Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-zzh7m Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-26T03:23:23.553Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

References

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 June 2012

Edward Slingerland
Affiliation:
University of British Columbia, Vancouver
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
What Science Offers the Humanities
Integrating Body and Culture
, pp. 321 - 356
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2008

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abdulmoneim, Mohamed Shokr. 2006. The metaphorical concept “Life is a Journey” in the Qur'an: A cognitive-semantic analysis. Metaphorik 10: 94–132.Google Scholar
Abler, William. 2005. Evidence of group learning does not add up to culture. Nature 438: 422.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Abutalebi, Jubin, Miozzo, Antonio & Cappa, Stefano. 2000. Do subcortical structures control “language selection” in polyglots? Evidence from pathological language mixing. Neurocase 6: 51–56.Google Scholar
Adams, Fred & Campbell, Kenneth. 1999. Modality and abstract concepts (response to Barsalou 1999a). Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22: 610.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aglioti, Salvatore, DeSouza, Joseph & Goodale, Melvyn. 1995. Size contrast illusions deceive the eye but not the hand. Current Biology 5: 679–685.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aguiar, André & Renée Baillargeon, . 1999. 2.5-month-old infants' reasoning about when objects should and should not be occluded. Cognitive Psychology 39: 116–157.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aiello, Leslie & Dunbar, Robin. 1993. Neocortex size, group size and the evolution of language. Current Anthropology 34: 184–193.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aiello, Leslie & Wheeler, Peter. 1995. The expensive tissue hypothesis. Current Anthropology 36: 199–211.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alexander, Richard. 1979. Darwinism and human affairs. Seattle: University of Washington Press.Google Scholar
Anthony, Herbert Douglas. 1948. Science and its background. London: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Arbib, Michael. 1972. The metaphorical brain: An introduction to cybernetics as artificial intelligence and brain theory. New York: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Arbib, Michael. 1985. In search of the person: Philosophical explorations in cognitive science. Amherst: University of Massachusetts Press.Google Scholar
Arbib, Michael. 1989. The metaphorical brain 2: Neural networks and beyond. New York: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Arbib, Michael, Conklin, E. Jeffrey & Hill, Jane. 1987. From schema theory to language. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Arbib, Michael & Rizzolatti, Giacomo. 1996. Neural expectations: A possible evolutionary path from manual skills to language. Communication and Cognition 29: 393–424.Google Scholar
Arnheim, Rudolf. 1969. Visual thinking. Berkeley: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Atran, Scott. 1987. Ordinary constraints on the semantics of living kinds. Mind & Language 2: 27–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Atran, Scott. 1990. Cognitive foundations of natural history: Toward an anthropology of science. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Atran, Scott. 1995. Causal constraints on categories and categorical constraints on biological reasoning across cultures. In Sperber et al. 1995, 205–233.
Atran, Scott. 1998. Folk biology and the anthropology of science: Cognitive universals and cultural particulars. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 21: 547–609.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aunger, Robert (ed.). 2000. Darwinizing culture: The status of memetics as a science. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Aunger, Robert. 2006. What's the matter with memes? In Grafen & Ridley 2006, 176–190.
Avis, Jeremy & Harris, Paul. 1991. Belief-desire reasoning among Baka children: Evidence for a universal conception of mind. Child Development 62: 460–467.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Axelrod, Robert. 1984. The evolution of cooperation. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Bacon, Francis. 1620/1999. Selected philosophical works (ed. Rose-Mary Sargent, ). Cambridge, MA: Hackett Publishing Company.Google Scholar
Baier, Annette. 1994. Moral prejudices: Essays on ethics. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Bailenson, Jeremy, Shum, Michael, Atran, Scott, Medin, Douglas & Coley, John. 2002. A bird's-eye view: Biological categorization and reasoning within and across cultures. Cognition 84: 1–53.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baillargeon, Renée. 1986. Representing the existence and the location of hidden objects: Object permanence in 6- and 8-month-old infants. Cognition 23: 21–41.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baillargeon, Renée. 1991. The object concept revisited: New directions in the investigation of infants' physical knowledge. In Granrud, Carl (ed.), Visual perception and cognition in infancy, 265–315. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Baillargeon, Renée, Laura Kotovsky & Amy Needham. 1995. The acquisition of physical knowledge in infancy. In Sperber et al. 1995, 79–116.
Baillargeon, Renée, Spelke, Elizabeth & Wasserman, Stanley. 1985. Object permanence in five-month-old infants. Cognition 20: 191–208.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baillargeon, Renée, & Su-hua Wang, . 2002. Event categorization in infancy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 6: 85–93.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baker, Catherine (ed. by Miller, James). 2006. The evolution dialogues: Science, Christianity, and the quest for understanding. Washington, DC: The American Association for the Advancement of Science.Google Scholar
Balaban, Victor. 1999. Self and agency in religious discourse: Perceptual metaphors for knowledge at a Marian apparition site. In Gibbs & Steen 1999, 125–144.
Ballard, Dana. 1991. Animate vision. Artificial Intelligence 48: 57–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ballard, Dana. 2002. On the function of visual representation. In Noë & Thompson 2002, 459–479.
Balter, Michael. 2006. First jewelry? Old shell beads suggest early use of symbols. Science 23: 1731.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bargh, John & Chartrand, Tanya. 1999. The unbearable automaticity of being. American Psychologist 54: 462–479.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barkow, Jerome. 1989. Darwin, sex, and status: Biological approaches to mind and culture. Toronto: University of Toronto Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barkow, Jerome, Cosmides, Leda & Tooby, John (eds.). 1992. The adapted mind: Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Barnes, Barry & David Bloor. 1982. Relativism, rationalism and the sociology of knowledge. In Hollis, Martin & Lukes, Steven (eds.), Rationality and relativism, 21–47. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Baron-Cohen, Simon. 1995. Mindblindness: An essay on autism and theory of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Baron-Cohen, Simon & Harrison, John (eds.). 1997. Synaesthesia: Classic and contemporary readings. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Baron-Cohen, Simon, Leslie, Alan & Frith, Christopher. 1985. Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind”? Cognition 21: 37–46.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barres, Ben. 2006. Does gender matter? Nature 442: 133–136.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, Barrett H., Todd, Peter, Miller, Geoffrey & Blythe, Philip. 2005. Accurate judgments of intention from motion cues alone: A cross-cultural study. Evolution and Human Behavior 26: 313–331.Google Scholar
Barrett, Justin. 1999. Theological correctness: Cognitive constraint and the study of religion. Method and Theory in the Study of Religion 11: 325–339.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barrett, Justin. 2000. Exploring the natural foundations of religion. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 4: 29–34.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barrett, Justin & Nyhof, Melanie. 2001. Spreading non-natural concepts: The role of intuitive conceptual structures in memory and transmission of cultural materials. Journal of Cognition and Culture 1: 69–100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barsalou, Lawrence. 1999a. Perceptual symbol systems. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22: 577–609.Google Scholar
Barsalou, Lawrence. 1999b. Perceptions of perceptual symbols (author's response to commentary). Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22: 633–660.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barsalou, Lawrence, Kyle, Simmons, Aron, Barbey & Christine, Wilson. 2003, Grounding conceptual knowledge in modality-specific systems. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 7: 84–91.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barsalou, Lawrence & Katja Wiemer-Hastings. 2005. Situating abstract concepts. In Pecher & Zwaan 2005, 129–163.
Barth, Hilary, Mont, Kristen, Lipton, Jennifer, Deheane, Stanislas, Kanwisher, Nancy & Spelke, Elizabeth. 2006. Non-symbolic arithmetic in adults and young children. Cognition 98: 199–222.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barthes, Roland. 1967. Writing degree zero (trans. Annette Lavers & Colin Smith). New York: Hill & Wang.Google Scholar
Barthes, Roland. 1968. Elements of semiology (trans. Annette Lavers & Colin Smith). New York: Hill & Wang.Google Scholar
Barthes, Roland. 1972. To write: An intransitive verb? In Macksey, Richard & Donato, Eugenio (eds.), The languages of criticism and the science of man: The structuralist controversy, 134–144. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.Google Scholar
Barthes, Roland. 1982. The empire of signs (trans. Richard Howard). New York: Hill & Wang.Google Scholar
Bar-Yosef, Ofer. 2006. Human migrations in prehistory: The cultural record. Presentation at World Knowledge Dialogue, Crans-Montana, Switzerland, September 15.
