Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-22dnz Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-30T02:15:34.601Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

24 - Illusions of time, space, and motion: flash-lag meets chopsticks and reversed phi

from Part IV - Spatial phenomena: forward shift effects

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 October 2010

Romi Nijhawan
Affiliation:
University of Sussex
Beena Khurana
Affiliation:
University of Sussex
Get access

Summary

Summary

In the “chopstick illusion” (Anstis 1990, 2003) a vertical and horizontal line overlapped to form a cross and followed clockwise circular orbits in counterphase, with one line being at 6 o'clock when the other was at 12 o'clock. The intersection of the lines moved counterclockwise, but it was wrongly perceived as rotating clockwise. This chopstick illusion reveals how moving objects are parsed, based upon the intrinsic and extrinsic terminators of lines viewed through apertures. We conclude that intersections were not parsed as objects, but instead the motion of the terminators (tips) propagated along the lines and was blindly assigned to the intersection. In the similar “sliding rings illusion,” we found that observers could use their eyes to track intersections only when these appeared rigid and not when they appeared to slide. Conclusion – smooth pursuit eye movements are under top-down control and are compelled to rely upon perceptual interpretation of objects.

In the “flash-lag” effect, a static object that is briefly flashed up next to a moving object appears to lag behind the moving object (Nijhawan 2002). We superimposed a flashed spot on a chopsticks intersection that appeared to be moving clockwise along a circular path but was actually moving counterclockwise. We found that the flash appeared displaced clockwise. This was appropriate to the physical, not the subjective direction of rotation, indicating that the flash-lag and the chopstick illusions coexist without interacting. Similarly, the flash-lag effect was unaffected by reversed phi. […]

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2010

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Adelson, E. H., & Bergen, J. R. (1985). Spatiotemporal energy models for the perception of motion. J Opt Soc Am A 2: 284–299.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anstis, S. M. (1970). Phi movement as a subtraction process. Vision Res 10: 1411–1430.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anstis, S. M. (1990). Imperceptible intersections: The chopstick illusion. In A., Blake & T., Troscianko (eds.), AI and the Eye (105–117). London: Wiley.Google Scholar
Anstis, S. M. (2003). Levels of motion perception. In L., Harris & M., Jenkin (eds.), Levels of Perception. New York: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
Anstis, S. M. (2007). The flash-lag effect during illusory chopstick rotation. Perception 36: 1043–1048.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anstis, S. M., & Rogers, B. J. (1975). Illusory reversal of visual depth and movement during changes of contrast. Vision Res 15: 957–961.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baldo, M. V., Kihara, A. H., Namba, J., & Klein, S. A. (2002). Evidence for an attentional component of the perceptual misalignment between moving and flashing stimuli. Perception 31: 17–30.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Braddick, O. (1974). A short-range process in apparent motion. Vision Res 14: 519–527.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Braddick, O. J. (1980). Low-level and high-level processes in apparent motion. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 290: 137–151.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cai, R., & Schlag, J. (2001). A new form of illusory conjunction between color and shape [Abstract]. J Vis 1(3): 127a, http://journalofvision.org/1/3/127/, doi:10.1167/1.3.127.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eagleman, D. M., & Sejnowski, T. J. (2000). Motion integration and postdiction in visual awareness. Science 287: 2036–2038.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Khurana, B., Watanabe, K., & Nijhawan, R. (2000). The role of attention in motion extrapolation: are moving objects “corrected” or flashed objects attentionally delayed? Perception 29: 675–692.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kowler, E. (ed.) (1990). Eye Movements and Their Role in Visual and Cognitive Processes. Amsterdam; New York: Elsevier.Google ScholarPubMed
Krauzlis, R. J. (1994). The visual drive for smooth eye movements. In A. T., Smith & R. J., Snowden (eds.), Visual Detection of Motion (437–473). London; New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Kreegipuu, K., & Allik, J. (2004). Confusion of space and time in the flash-lag effect. Perception 33: 293–306.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krekelberg, B., & Lappe, M. (2000). A model of the perceived relative position of moving objects based upon a slow averaging process. Vision Res 40: 201–215.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lisberger, S. G., Morris, E. J., & Tychsen, L. (1987). Visual motion processing and sensory-motor integration for smooth pursuit eye movements. Ann Rev Neurosci 10: 97–129.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lu, Z. L., & Sperling, G. (1999). Second-order reversed phi. Perception & Psychophysics 61: 1075–1088.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mackay, D. M. (1961). Interactive processes in visual perception. In W. A., Rosenblith (ed.), Sensory Communication (339–355). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Metelli, F. (1974). The perception of transparency. Scientific American 230: 90–98.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nijhawan, R. (2002). Neural delays, visual motion and the flash-lag effect. Trends Cogn Sci 6: 387–393.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rogers, B. J., & Anstis, S. M. (1975). Reversed depth from positive and negative stereograms. Perception 4: 193–201.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shiffrar, M., & Pavel, M. (1991). Percepts of rigid motion within and across apertures. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 17: 749–761.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shimojo, S., Silverman, G. H., & Nakayama, K. (1989). Occlusion and the solution to the aperture problem for motion. Vision Res 29: 619–626.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sinha, P., & Poggio, T. (1996). I think I know that face …Nature 384: 404.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stoner, G. R., & Albright, T. D. (1994). Visual motion integration: A neurophysiological and psychophysical perspective. In A. T., Smith & R. J., Snowden (eds.), Visual Detection of Motion (253–290). London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Ullman, S. (1979). The Interpretation of Visual Motion. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Whitney, D., Cavanagh, P., & Murakami, I. (2000). Temporal facilitation for moving stimuli is independent of changes in direction. Vision Res 40: 3829–3839.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whitney, D., Murakami, I., & Cavanagh, P. (2000). Illusory spatial offset of a flash relative to a moving stimulus is caused by differential latencies for moving and flashed stimuli. Vision Res 40: 137–149.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×