37(3): 401417, at p. 402.
23 Herrmann and Shikano, p. 414.
24 Samochowiec, Wanke, and Fiedler, 2010.
25 Olivola and Todorov, 2010b.
26 Carpinella and Johnson, 2013.
27 Olivola and Todorov, 2010b.
28 Samochowiec, Wanke, and Fiedler, 2010.
29 Todorov, A., Olivola, C., Dotsch, R., and Mende-Sidlecki, P., “Social attributions from faces: Determinants, consequences, accuracy, and functional significance,” Annual Review of Psychology , 2015, 66: 519545.
30 Todorov, A. and Duchaine, B., “Reading trustworthiness in faces without recognizing faces,” Cognitive Neuropsychology , 2008, 25(3): 116.
31 Todorov et al. , 2008.
32 Ballew, C. C. and Todorov, A., “Predicting political elections from rapid and unreflective face judgments,” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences , 2008, 104(46): 1794817953.
33 Laustsen, 2014.
34 Poutvaara, P., Jordahl, H., and Berggren, N., “Faces of politicians: Babyfacedness predicts inferred competence but not electoral success,” Journal of Experimental Psychology , 2009, 45(5): 11321135.
35 Verhulst, G., Lodge, M., and Lavine, H., “The attractiveness halo: Why some candidates are perceived more favorably than others,” Journal of Nonverbal Behavior , 2010, 34(2): 111117.
36 Olivola et al. , 2012.
37 Rule and Ambady, 2010.
38 Olivola, C., Tingley, D., and Todorov, A., The Donkey in Elephant’s Clothing: Revising the Impact and Validity of Political Facial Stereotypes (Pittsburgh, PA: Tepper, School of Business, Carnegie Mellon University, 2014).
39 Todorov et al. , 2015, p. 532.
40 Olivola and Todorov, 2010b, p. 323.
41 Samochoweic, Wanke, and Fiedler, 2010, p. 211.
42 Schmidt, K. L. and Cohn, J. F., “Human facial expressions as adaptations: Evolutionary questions in facial expression research,” American Journal of Physical Anthropology , 2001, Suppl. 33: 324.
43 Ekman, P., “Facial expressions,” in The Handbook of Cognition and Emotion, Dalgleish, T. and Power, M., eds. (New York: Wiley, 1999), pp. 301320.
44 Cohn, J., Schmidt, K., Gross, R., and Ekman, P., “Individual differences in facial expression: Stability over time, relation to self-reported emotion, and ability to inform person identification,” in paper presented at the 4th IEEE International Conference on Multimodal Interfaces (Pittsburgh, PA: 2002).
45 Schmidt and Cohn, 2001, p. 17.
46 Van Vugt, M. and Allen, G. E., “The many faces of leadership,” Current Directions in Psychological Science , 2015, 6: 484489.
47 Laustsen, L. and Petersen, M. B., “Does a competent leader make a good friend? Conflict, ideology and the psychologies of friendship and followership Evolution and Human Behavior , 2015, 36(4): 286293.
48 Todorov et al. , 2015.
49 Kring, A. M. and Gordon, A. H., “Sex differences in emotion: expression, experience and physiology,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 1998, 74(3): 686703.
50 Hareli, S., Shomrat, N., and Hess, U., “Emotional versus neutral expressions and perceptions of social dominance and submissiveness,” Emotion , 2009, 9(3): 378384.
51 Gross, J. J. and Oliver, J. P., “Facets of emotional expressivity: Three self-report factors and their correlates,” Personality and Individual Differences , 1995, 19(4): 555568.
52 Gross, J. J. and Oliver, J. P., “Revealing feelings: Facets of emotional expressivity in self-reports, peer ratings and behavior,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 1998, 72(2): 435448.
53 Gross, J. J. and Oliver, J. P., “Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes: implications for affect, relationships and well-being,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 2003, 85(2): 348362.
54 Leone, L. and Chirumbolo, A., “Conservatism as motivated avoidance of affect: Need for affect scales predict conservatism measures,” Journal of Research in Personality , 2008, 42(3): 755762.
55 Constantini, E. and Craik, K. H., “Personality and politicians: California party leaders, 1960–1976,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 1980, 38(4): 641661.
56 Pliskin, R., Bar-Tal, D., Sheppes, G., and Halperin, E., “Are leftists more emotion-driven than rightests? The interactive influence of ideology and emotions on support for policies,” Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin , 2014, 40(12): 16811697.
57 Gross and Oliver, 1995.
58 Hibbing, J. R., Smith, K. B., and Alford, J. R., Predisposed (New York: Routledge, 2013).
59 Gross and Oliver, 1995.
60 Gross and Oliver, 1998.