Baudrillard, Jean. 1994. Simulacra and simulation (trans. Sheila Faria Glaser). Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press.Google Scholar
Baudrillard, Jean. 1995. The Gulf War did not take place (trans. Paul Patton). Bloomington and Indianapolis: Indiana University Press.Google Scholar
Baumeister, Roy. 1984. Choking under pressure: Self-consciousness and paradoxical effects of incentives on skillful performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 46: 610–620.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baumeister, Roy, Bratslavsky, Ellen, Muraven, Mark & Tice, Dianne. 1998. Ego depletion: Is the active self a limited resource? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 74: 1252–1265.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bechara, Antoine, Damasio, Antonio, Damasio, Hannah & Anderson, Steven. 1994. Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex. Cognition 50: 7–15.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bechara, Antoine, Damasio, Hannah & Damasio, Antonio. 2000. Emotion, decision making, and the orbitofrontal cortex. Cerebral Cortex 10: 295–307.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bechara, Antoine, Damasio, Hannah, Tranel, Daniel & Damasio, Antonio. 1997. Deciding advantageously before knowing the advantageous strategy. Science 275: 1293–1295.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beer, Francis & Christ'l de Landtsheer, (eds.). 2004. Metaphorical world politics. East Lansing: Michigan State University Press.Google Scholar
Behl-Chadha, Gundeep. 1996. Basic-level and superordinate-like categorical representations in early infancy. Cognition 60: 105–141.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bekoff, Marc & Byers, John Alexander (eds.). 1998. Animal play: Evolutionary, comparative, and ecological perspectives. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berger, Peter & Luckmann, Thomas. 1966. The social construction of reality: A treatise in the sociology of knowledge. New York: Anchor Books.Google Scholar
Bering, Jesse. 2002. The existential theory of mind. Review of General Psychology 6: 3–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bering, Jesse & Parker, Becky. 2006. Children's attributions of intentions to an invisible agent. Developmental Psychology 42: 253–262.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berkson, Mark. 1996. Language: The guest of reality – Zhuangzi and Derrida on language, reality, and skillfulness. In Kjellberg, Paul & Ivanhoe, Philip J. (eds.), Essays on skepticism, relativism and ethics in the Zhuangzi, 97–126. Albany, NY: SUNY Press.Google Scholar
Berlin, Brent, Breedlove, Dennis & Raven, Peter. 1973. General principles of classification and nomenclature in folk biology. American Anthropologist 75: 214–242.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berlin, Brent & Kay, Paul. 1969. Basic color terms: Their universality and growth. Berkeley: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Bernstein, Richard. 1983. Beyond objectivism and relativism: Science, hermeneutics, and praxis. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press.Google Scholar
Black, Max. 1954–1955. Metaphor. Proceedings of the Aristotelian Society 55: 273–294.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blackmore, Susan, Brelstaff, Gavin, Nelson, Kay & Troscianko, Tom. 1995. Is the richness of our visual world an illusion? Transsaccadic memory for complex scenes. Perception 24: 1075–1081.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
James, Blair R.. 1995. A cognitive developmental approach to morality: Investigating the psychopath. Cognition 57: 1–29.Google Scholar
James, Blair R.. 2001. Neurocognitive models of aggression, the antisocial personality disorders, and psychopathy. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry 71: 727–731.Google Scholar
Blaisdell, Aaron, Sawa, Kosuke, Leising, Kenneth & Waldmann, Michael. 2006. Causal reasoning in rats. Science 311: 1020–1022.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blakemore, Sarah-Jayne & Decety, Jean. 2001. From the perception of action to the understanding of intention. Nature Neuroscience 2: 561–567.Google ScholarPubMed
Block, Ned. 1983. Mental pictures and cognitive science. Philosophical Review 93: 499–542.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bloom, Paul. 2004. Descartes' baby: How the science of child development explains what makes us human. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Boas, Franz. 1962. Anthropology and modern life. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Boden, Margaret. 1990. The creative mind: Myths and mechanisms. London: Weidenfeld & Nicholson.Google Scholar
Boden, Margaret. 1994. Précis of The creative mind: Myths and mechanisms. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17: 519–570.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boghossian, Paul. 2000. What the Sokal hoax ought to teach us, and selected responses. In The Editors of Lingua Franca 2000, 172–186.Google Scholar
Borges, Bernhard, Daniel Goldstein, Andreas Ortmann & Gerd Gigerenzer. 1999. Can ignorance beat the stock market? In Gigerenzer et al. 1999, 59–74.
Borghi, Anna. 2004. Objects, concepts, and actions: Extracting affordances from objects' parts. Acta Psychologia 115: 69–96.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Borghi, Anna. 2005. Objects, concepts, and actions. In Pecher & Zwaan 2005, 8–34.
Bornstein, Marc. 1985. Habituation as a measure of visual information processing in human infants: Summary, systemization, and synthesis. In Gottlieb, Gilbert & Krasnegor, Norman (eds.), Measurement of audition and vision in the first year of postnatal life, 253–295. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Boroditsky, Lera. 2001. Does language shape thought? English and Mandarin speakers' conceptions of time. Cognitive Psychology 43: 1–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Botz-Bornstein, Thorsten. 2006. Ethnophilosophy, comparative philosophy, and pragmatism: Toward a philosophy of ethnoscapes. Philosophy East and West 56: 153–171.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bouchard, Thomas. 1994. Genes, environment, and personality. Science 264: 1700–1701.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bouchard, Thomas. 1998. Genetic and environmental influences of intelligence and special mental abilities. Human Biology 70: 257–259.Google ScholarPubMed
Bourdieu, Pierre. 1977. Outline of a theory of practice (trans. Richard Nice). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bourdieu, Pierre 1984. Distinction: A social critique of the judgment of taste (trans. Richard Nice). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Bourdieu, Pierre. 1990a. In other words: Essays toward a reflexive sociology (trans. Matthew Adamson). Stanford: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Bourdieu, Pierre. 1990b. The logic of practice (trans. Richard Nice). Stanford: Stanford University Press.
Bourdieu, Pierre. 1993. The field of cultural production. New York: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
Boyd, Brian. 2006. Getting it all wrong: Bioculture critiques cultural critique. American Scholar 75: 18–30.Google Scholar
Boyd, Robert & Richerson, Peter. 1992. Punishment allows the evolution of cooperation (or anything else) in sizable groups. Ethology and Sociobiology 13: 171–195.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boyd, Robert & Richerson, Peter. 2005. Not by genes alone: How culture transformed human evolution. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Boyer, Pascal. 1994. Cognitive constraints on cultural representations: Natural ontologies and religious ideas. In Hirschfeld & Gelman 1994a, 391–411.
Boyer, Pascal. 2001. Religion explained: The evolutionary origins of religious thought. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Boyer, Pascal. 2005. Domain specificity and intuitive ontology. In Buss 2005, 96–118.
Boyer, Pascal & H. Clark Barrett. 2005. Domain specificity and intuitive ontology. In Buss 2005, 96–118.
Bregman, Albert & Pinker, Steven. 1978. Auditory streaming and the building of timbre. Canadian Journal of Psychology 32: 19–31.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bruce, Brooks E. & Brooks, Taeko. 1998. The original Analects. New York: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
Brooks, Rodney. 1991. Intelligence without representation. Artificial Intelligence 47: 139–159.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brosnan, Sarah & Waal, Frans. 2003. Monkeys reject unequal pay. Nature 425: 297–299.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, Donald. 1991. Human universals. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Brown, Donald. 2000. Human universals and their implications. In Roughley, Neil (ed.), Being humans: Anthropological universality and particularity in transdisciplinary perspectives, 156–174. New York: Walter de GruyterCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, Theodore. 2003. Making truth: Metaphor in science. Urbana: University of Illinois Press.Google Scholar
Bruner, J. S. & Postman, Leo. 1949. On the perception of incongruity: A paradigm. Journal of Personality 18: 206–223.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buonomano, Dean & Merzenich, Michael. 1998. Cortical plasticity: From synapses to maps. Annual Review of Neuroscience 21: 149–186.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burgess, Curt & Chiarello, Christine. 1996. Neurocognitive mechanisms underlying metaphor comprehension and other figurative language. Metaphor and Symbolic Activity 11: 67–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burnyeat, Myles. 1980. Aristotle on learning to be good. In Rorty, Amélie O. (ed.), Essays on Aristotle's ethics, 69–92. Berkeley: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Buss, David (ed.). 2005. The handbook of evolutionary psychology. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Butler, Judith. 1997. Further reflections on the conversations of our time. Diacritics 27: 13–15.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Butler, Judith. 2004. Undoing gender. New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Butterworth, George. 1991. The ontogeny and phylogeny of joint visual attention. In Whiten, Andrew (ed.), Natural theories of mind: Evolution, development, and simulation of everyday mind reading, 223–232. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar
Byrne, Richard & Whiten, Andrew (eds.). 1988. Machiavellian intelligence: Social expertise and the evolution of intellect in monkeys, apes, and humans. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Camus, Albert. 1942. Le mythe de sisyphe. Paris: Gallimard.Google Scholar
Camus, Albert. 1947. La peste. Paris: Gallimard.Google Scholar
Caramazza, Alfonso, Argye Hillis, Elwyn Lee & Michele Miozzo. 1994. The organization of lexical knowledge in the brain: Evidence from category- and modality-specific deficits. In Hirschfeld & Gelman 1994a, 68–84.
