Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-r6qrq Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-29T20:32:31.578Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

13 - Neurology and Experience: The Language Organ and Externalization

from Part II - Interfaces

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 October 2018

Ángel J. Gallego
Affiliation:
Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona
Roger Martin
Affiliation:
Yokohama National University, Japan
Get access

Summary

In the study of human speech, as in the study of birdsong, it is arguably crucial to distinguish what is innate from what is learned. Human newborns are equipped with all the brain power needed to speak one or more human languages. Their respective language organs are biologically given in essence (hence universal), while the particular grammar they grow in the first years of life from hearing the language spoken around them is, to a considerable extent, a historically evolving subsystem, under the weight of the past like other cultural systems. A crucial part of this growth is the memorization of the vocabulary of at least one language spoken by the fellow humans the infant hear speak day after day. If they grow up in a modern industrialized community, they are likely to eventually have larger vocabularies than infants who grow up in less industrialized ones, but the underlying structure of their respective languages is essentially the same, needless to say. Taking this observations as a point of departure, this paper argues that a psychoneurological theory of human language structure must mirror this essential property by placing the insertion of vocabulary items of each particular language grown in the mind of an infant, in their ultimate morphological forms, into the abstract phrase-marker generated by the syntax , thus at the end of the derivation of a sentence, after all the purely syntactic rules have applied. It goes on to argue that properties of a vocabulary have a bearing on the range of possibilities open to their speakers by examining three of Shakespeare’s sonnets and one of their Spanish translations.
Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2018

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Aitchison, Jean. 2003. Words in the Mind: An Introduction to the Mental Lexicon. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Ayto, John. 2010. A Century of New Words. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Berlin, Brent, and Kay, Paul. 1969. Basic Color Terms: Their Universality and Evolution. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Cernuda, Luis. 1994. Prosa I. Obra completa, volumen II. Edición a cargo de Derek Harris y Luis Maristany. Barcelona: Ediciones Siruela (2nd edn. 2002).Google Scholar
Chomsky, Noam. 1965. Aspects of the Theory of Syntax. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Chomsky, Noam 1969. “Some observations on the teaching of language,” The Pedagogic Reporter 21.1: 56 & 13.Google Scholar
Chomsky, Noam 1991. “Language, politics, and composition: A conversation with Noam Chomsky,” conducted and transcribed by Olson, Gary A. and Faigley, Lester, Journal of Advanced Composition 11.1: 435.Google Scholar
Chomsky, Noam 1992a. “Explaining language use,” in The Philosophy of Hilary Putnam, ed. Hill, C. S.. Fayetteville, NC: Open Court.Google Scholar
Chomsky, Noam 1992b. “Explaining language use,” Philosophical Topics 20: 205231.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, Noam 1993. “A minimalist program for linguistic theory,” in The View from Building 20, ed. Hale, Ken and Keyser, Samuel J.. 152. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Chomsky, Noam 1995. “Language and nature,” Mind 104: 161.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, Noam 2000. New Horizons in the Study of Language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, Noam, and Halle, Morris. 1968. The Sound Pattern of English. New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
Crystal, David. 1997. English as a Global Language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press (http://dannyreviews.com/h/English_Global_Language.html).Google Scholar
Dahl, Hartvig. 1979. Word Frequencies of Spoken American English. Essex, CT: Verbatim.Google Scholar
Deutscher, Guy. 2010. Through the Language Glass: Why the World Looks Different in Other Languages. New York: Metropolitan Books.Google Scholar
Emonds, Joseph E. 2001. Lexicon and Grammar: The English Syntacticon. Berlin and New York: Gruyter Mouton.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fabb, Nigel, and Halle, Morris. 2008. Meter in Poetry: A New Theory. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
García Calvo, Agustín. 1983. Shakespeare: The Sonnets. Texto crítico y traducción en verso. Barcelona: Editorial Anagrama.Google Scholar
Grayson, Gabriel. 2003. Talking with Your Hands, Listening with Your Eyes: A Complete Photographic Guide to American Sign Language. New York: Square One.Google Scholar
Hale, Ken, and Keyser, Samuel J. (eds.). 1993. The View from Building 20. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Hardin, C. L., and Maffi, Luisa (eds.). 1997. Color Categories in Thought and Language. Cambridge and New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hill, C. S. (ed.). 1992. The Philosophy of Hilary Putnam. Fayetteville, NC: Open Court.Google Scholar
Katamba, Francis. 2005. English Words: Structure, History, Usage. London and New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Kayne, Richard S. 2008. “Comparative syntax and the lexicon” (ms., New York University), Congrès Mondial de Linguistique Française – CMLF'08. (French version available at www.linguistiquefrancaise.org/index.php?option=article&access=standard&Itemid=129&url=/articles/cmlf/pdf/2008/01/cmlf08350.pdf.)Google Scholar
McCrum, Robert. 2010. Globish: How the English Language Became the World's Language. New York and London: W. W. Norton.Google Scholar
Miller, George A. 1991. The Science of Words. New York: Scientific American Library.Google Scholar
Motoriol, Carme. 1928. Els Sonnets de Shakespeare. Barcelona: Verdaguer.Google Scholar
Otero, Carlos P. 1966. “Las sílabas de la poesía.” Papeles de Son Armadans. CXIX. 141–72. Reprinted (with additions) in Otero, Letras I. Barcelona: Seix Barral.Google Scholar
Otero, Carlos P. 1996. “Language growth after puberty?Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19: 677758.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Piera, Carlos. 1980. “Spanish verse and the theory of meter.” Ph.D. dissertation, UCLA.Google Scholar
Piera, Carlos, in collaboration with Fabb, N. and Halle, M.. 2008. “Southern Romance [meters],” in Fabb, and Halle, , pp. 94132.Google Scholar
Pinker, Steven. 2000. Words and Rules: The Ingredients of Language. New York: Harper.Google Scholar
Prado, Marcial. 1993. NTC's Dictionary of Spanish False Cognates: A Dictionary of Words in Spanish that Look like English Words but Whose Meanings Are Quite Different. Madrid: NTC Publishing Group.Google Scholar
Prado, Marcial 2001. Diccionario de falsos amigos. Madrid: Gredos.Google Scholar
Pujante, Ángel-Luis. 2009. “Shakespeare's sonnets in Spanish: Rescuing the early verse translations (1),” A Journal of Translation History 3.3 (www.traduccionliteraria.org/1611/art/pujante.htm).Google Scholar
Sacks, David. 2004. Letter Perfect: The Marvelous History of our Alphabet from A to Z. Broadway: Broadway Books.Google Scholar
Shakespeare, William. 1609. The Sonnets of Shakespeare. Edited from the Quarto of 1609 with Introduction and Commentary by Tucker, T. G.. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Library, 1904.Google Scholar
Stavans, Ilan. 2003. Spanglish: The Making of a New American Language. New York: HarperCollins. (With a Spanglish translation of chapter 1 of Don Quixote.)Google Scholar
Weisberg, Jacob (ed.). 2001. George W. Bushisms: The Slate Book of the Accidental Wit and Wisdom of our 43rd President. New York: Fireside.Google Scholar
Wunderlich, Dieter (ed.). 2006. Advances in the Theory of the Lexicon. Berlin and New York: Mouton de Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
YALE and Sordo, Julen. 1979. Diccionario del Pasota. Barcelona: Editorial Planeta.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×