61 Gross and Oliver, 1995.
62 Gross and Oliver, 1998.
63 Gross and Oliver, 1995.
64 Napier, J. L. and Jost, J. T., “Why are conservatives happier than liberals? Psychological Science , 2008, 19(6): 565572.
65 Schenckler, B. R., Chambers, J. R., and Le, B. M., “Conservatives are happier than liberals, but why? Political ideology, personality, and life satisfaction,” Journal of Research in Personality , 2012, 46(2): 127146.
66 Gross and Oliver, 1995, p. 558.
67 Newport, F., “American’s church attendance inches up in 2010,” Gallup, June 25, 2010, http://www.gallup.com/poll/141044/americans-church-attendance-inches-2010.aspx (accessed November 28, 2017).
68 Larson, J. T., Norris, C. J., and Cacioppo, J. T., “Effects of positive and negative affect on electromyographic activity over zygomaticus major and corrugator supercilii,” Psychophysiology , 2003, 40(5): 776785.
69 Hibbing, J. R., Smith, K. B., and Alford, J. R., “Differences in negativity bias underlie variations in political ideology,” Behavioral and Brain Sciences , 2014, 37(3): 297350.
70 Gross, J. J., “Antecedent- and response-focused emotion regulation: Divergent consequences for experience, expression, and physiology,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 1998, 74(1): 224237.
71 Gross, J. J. and Levenson, R., “Emotional suppression: Physiology, self-report, and expressive behavior,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 1993, 64(6): 970986.
72 Greenwald, M. K., Cook, E. W., and Lang, P. J., “Affective judgment and psychophysiological response: Dimensional covariation in the evaluation of pictorial stimuli,” Journal of Pyschophysiology , 1989, 3(1): 5164.
73 Kreibig, S. D., Wilhelm, F. H., Roth, W. T., and Gross, J., “Cardiovascular, electrodermal and respiratory response patterns to fear- and sadness-inducing films,” Psychophysiology , 2017, 44(5): 787806.
74 Tassinary, L. G., Caccioppo, J. T., and Vanman, E., “The skeletomotor system: Surface electromyography,” in Handbook of Psychophysiology, 3rd ed. Caccioppo, J. T., Tassinary, L. G., and Berntson, G. G., eds. (New York: Cambridge University Press, 2007), pp. 267299.
75 Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., and Cuthbert, B. N., “International affective picture systems (IAPS): Affective ratings of pictures and instruction manual,” Technical Report A-6, 2005, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL.
76 Larson, Norris, and Cacioppo, 2003.
77 Hix, S., Noury, A., and Roland, G., “Roll call votes in the European Parliament. July 26,” 2011, http://personal.lse.ac.uk/hix/HixNouryRolandEPdata.HTM (accessed September 15, 2014).
78For a survey, see Todorov et al., 2015.
79 Oosterhof, N. and Todorov, A., “The functional basis of face evaluation,” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences , 2009, 105: 1108711092.
80 Oosterhof and Todoro, A., 2009.
81 Gross and Oliver, 1995.
82 Todorov et al. , 2015.
83 Gerring, J., “Ideology: A definitional analysis,” Political Research Quarterly , 1997, 50(4): 957994.
84 Jost, J. T., Federico, C. M., and Napier, J. L., “Political ideology: Its structure, functions and elective affinities,” Annual Review of Psychology , 2009, 60: 307337.
85 Feldman, S. and Johnston, C., “Understanding the determinants of political ideology: Implications of structural complexity,” Political Psychology , 2014, 35(3): 337358.
86 Ekman, P. and Friesen, V. W., “Felt, false, and miserable smiles,” Journal of Nonverbal Behavior , 1982, 6(4): 238252.
87 Stewart and Dowe, 2013.
88 Stewart, P., Waller, B., and Schubert, J., “Presidential speechmaking style: Emotional response to micro-expressions of facial affect,” Motivation and Emotion , 2009, 33(2): 125135.
89 Laustsen and Petersen, 2015.
Recommend this journal

Email your librarian or administrator to recommend adding this journal to your organisation's collection.

Politics and the Life Sciences
  • ISSN: 0730-9384
  • EISSN: 1471-5457
  • URL: /core/journals/politics-and-the-life-sciences
Please enter your name
Please enter a valid email address
Who would you like to send this to? *
×

Keywords

Metrics

Altmetric attention score

Full text views

Total number of HTML views: 18
Total number of PDF views: 68 *
Loading metrics...

Abstract views

Total abstract views: 876 *
Loading metrics...

* Views captured on Cambridge Core between 14th February 2018 - 17th August 2018. This data will be updated every 24 hours.