Carey, Susan. 1985. Conceptual change in childhood. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Carey, Susan. 1991. Knowledge acquistion: Enrichment or conceptual change? In Carey & Spelke, 1994, 257–291.
Carey, Susan. 1995. On the origin of causal understanding. In Sperber et al. 1995, 268–302.
Carey, Susan & Gelman, Rochel (eds.). 1991. The epigenesis of mind: Essays on biology and cognition. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Carey, Susan & Elizabeth Spelke. 1994. Domain-specific knowledge and conceptual change. In Hirschfeld & Gelman 1994a, 169–200.
Carruthers, Peter. 2005. The case for massively modular models of mind. In Stainton, Robert (ed.), Contemporary debates in cognitive science, 205–225. Oxford Basil: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Carruthers, Peter & Boucher, Jill (eds.). 1998. Language and thought: Interdisciplinary themes. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carruthers, Peter & Andrew, Chamberlain (eds.). 2000. Evolution and the human mind: Modularity, language and meta-cognition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carruthers, Peter, Laurence, Stephen & Stich, Stephen (eds.). 2005. The innate mind: Structure and content. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carruthers, Peter, Laurence, Stephen & Stich, Stephen. 2006. The innate mind: Culture and cognition. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Carruthers, Peter & Smith, Peter (eds.). 1996. Theories of theories of mind. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cartwright, Nancy. 1983. How the laws of physics lie. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cartwright, Nancy. 1999. The dappled world: A study of the boundaries of science. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Casasola, Marianella, Cohen, Leslie & Chiarello, Elizabeth. 2003. Six-month-old infants' categorization of containment spatial relations. Child Development 74: 679–693.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cavell, Stanley. 1979. The claim of reason. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Chaibong, Hahm. 2001. Confucian rituals and the technology of the self: A Foucaultian interpretation. Philosophy East and West 51: 315–324.Google Scholar
Chalmers, Alan. 1985. Galileo's telescopic observations of Venus and Mars. British Journal for the Philosophy of Science 36: 175–191.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chalmers, Alan. 1999. What is this thing called science? 3rd ed. Indianapolis: Hackett Publishing Company.Google Scholar
Chalmers, David. 1995. Facing up to the problem of consciousness. Journal of Consciousness Studies 2: 200–219.Google Scholar
Chalmers, David. 1996. The conscious mind: In search of a fundamental theory. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Cheney, Dorothy & Seyfarth, Robert. 1990. How monkeys see the world: Inside the mind of another species. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Shude, Cheng. 1990. Lunyu Jijie. Beijing: Zhonghua Shuju.Google Scholar
Chilton, Paul. 1996. Security metaphors: Cold War discourse from containment to common house. New York: Peter Lang.Google Scholar
Choi, Hoon & Scholl, Brian. 2006. Perceiving causality after the fact: Postdiction in the temporal dynamics of causal perception. Perception 35: 385–399.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chomsky, Noam. 1965. Aspects of the theory of syntax. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Christoff, Kalina & Owen, Adrian. 2006. Improving reverse neuroimaging inference: Cognitive domain versus cognitive complexity. Trends in Cognitive Science 10: 352–353.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Churchland, Patricia. 1986. Neurophilosophy: Toward a unified science of the mind-brain. Cambridge, MA: Bradford Books/MIT Press.Google Scholar
Churchland, Patricia, V. S. Ramachandran & Terrence Sejnowski. 1994. A critique of pure vision. In Koch, Christof & Davis, Joel (eds.), Large-scale neuronal theories of the brain, 23–60. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Churchland, Paul. 1979. Scientific realism and the plasticity of the mind. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Churchland, Paul. 1985. The ontological status of observables: In praise of the superempirical virtues. In Churchland, Paul & Hooker, Clifford (eds.), Images of science: Essays on realism and empiricism, 35–47. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Churchland, Paul & Sejnowski, Terrence. 1992. The computational brain. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Cienki, Alan. 2005. Image schemas and gesture. In Hampe 2005, 421–442.
Clark, Andy. 1997. Being there: Putting brain, body and world together again. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Clarke, Andy, 1999. Visual awareness and visuomotor action. In Núũñez & Freeman 1999, 1–18.
Clements, Wendy & Perner, Josef. 1994. Implicit understanding of belief. Cognitive Development 9: 377–397.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coleman, Linda & Kay, Paul. 1981. Prototype semantics: The English word lie. Language 57: 26–44.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Connor, Steven (ed.). 2004. The Cambridge companion to postmodernism. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coppola, Marie & Newport, Elissa. 2005. Grammatical subjects in home sign: Abstract linguistic structure in adult primary gesture systems without linguistic input. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 102: 19249–19253.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Corbey, Raymond. 2005. The metaphysics of apes: Negotiating the animal–human boundary. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Cosmides, Leda. 1989. The logic of social exchange: Has natural selection shaped how humans reason? Studies with the Wason selection task. Cognition 31: 187–276.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cosmides, Leda & John Tooby. 1992. Cognitive adaptations for social exchange. In Barkow et al. 1992, 163–228.
Cosmides, Leda & John Tooby. 1994. Origins of domain specificity: The evolution of functional organization. In Hirschfeld & Gelman 1994a, 85–116.
Cosmides, Leda & John Tooby. 2000. The cognitive neuroscience of social reasoning. In Gazzaniga, Michael (ed.), The new cognitive neurosciences, 2nd ed., 1259–1270. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Cosmides, Leda & John Tooby. 2005. Neurocognitive adaptations designed for social exchange. In Buss 2005, 584–627.
Coulson, Seana. 2001. Semantic leaps: Frame-shifting and conceptual blending in meaning construction. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Creem-Regehr, Sarah & Lee, James. 2005. Neural representations of graspable objects: Are tools special? Cognitive Brain Research 22: 457–469.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crick, Francis. 1994. The astonishing hypothesis: The scientific search for the soul. New York: Simon & Schuster.Google Scholar
Crinion, Jenny, Turner, R., Grogan, A., Hanakawa, T., Noppeney, U., Devlin, J. T., Aso, T., Urayama, S., Fukuyama, H., Stockton, K., Usui, K., Green, D. W. & Price, C. J.. 2006. Language control in the bilingual brain. Science 312: 1537–1540.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crutch, Sebastian & Warrington, Elizabeth. 2005. Abstract and concrete concepts have structurally different representational frameworks. Brain 128: 615–627.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dally, Joanna, Emery, Nathan & Clayton, Nicola. 2006. Food-caching Western scrub-jays keep track of who was watching when. Science 312: 1662–1665.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Damasio, Antonio. 1985. Disorders of Complex visual processing: Agnosias, achromatopsia, Balint's syndrome, and related difficulties of orientation and construction. In Mesulam, M. M. (ed.), Principles of behavioural neurology, 259–288. Philadelphia: Davis.Google Scholar
Damasio, Antonio. 1989. The brain binds entities and events by multiregional activation from convergence zones. Neural Computation 1: 123–132.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Damasio, Antonio. 1994. Descartes' error: Emotion, reason, and the human brain. New York: G. P. Putnam's Sons.Google Scholar
Damasio, Antonio. 2000. The feeling of things: Body and emotion in the making of consciousness. New York: Harvest.Google Scholar
Damasio, Antonio. 2003. In search of Spinoza: Joy, sorrow, and the feeling brain. New York: Harvest.Google Scholar
Damasio, Antonio & Hannah Damasio. 1994. Cortical systems for retrieval of concrete knowledge: The convergence zone framework. In Koch, Christof & Davis, Joel (eds.), Large-scale neuronal theories of the brain, 61–74. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Dancygier, Barbara. 2006. What can blending do for you? Language and Literature 15: 5–15.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dancygier, Barbara & Sweetser, Eve. 2005. Mental spaces in grammar: Conditional structures. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Darden, Lindley & Maull, Nancy. 1977. Interfield theories. Philosophy of Science 44: 43–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Darwin, Charles. 1872. The expression of emotions in man and animals. New York: New York Philosophical Library.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Darwin, Charles & Ekman, Paul. 1998. The expression of emotions in man and animals (collated and edited, with introduction and afterword, by Paul Ekman). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Davidson, Donald. 1974. On the very idea of a conceptual scheme. Proceedings and Addresses of the American Philosophical Association 47: 5–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davidson, Donald. 1978/1981. What metaphors mean. Critical Inquiry 5: 31–47. [Reprinted in , Johnson 1981a, 200–220].
Dawkins, Richard. 1976/2006. The selfish gene. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gelder, Beatrice. 2006. Towards the neurobiology of emotional body language. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 7: 242–249.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dehaene, Stanislas. 1997. The number sense: How the mind creates mathematics. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Dehaene, Stanislas, Izard, Veronique, Pica, Pierre & Spelke, Elizabeth. 2006. Core knowledge of geometry in an Amazonian indigene group. Science 311: 381–384.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dehaene, Stanislas, Spelke, Elizabeth, Pinel, Philippe, Stanescu, Ruxandra & Tsivkin, Sanna. 1999. Sources of mathematical thinking: behavioral and brain-imaging evidence. Science 284: 970–974.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Man, Paul. 1978. The epistemology of metaphor. Critical Inquiry 5: 31–47.Google Scholar
Dennett, Daniel. 1978. Beliefs about beliefs. Behavior and Brain Science 4: 568–570.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dennett, Daniel. 1984a. Cognitive wheels: The frame problem of artificial intelligence. In Hookway, Christopher (ed.), Minds, machines, and evolution, 129–151. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Dennett, Daniel. 1984b. Elbow room: The varieties of free will worth wanting. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Dennett, Daniel. 1987. The intentional stance. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Dennett, Daniel. 1988. Quining qualia. In Marcel, Anthony & Bisiach, Edoardo (eds.), Consciousness in contemporary science. 42–77. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Dennett, Daniel. 1991. Consciousness explained. Boston: Little Brown.Google Scholar
Dennett, Daniel. 1995. Darwin's dangerous idea: Evolution and the meaning of life. New York: Simon & Schuster.Google Scholar
Dennett, Daniel. 2003. Freedom evolves. New York: Viking.Google Scholar
Dennett, Daniel. 2005. Sweet dreams: Philosophical obstacles to a science of consciousness. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Quervain, Dominique, Fischbacher, Urs, Treyer, Valerie, Schellhammer, Melanie, Schnyder, Ulrich, Buck, Alfred & Fehr, Ernst. 2004. The neural basis of altruistic punishment. Science 305: 1254–1258.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Derrida, Jacques. 1978. Of grammatology (trans. Gayatri Spivak). Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.Google Scholar
Derrida, Jacques. 1979. Living on: Borderlines. In Bloom, Harold et al. (eds.), Deconstruction and criticism, 75–176. New York: Seabury Press.Google Scholar
Derrida, Jacques. 1981. Positions (trans. Alan Bass). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Derrida, Jacques. 1984. Interview with Richard Kearney in Kearney, (ed.), Dialogues with contemporary thinkers, 83–105. Manchester, UK: Manchester University Press.Google Scholar
Sousa, Ronald. 1987. The rationality of emotion. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Devlin, Joseph, Russell, Richard, Davis, Matthew, Price, Cathy, Moss, Helen, Fadili, M. Jalal & Tyler, Lorraine. 2002. Is there an anatomical basis for category-specificity? Semantic memory studies in PET and fMRI. Neuropsychologia 40: 54–75.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Waal, Frans. 1983/1998. Chimpanzee politics: Power and sex among apes. 2nd ed. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.Google Scholar
Waal, Frans. 1996. Good natured: The origins of right and wrong in humans and other animals. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Waal, Frans. 2001. The ape and the sushi master: Cultural reflections of a primatologist. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Dewan, Edmond. 1976. Consciousness as an emergent causal agent in the context of control system theory. In Globus, Gordon, Maxwell, Grover, Savodnik, Irwin & Dewan, Edmond (eds.), Consciousness and the brain, 181–198. New York: Plenum.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dewey, John. 1938/1991. John Dewey, The later works, 1925–1953. Vol. 12: Logic: The theory of inquiry. Carbondale: Southern Illinois University Press.Google Scholar
Dewey, John. 1981. Experience and nature. In Boydston, Jo Ann (ed.), John Dewey: The later works, 1925–1953. Vol. 1. Carbondale: Southern Illinois University Press.Google Scholar
Diamond, Jared. 1997. Guns, germs, and steel: The fates of human societies. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Dickinson, Anthony & David Shanks. 1995. Instrumental action and causal representation. In Sperber et al. 1995, 5–25.
Dijksterhuis, Ap, Bos, Maarten, Nordgren, Loran & Baaren, Rick. 2006. On making the right choice: The deliberation-without-attention effect. Science 311: 1005–1008.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Donald, Merlin. 1991. Origins of the modern mind: Three stages in the evolution of culture and cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Duhem, Pierre. 1906/1954. The aim and structure of physical theory. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Dunbar, Kevin. 1999. The scientist in vivo: How scientists think and reason in the laboratory. In Magnani, Lorenzo, Nersessian, Nancy & Thagard, Paul (eds.), Model-based reasoning in scientific discovery, 89–98. New York: Plenum.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunbar, Kevin. 2001. The analogical paradox: Why analogy is so easy in naturalistic settings yet so difficult in the psychological laboratory. In Gentner et al. 2001, 313–334.
Dunbar, Robin. 1992. Neocortex size as a constraint on group size in primates. Journal of Human Evolution 20: 469–493.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunbar, Robin. 1993. Co-evolution of neocortical size, group size and language in humans. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16: 681–735.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunbar, Robin. 1995. The trouble with science. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Dunbar, Robin. 1996. Gossip, grooming, and the evolution of language. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Dunbar, Robin. 2004. Gossip in evolutionary perspective. Review of General Psychology 8: 100–110.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunbar, Robin, Knight, Chris & Power, Camilla (eds.). 1999. The evolution of culture: An interdisciplinary view. New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press.Google Scholar
Dupré, John. 1993. The disorder of things: Metaphysical foundations of the disunity of science. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Durkheim, Emile. 1895/1962. The rules of the sociological method. Glencoe, IL: Free Press.Google Scholar
Durkheim, Emile. 1915/1966. The elementary forms of the religious life (trans. Joseph Swain). New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
Eagleton, Terry. 1983. Literary theory: An introduction. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.Google Scholar
Eagleton, Terry. 2003. After theory. New York: Penguin.Google Scholar
Edelman, Gerald. 1992. Bright air, brilliant fire: On the matter of the mind. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Egge, James. 2004. Comparative analysis of religious metaphor: Appreciating similarity as well as difference. Presented at the American Academy of Religion Annual Meeting, San Antonio, TX, November 22.
Ekman, Paul. 1972/1982. Emotion in the human face. 2nd ed. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Ekman, Paul. 1980. Biological and cultural contributions to body and facial movement in the expression of emotions. In A. Rorty 1980b, 73–102.
Ekman, Paul. 1999. Basic emotions. In Dalgleish, Tim and Power, Mick (eds.), Handbook of cognition and emotion, 45–60. Sussex, UK: John Wiley & Sons.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ekman, Paul. 2003. Emotions revealed: Recognizing faces and feelings to improve communication and emotional life. New York: Owl Books.Google Scholar
Ekman, Paul & Davidson, Richard. 1994. The nature of emotion. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Elbert, Thomas et al. 1995. Increased cortical representation of the fingers of the left hand in string players. Science 270: 305–307.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elvee, Richard (ed.). 1992. The end of science? Attack and defense/ Nobel Conference XXV. Lanham, MD: University Press of America.Google Scholar
Embry, Ainslie (ed.). 1988. Sources of Indian tradition. 2nd ed. Vol. 1. New York: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
Emery, Nathan & Clayton, Nicola. 2001. Effects of experience and social context on prospective caching strategies by scrub jays. Nature 414: 443.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Emery, Nathan & Clayton, Nicola. 2004. The mentality of crows: Convergent evolution of intelligence in corvids and apes. Science 306: 1903–1907.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fadiga, Luciano, Craighero, Laila & Olivier, Etienne. 2005. Human motor cortex excitability during the perception of others' actions. Current Opinion in Neurobiology 15: 213–218.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fauconnier, Gilles. 1996. Analogical counterfactuals. In Fauconnier & Sweetser 1996, 57–90.
Fauconnier, Gilles. 1997. Mappings in thought and language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fauconnier, Gilles. 1999. Creativity, simulation, and conceptualization (response to Barsalou 1999a). Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22: 615.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fauconnier, Gilles & Sweetser, Eve (eds.). 1996. Spaces, worlds and grammar. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Fauconnier, Gilles & Turner, Mark. 2002. The way we think: Conceptual blending and the mind's hidden complexities. New York: Basic Books.
Fehr, Ernst, Fischbacher, Urs & Gächter, Simon. 2002. Strong reciprocity, human cooperation and the enforcement of social norms. Human Nature 13: 1–25.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fehr, Ernst & Gächter, Simon. 2002. Altruistic punishment in humans. Nature 415: 137–140.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Feigenson, Lisa & Carey, Susan. 2003. Tracking individuals via object-files: Evidence from infants' manual search. Developmental Science 6: 568–584.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Feigenson, Lisa & Carey, Susan. 2005. On the limits of infants' quantification of small object arrays. Cognition 97: 292–313.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fernandez, James (ed.). 1991. Beyond metaphor: The theory of tropes in anthropology. Stanford: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Fesmire, Steven. 1994. What is “cognitive” about cognitive linguistics? Metaphor and Symbolic Activity 9: 149–154.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Feyerabend, Paul. 1993. Against method. 3rd ed. New York: Verso.Google Scholar
Figueredo, Aurelio, Jon Sefcek, Geneva Vasquez, Barbara Brumbach, James King & W. Jake Jacobs. 2005. Evolutionary personality psychology. In Buss 2005, 851–877.
Fingarette, Herbert. 1972. Confucius: Secular as sacred. New York: Harper Torchbooks.Google Scholar
Fingarette, Herbert. 2004. Confucius: Du profane au sacré (trans. Charles Le Blanc). Montréal: Les Presses de l'Université de Montréal.Google Scholar
Finocchiaro, Maurice (ed.). 1989. The Galileo affair: A documentary history. Berkeley: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Flanagan, Owen. 1991. Varieties of moral personality: Ethics and psychological realism. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Flanagan, Owen. 1992. Consciousness reconsidered. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Flanagan, Owen. 2002. The problem of the soul: Two visions of mind and how to reconcile them. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Flanagan, Owen & Rorty, Amélie O. (eds.). 1990. Identity, character, and morality: Essays in moral psychology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Flinn, Mark, Geary, David & Ward, Carol. 2005. Ecological dominance, social competition, and coalitionary arms races: Why humans evolved extraordinary intelligence. Evolution and Human Behavior 26: 10–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flombaum, Jonathan, Junge, Justin & Hauser, Mark. 2005. Rhesus monkeys (macaca mulatta) spontaneously compute addition operations over large numbers. Cognition 97: 315–325.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fodor, Jerry. 1983. The modularity of the mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Fodor, Jerry. 1987. Modules, frames, fridgeons, sleeping dogs, and the music of the spheres. In Garfield, Jay (ed.), Modularity in knowledge representation and natural-language understanding, 26–36. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Foucault, Michel. 1971. The order of things: An archaeology of the human sciences. New York: Pantheon.Google Scholar
Foucault, Michel. 1972. The archaeology of knowledge (trans. A. M. Sheridan Smith). New York: Pantheon.CrossRef
Foucault, Michel. 1977. Nietzsche, genealogy, and history. In Bouchard, Donald (ed.), Language, counter-memory, practice: Selected essays and interviews (trans. Donald Bouchard & Sherry Simon), 139–164. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press.Google Scholar
Foucault, Michel. 1978. The history of sexuality. Vol. 1: An introduction (trans. Robert Hurley). New York: Random House.Google Scholar
Fox, Alan. 2005. Process ecology and the “ideal” Dao. Journal of Chinese Philosophy 32.1: 47–57.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fraser, Mariam & Greco, Monica (eds.). 2005. The body: A reader. New York: Routledge.
Frazer, Sir James George. 1922/1963. The golden bough. New York: Macmillan Publishing Company.Google Scholar
Fredrickson, Barbara & Kahneman, Daniel. 1993. Duration neglect in retrospective evaluations of affective episodes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 65: 45–55.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Freeman, Derek. 1983. Margaret Mead and Samoa: The making and unmaking of an anthropological myth. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Friedman, Thomas. 1999. The lexus and the olive tree. New York: Farrar, Straus & Giroux.Google Scholar
Frisina, Warren. 2002. The unity of knowledge and action: Toward a nonrepresentational theory of knowledge. Albany, NY: SUNY Press.Google Scholar
Gadamer, Hans-Georg. 1975. Truth and method. New York: Continuum.Google Scholar
Gadamer, Hans-Georg. 1976a. Man and language. In Linge, David (trans. and ed.), Philosophical Hermeneutics, 59–68. Berkeley: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Gadamer, Hans-Georg. 1976b. On the problem of self-understanding. In Linge 1976a, 44–58.
Gadamer, Hans-Georg. 1976c. The universality of the hermeneutical problem. In Linge 1976a, 3–17.
Galef, Bennett. 1987. Social influences on the identification of toxic foods by Norway rats. Animal Learning and Behavior 15: 327–332.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gallagher, Helen & Frith, Christopher. 2003. Functional imaging of “theory of mind.”Trends in Cognitive Sciences 7: 77–83.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gallagher, Shaun. 2005. How the body shapes the mind. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gallese, Vittorio, Fadiga, Luciano, Fogassi, Leonardo & Rizzolatti, Giacomo. 1996. Action recognition in the premotor cortex. Brain 119: 593–609.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gallese, Vittorio & Goldman, Alvin. 1998. Mirror neurons and the simulation theory of mind-reading. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 2: 493–501.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gallese, Vittorio & Lakoff, George. 2005. The brain's concepts: The role of the sensory-motor system in conceptual knowledge. Cognitive Neuropsychology 22: 455–479.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Galton, Francis. 1880. Statistics of mental imagery. Mind 5: 301–318.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garcia, John & Koelling, Robert. 1966. Relation of cue to consequence in avoidance learning. Psychonomics Science 4: 123–124.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gardner, Martin. 1970. Mathematical games. Scientific American 223: 120–123.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gardner, Martin. 1971. Mathematical games. Scientific American 224: 112–117.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gazzaniga, Michael. 1998. The mind's past. Berkeley: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Gazzaniga, Michael 2001. The cognitive neurosciences. 2nd ed. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
Gazzaniga, Michael & LeDoux, Joseph. 1978. The integrated mind. New York: Plenum.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Geaney, Jane. 2002. On the epistemology of the senses in early Chinese thought. Honolulu: University of Hawaii Press.Google Scholar
Geertz, Clifford. 1973. The interpretation of cultures: Selected essays. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Geertz, Clifford. 1984/2000. Anti anti-relativism. The American Anthropologist 86: 263–278. (Reprinted in Geertz 2000.)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Geertz, Clifford. 1997/2000. The legacy of Thomas Kuhn: The right text at the right time. Common Knowledge 6: 1–5. (Reprinted in Geertz 2000, 160–166.)Google Scholar
Geertz, Clifford. 2000. Available light: Anthropological reflections on philosophical topics. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Gelman, Rochel. 1990. First principles organize attention to and learning about relevant data: Number and the animate-inanimate distinction as examples. Cognitive Science 14: 79–106.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gelman, Rochel. 1991. Epigenetic foundation of knowledge structures: Initial and transcendent constructions. In Carey & Gelman 1991, 293–322.
Gelman, Rochel & Gallistel, Randy. 2004. Language and the origin of numerical concepts. Science 306: 441–443.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gelman, Susan. 2003. The essential child. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gelman, Susan. 2004. Psychological essentialism in children. Trends in Cognitive Science 8: 404–409.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gelman, Susan, John Coley & Gail Gottfried. 1994. Essentialist beliefs in children: The acquisition of concepts and theories. In Hirschfeld & Gelman 1994a, 341–365.
Gelman, Susan & Gottfried, Gail. 1996. Children's causal explanations of animate and inanimate motion. Child Development 67: 1970–1987.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gelman, Susan & Wellman, Henry. 1991. Insides and essences: Early understandings of the nonobvious. Cognition 38: 213–244.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gentner, Dedre & Donald Gentner. 1983. Flowing water or teeming crowds: Mental models of electricity. In Gentner, Dedre & Stevens, Albert (eds.), Mental models, 99–129. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Gentner, Dedre, Holyoak, Keith & Kokinov, Boicho (eds.). 2001. The analogical mind: Perspectives from cognitive science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Gentner, Dedre, Imai, Mutsumi & Boroditsky, Lera. 2002. As time goes by: Evidence for two systems in processing space-time metaphors. Language and Cognitive Processes 17: 537–565.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gentner, Timothy, Fenn, Kimberly, Margoliash, Daniel & Nusbaum, Howard. 2006. Recursive syntactic pattern learning by songbirds. Nature 440: 1204–1207.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gergely, György, Zoltán Nádasdy, , Csibra, Gergely & Bíró, Szilvia. 1995. Taking the intentional stance at 12 months of age. Cognition 56: 165–193.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gerstmann, Josef. 1940. Syndrome of finger agnosia, disorientation for right and left, agraphia, acalculia. Archives of Neurology and Psychology 44: 398–408.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Geurts, Kathryn. 2003. Culture and the senses: Bodily ways of knowing in an African community. Berkeley: University of California Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gibbons, Euell. 1962. Stalking the wild asparagus. New York: David McKay Company.Google Scholar
Gibbs, Raymond. 1994. The poetics of mind: Figurative thought, language, and understanding. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Gibbs, Raymond. 1999. Taking metaphor out of our heads and putting it into the cultural world. In Gibbs & Steen 1999, 145–166.
Gibbs, Raymond. 2003. Embodied experience and linguistic meaning. Brain and Language 84: 1–15.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gibbs, Raymond. 2005. Embodiment in metaphorical imagination. In Pecher & Zwaan 2005, 65–92.
Gibbs, Raymond. 2006. Embodiment and cognitive science. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Gibbs, Raymond & Berg, Eric. 1999. Embodied metaphor in perceptual symbols (response to Barsalou 1999a). Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22: 617–618.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gibbs, Raymond & Colston, Herbert. 1995. The cognitive psychological reality of image schemas and their transformations. Cognitive Linguistics 6: 347–378.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gibbs, Raymond & Steen, Gerard (eds.). 1999. Metaphor in cognitive linguistics. Philadelphia: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gibson, James. 1979. The ecological approach to visual perception. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
Gifford, Julie. 2004. The art of seeing the invisible: An interpretation of the terraces atop Barabudur. Presented at the American Academy of Religion Annual Meeting, San Antonio, TX, November 22.
Gigerenzer, Gerd. 2000. Adaptive thinking: Rationality in the real world. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gigerenzer, Gerd & Goldstein, Daniel. 1996. Reasoning the fast and frugal way: Models of bounded rationality. Psychological Review 103: 650–669.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gigerenzer, Gerd, & Selten, Reinhard (eds.). 2001. Bounded rationality: The adaptive toolbox. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Gigerenzer, Gerd, Todd, Peter & the ABC Research Group (eds.). 1999. Simple heuristics that make us smart. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gintis, Herbert. 2000. Strong reciprocity and human sociality. Journal of Theoretical Biology 206: 169–179.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Giorell0, Giulio. 1997. Sì, abbiamo un anima. Ma è fatta di tanti piccoli robot (interview with Daniel Dennett). Corriere della Sera (Milan), April 28.Google Scholar
Glenberg, Arthur, David Havas, Raymond Becker & Mike Rinck. 2005. Grounding language in bodily states. In Pecher & Zwaan 2005, 115–28.
Glenberg, Arthur & Kaschak, Michael. 2002. Grounding language in action. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 9: 558–565.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldstein, Daniel & Gigerenzer, Gerd. 2002. Models of ecological rationality: The recognition heuristic. Psychological Review 109: 75–90.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gonzalez-Marquez, Monica & Michael Spivey. 2004. Mapping from real to abstract locations: Experimental evidence from the Spanish verb estar. Unpublished manuscript.
Melvyn, Goodale, Milner, A. David, Jakobson, Lorna & Carey, David. 1991. A neurological dissociation between perceiving objects and grasping them. Nature 349: 154–156.Google Scholar
Goodall, Jane. 1979. Life and death at Gombe. National Geographic 155: 592–621.Google Scholar
Goodman, Nelson. 1954/1983. Fact, fiction and forecast. 4th ed. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Gopnik, Alison & Henry Wellman. 1994. The theory theory. In Hirschfeld & Gelman 1994a, 257–293.
Gordon, Peter. 2004. Numerical cognition without words: Evidence from Amazonia. Science 306: 496–499.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gordon, Robert. 1986. Folk psychology as simulation. Mind and Language 1: 158–171.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gore, Jeff, Bryant, Zev, Stone, Michael, Nöllman, Marcelo, Cozzarelli, Nicholas & Bustamante, Carlos. 2006. Mechanochemical analysis of DNA gyrase using rotor bead tracking. Nature Reviews 439: 100–104.Google ScholarPubMed
Gore, Rick. 2000. The dawn of humans: People like us. National Geographic 198: 90–117.Google Scholar
Gould, Stephen Jay. 1997. Nonoverlapping magisteria. Natural History 106: 16–22.Google Scholar
Gould, Stephen Jay. 2000. More things in heaven and earth. In Rose & Rose 2000, 101–125.
Grady, Joseph. 1997. Foundations of meaning: Primary metaphors and primary scenes. Ph.D. diss. University of California, Berkeley.Google Scholar
Grady, Joseph, Todd Oakley & Seana Coulson. 1999. Blending and metaphor. In Gibbs & Steen 1999, 101–124.
Grafen, Alan & Ridley, Mark (eds.). 2006. Richard Dawkins: How a scientist changed the way we think. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Greene, Joshua. 2003. From neural “is” to moral “ought”: What are the moral implications of neuroscientific moral psychology? Nature Reviews Neuroscience 4: 847–850.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greene, Joshua. (in press). The secret joke of Kant's soul. In Sinnott-Armstrong, W. (ed.), Moral psychology. Vol. 3: The neuroscience of morality: Emotion, disease, and development. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Greene, Joshua & Haidt, Jonathan. 2002. How (and where) does moral judgment work? Trends in Cognitive Science 6: 517–523.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grewel, F. 1952. Acalculia. Brain 75: 397–427.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Griffin, Donald. 1984. Animal thinking. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Gross, Paul & Levitt, Norman. 1994. Higher superstition: The academic left and its quarrels with science. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.Google Scholar
Gross, Paul, Levitt, Norman & Lewis, Martin. 1996. The flight from science and reason. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.Google Scholar
Gumperz, John & Levinson, Stephen (eds). 1996. Rethinking linguistic relativity. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Guthrie, Stewart. 1993. Faces in the clouds: A new theory of religion. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Haack, Susan. 2003. Defending science within reason: Between scientism and cynicism. Amherst, NY: Prometheus Books.Google Scholar
Hacking, Ian (ed.). 1981. Scientific revolutions. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hacking, Ian. 1983a. Experimentation and scientific realism. Philosophical Topics 13: 71–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hacking, Ian. 1983b. Representing and intervening: Introductory topics in the philosophy of natural science. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hacking, Ian. 1999. The social construction of what. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Haidt, Jonathan. 2001. The emotional dog and its rational tail: A social intuitionist approach to moral judgment. Psychological Review 108: 813–834.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haidt, Jonathan, Koller, Silvia & Dias, Maria. 1993. Affect, culture, and morality, or is it wrong to eat your dog? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 65: 613–628.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hamilton, William. 1964. The genetical evolution of social behaviour (I and II). Journal of Theoretical Biology 7: 1–16, 17–52.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hampe, Beate (ed.). 2005. From perception to meaning: Image schemas in cognitive linguistics. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hansen, Chad. 1983. Language and logic in early China. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press.Google Scholar
Hansen, Chad. 1992. A Daoist theory of Chinese thought. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Haraway, Donna. 1991. Simians, cyborgs, and women: The reinvention of nature. New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Harding, Sandra. 1992. Why physics is a bad model of physics. In Elvee, Richard (ed.), The end of science? Attack and defense, 1–21. Lanham, MD: University Press of America.Google Scholar
Harding, Sandra. 1996. Science is “good to think with.” Social Text 46/47: 15–26.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hare, Brian, Call, Josep, Agnetta, Brian & Tomasello, Michael. 2000. Chimpanzees know what conspecifics do and do not see. Animal Behavior 59: 771–785.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hare, Brian, Call, Josep, Agnetta, Brian & Tomasello, Michael. 2001. Do chimpanzees know what conspecifics know? Animal Behavior 61: 139–151.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hare, Brian, Call, Josep & Tomasello, Michael. 2006. Chimpanzees deceive a human competitor by hiding. Cognition 101: 495–514.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harris, Judith. 1998. The nurture assumption: Why children turn out the way they do. New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
Hauser, Marc. 2005. Our chimpanzee mind. Nature 437: 60–63.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heidegger, Martin. 1962. Being and Time (trans. John Macquarrie & Edward Robinson). New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
Heidegger, Martin. 1971. Poetry, language, thought (trans. Albert Hofstadter). New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
Heidegger, Martin. 1993a. Building, dwelling, thinking. In David Krell, Martin Heidegger: Basic writings, 2nd ed., 344–363. New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
Heidegger, Martin. 1993b. Letter on humanism. In Krell 1993, 217–265.
Heidegger, Martin. 1993c. On the essence of truth. In Krell 1993, 115–138.
Heidegger, Martin. 1993d. The origin of the work of art. In Krell 1993, 143–212.
Heidegger, Martin. 1993e. The way to language. In Krell 1993, 397–426.
Heider, Fritz & Mary-Ann Simmel, . 1944. An experimental study of apparent behavior. American Journal of Psychology 57: 243–259.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heise, Ursula. 2004. Science, technology, and postmodernism. In Connor 2004, 136–167.
Hempel, Carl. 1966. Philosophy of natural science. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
Henrich, Joseph & Boyd, Robert. 2001. Why people punish defectors: Weak conformist transmission can stabilize costly enforcement of norms in cooperative dilemmas. Journal of Theoretical Biology 208: 79–89.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Henrich, Joseph, McElreath, Richard, Barr, Abigail, Ensminger, Jean, Barrett, Clark, Bolyanatz, Alexander, Cardenas, Juan Camilo, Gurven, Michael, Gwako, Edwins, Henrich, Natalie, Lesorogol, Carolyn, Marlowe, Frank, Tracer, David & Ziker, John. 2006. Costly punishment across human societies. Nature 312: 1767–1770.Google ScholarPubMed
Hespos, Susan & Renée Baillargeon, . 2006. Décalage in infants' knowledge about occlusion and containment events: Converging evidence from action tasks. Cognition 99: B31–B41.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hesse, Mary. 1966. Models and analogies in science. Notre Dame, IN: University of Notre Dame Press.Google Scholar
Hiraga, Masako. 1995. Literary pragmatics: Cognitive metaphor and the structure of the poetic text. New York: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Hiraga, Masako. 1999. deference as distance: Metaphorical base of honorific verb construction in Japanese. In Hiraga, Masako, Sinha, Chris & Wilcox, Sherman (eds.), Cultural, psychological and typological issues in cognitive linguistics: Selected papers of the bi-annual ICLA meeting in Albuquerque, July 1995, 47–68. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hirschfeld, Lawrence & Gelman, Susan (eds.). 1994a. Mapping the mind: Domain specificity in cognition and culture. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hirschfeld, Lawrence & Susan Gelman. 1994b. Toward a topography of mind: An introduction to domain specificity. In Hirschfeld & Gelman 1994a, 3–35.
Hirschkop, Ken. 2000. Cultural studies and its discontents: A comment on the Sokal affair. In The Editors of Lingua Franca 2000, 230–233.Google Scholar
Hogan, Patrick. 2003. Cognitive science, literature, and the arts: A guide for humanists. New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Holton, Gerald. 1993. Science and anti-science. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Holyoak, Keith & Thagard, Paul. 1995. Mental leaps: Analogy in creative thought. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Hooker, Clifford. 1981. Towards a general theory of reduction. Part I: Historical and scientific setting. Part II: Identity in reduction. Part III: Cross-categorical reduction. Dialogue 20: 38–59, 201–236, and 496–529.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Horton, Robin. 1993. Patterns of thought in Africa and the West: Essays on magic, religion and science. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hubbard, Edward, Arman, A. Cyrus, Ramachandran, V. S. & Boynton, Geoffrey. 2005. Individual differences among grapheme-color synesthetes: Brain-behavior correlations. Neuron 45: 975–985.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hubbard, Edward & Ramachandran, V. S.. 2001. Cross wiring and the neural basis of synasthesia. Investigative Opthamology and Visual Science 42: S712.Google Scholar
Hubbard, Edward & Ramachandran, V. S.. 2005. Neurocognitive mechanisms of synesthesia. Neuron 48: 509–520.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hubbard, Timothy. 1996. Synesthesia-like mappings of lightness, pitch and melodic interval. American Journal of Psychology 109: 219–238.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hume, David. 1739/1978. A treatise of human nature. 2nd ed. (ed. Selby-Bigge, L. A.). Oxford: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Hume, David. 1777/1975. Enquiries concerning human understanding and concerning the principles of morals. 3rd ed. (ed. Selby-Bigge, L. A.). Oxford: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Humphrey, Nicholas. 1976. The social function of intellect. In Bateson, P. P. G. & Hinde, R. A. (eds.), Growing points in ethology, 303–318. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Humphrey, Nicholas. 1984. Consciousness regained. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Humphery, Nicolas. 1986. The inner eye. London: Faber and Faber.Google Scholar
Humphrey, Nicholas. 1992. A history of the mind: Evolution and the birth of consciousness. London: Chatto & Windus.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ibarretxe-Antuñano, Iraide. 1999. Metaphorical mappings in the sense of smell. In Gibbs & Steen 1999, 29–46.
Inagaki, Kayako & Hatano, Giyoo. 1993. Young children's understanding of the mind-body distinction. Child Development 64: 1534–1549.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Inagaki, Kayako & Hatano, Giyoo. 2004. Vitalistic causality in young children's naïve biology. Trends in Cognitive Science 8: 356–362.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Irwin, Terence (trans.). 1985. Aristotle: Nichomachean ethics. Indianapolis: Hackett Publishing Company.Google Scholar
Jackendoff, Ray. 1996. How language helps us think. Pragmatics and Cognition 4: 1–34.Google Scholar
Jackson, Frank. 1982. Epiphenomenal qualia. Philosophical Quarterly 32: 127–136.
Jäkel, Olaf. 2003. Hypotheses revisited: The cognitive theory of metaphor applied to religious texts. Metaphorik 2: 20–41.Google Scholar
James, William. 1907/1995. Pragmatism. New York: Dover.Google Scholar
Jammer, Max. 1961. Concepts of mass. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Jefferys, William & James, Berger. 1992. Ockham's razor and Bayesian analysis. American Scientist 89: 64–72.Google Scholar
bowuguan, Jinmenshi. 1998. Guodian Chumu Zhujian. Beijing: Wenwu.Google Scholar
Jochim, Chris. 1998. Just say “no” to no-self in Zhuangzi. In Ames, Roger (ed.), Wandering at ease in the Zhuangzi, 35–74. Albany, NY: SUNY Press.Google Scholar
Johnson, Mark. 1981a (ed.). Philosophical perspectives on metaphor. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.Google Scholar
Johnson, Mark. 1981b. Metaphor in the philosophical tradition. In Johnson 1981a, 3–47.
Johnson, Mark. 1987. The body in the mind: The bodily basis of meaning, imagination and reason. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Johnson, Mark. 1993. Moral imagination: Implications of cognitive science for ethics. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Johnson, Mark. 2005. The philosophical significance of image schemas. In Hampe 2005, 15–34.
Johnson-Laird, Phillip. 1989. Analogy and the exercise of creativity. In Vosniadou, Stella & Ortony, Andrew (eds.), Similarity and analogical reasoning, 313–331. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Joseph, George Gheverghese. 1991. The crest of the peacock. London: Penguin.Google Scholar
Kahneman, Daniel, Slovic, Paul & Tversky, Amos (eds.). 1982. Judgement under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kahneman, Daniel & Tversky, Amos. 2000. Choices, values, and frames. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Kahneman, Daniel, Fredrickson, Barbara, Schreiber, Charles & Redelmeier, Donald. 1993. When more pain is preferred to less: Adding a better end. Psychological Science 4: 401–405.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kahneman, Daniel, Schkade, David & Sunstein, Cass. 1998. Shared outrage and erratic rewards: The psychology of punitive damages. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty 16: 49–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kant, Immanuel. 1785/1964. Groundwork of the metaphysic of morals (trans. H. J. Paton). New York: Harper Torchbooks.Google Scholar
Kanwisher, Nancy. 2006. What's in a face? Science 311: 617–618.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaschak, Michael & Glenberg, Arthur. 2000. Constructing meaning: The role of affordances and grammatical constructions in sentence comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language 43: 508–529.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaschak, Michael, Madden, Carol, Therriault, David, Yaxley, Richard, Aveyard, Mark, Blanchard, Adrienne & Zwaan, Rolf. 2005. Perception of motion affects language processing. Cognition 94: B79–B89.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Katz, Albert, Cacciari, Cristina, Gibbs, Raymond & Turner, Mark. 1998. New York: Oxford University Press.
Katz, Steven. 1978. Language, epistemology, and mysticism. In Katz, Steven (ed.), Mysticism and Philosophical Analysis, 22–74. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Kay, Paul & Kempton, Willett. 1984. What is the Sapir-Whorf hypothesis? American Anthropologist 86: 65–79.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kay, Paul & Regier, Terry. 2006. Language, thought and color: Recent developments. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 10: 51–54.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kay, Paul & Regier, Terry. 2007. Color naming universals: The case of Berinmo. Cognition 102: 289–298.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keil, Frank. 1989. Concepts, kinds, and cognitive development. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Keil, Frank. 1994. The birth and nurturance of concepts by domains: The origins of concepts of living things. In Hirschfeld & Gelman 1994a, 234–254.
Kelemen, Deborah. 1999a. Function, goals, and intention: Children's teleological reasoning about objects. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 3: 461–468.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kelemen, Deborah. 1999b. Why are rocks pointy? Children's preference for teleological explanations of the natural world. Developmental Psychology 35: 1440–1452.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kelemen, Deborah. 2003. British and American children's preferences for teleo-functional explanations. Cognition 88: 201–221.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kelemen, Deborah. 2004. Are children “intuitive theists”? Reasoning and purpose and design in nature. Psychological Science 15: 295–301.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kenshur, Oscar. 1996. The allure of the hybrid: Bruno Latour and the search for a new grand theory. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 775: 288–297.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kenward, Ben, Weir, Alex, Rutz, Christian & Kacelnik, Alex. 2005. Tool manufacture by naïve juvenile crows. Nature 433: 121.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kimmel, Michael. 2005. Culture regained: Situated and compound image schemas. In Hampe 2005, 285–311.
Kitayama, Shinobu, Duffy, Sean, Kawamura, Tadashi & Larsen, Jeff. 2003. Perceiving an object and its context in different cultures: A cultural look at new look. Psychological Science 14: 201–206.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kitcher, Philip. 1985. Vaulting ambition: Sociobiology and the quest for human nature. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Kitcher, Philip. 1993. The advancement of science: Science without legend, objectivity without illusions. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Kitcher, Philip. 1998. A plea for science studies. In Koertge 1998, 32–56.
Knight, Chris, Robin Dunbar, Robin Power & Camilla Power. 1999. An evolutionary approach to human culture. In Dunbar et al. 1999, 1–11.
Knight, Nicola, Sousa, Paulo, Barrett, Justin & Atran, Scott. 2004. Children's attributions of beliefs to humans and God: Cross-cultural evidence. Cognitive Science 28: 117–126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Knoblock, John (trans.). 1994. Xunzi: A translation and study of the complete works. Vol. 3. Stanford: Stanford University Press.CrossRef
Kobayashi, Tessei, Hiraki, Kazuo, Mugitani, Ryoko & Hasegawa, Toshikazu. 2004. Baby arithmetic: One object plus one tone. Cognition 91: B23–B34.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koch, Christof. 2004. The quest for consciousness: A neurobiological approach. Englewood, CO: Roberts & Company.Google Scholar
Koertge, Noretta (ed.). 1998. A house built on sand: Exposing postmodernist myths about science. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koestler, Arthur. 1975. The act of creation. London: Picador.Google Scholar
Köhler, Wolfgang. 1929. Gestalt psychology. New York: Liveright.Google Scholar
Kokinov, Boicho & Alexander Petrov. 2001. Integrating memory and reasoning in analogy-making: The AMBR model. In Gentner et al. 2001, 59–124.
Kosslyn, Stephen. 1994. Image and brain: The resolution of the imagery debate. Camrbidge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Kosslyn, Stephen. 2005. Mental images and the brain. Cognitive Neuropsychology 22: 333–347.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kosslyn, Stephen, Cave, Carolyn Backer, Provost, David & Gierke, Susanne. 1998. Sequential processing in image generation. Cognitive Psychology 20: 319–343.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kövecses, Zoltán. 1986. Metaphors of anger, pride and love: A lexical approach to the structure of concepts. Philadelphia: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kövecses, Zoltán 1990. Emotional concepts. New York: Springer-Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kövecses, Zoltán. 2002. Metaphor: A practical introduction. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Kuhlmeier, Valerie, Wynn, Karen & Bloom, Paul. 2003. Attribution of dispositional states by 12-month-old infants. Psychological Science 14: 402–408.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuhn, Thomas. 1962/1970. The structure of scientific revolutions. Chicago: University of Chicago Press (2nd ed., 1970).Google Scholar
Kuhn, Thomas. 1970. Reflections on my critics. In Lakatos, Imre and Musgrave, Alan (eds.), Criticism and the growth of knowledge, 231–278. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kummer, Hans. 1995. Causal knowledge in animals. In Sperber et al. 1995, 26–36.
Kunst-Wilson, William & Zajonc, Robert. 1980. Affective discrimination of stimuli that cannot be recognized. Science 207: 557–558.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lagalla, Julius Caesar. 1612. De phaenomenis in orbe lunae novi telescopii use a D. Galileo Galilei nunc iterum suscitatis physica disputatio. Venice.Google Scholar
Lakoff, George. 1987. Women, fire and dangerous things: What categories reveal about the mind. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lakoff, George. 1990. The invariance hypothesis: Is abstract reasoning based upon image schemas? Cognitive Linguistics 1: 39–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lakoff, George. 1993. The contemporary theory of metaphor. In Ortony 1993a, 202–251.
Lakoff, George. 1996. Moral politics: What conservatives know that liberals don't. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Lakoff, George. 2004. Don't think of an elephant! Know your values and frame the debate. White River Junction, VT: Chelsea Green.
Lakoff, George & Johnson, Mark. 1980. Metaphors we live by. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Lakoff, George & Johnson, Mark. 1999. Philosophy in the flesh: The embodied mind and its challenge to Western thought. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Lakoff, George & Núũñez, Raphael. 2000. Where mathematics comes from: How the embodied mind brings mathematics into being. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Lakoff, George & Turner, Mark. 1989. More than cool reason: A field guide to poetic metaphor. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Langacker, Ronald. 1987. Foundations of cognitive grammar. Vol. 1: Theoretical prerequisites. Stanford: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Langacker, Ronald. 1991. Foundations of cognitive grammar. Vol. 2: Descriptive application. Stanford: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Langacker, Ronald. 1999. A view from cognitive linguistics (response to Barsalou 1999a). Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22: 625.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Langacker, Ronald. 2005. Dynamicity, ficticity, and scanning: The imaginative basis of logic and linguistic meaning. In Pecher & Zwaan 2005, 164–197.
Latour, Bruno. 1993. We have never been modern (trans. Catherine Porter). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Latour, Bruno. 2004. Why has critique run out of steam? From matters of fact to matters of concern. Critical Inquiry 30: 225–248.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Latour, Bruno & Woolgar, Steven. 1979/1986. Laboratory life: The construction of scientific facts. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press (2nd ed., 1986).Google Scholar
Lau, D. C. (trans.). 1970. Mencius. London: Penguin.Google Scholar
Laudan, Larry. 1990. Demystifying underdetermination. In Savage, C. Wage (ed.), Minnesota studies in the philosophy of science. Vol. 14, 267–297. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.Google Scholar
Laudan, Larry. 1996. Beyond positivism and relativism: Theory, method, and evidence. Boulder, CO: Westview Press.Google Scholar
LeDoux, Joseph. 1996. The emotional brain: The mysterious underpinnings of emotional life. New York: Simon & Schuster.Google Scholar
Leibniz, Gottfried Wilhelm. 1768/1996. New essays on human understanding (trans. and ed. Remnant, Peter & Bennett, Jonathan). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Leslie, Alan. 1982. The perception of causality in infants. Perception 11: 173–186.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leslie, Alan. 1987. Pretence and representation in infancy: The origins of “theory of mind.” Psychological Review 94: 412–426.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leslie, Alan. 1988. The necessity of illusion: Perception and thought in infancy. In Weiskrantz, Lawrence (ed.), Thought without language, 85–210. Oxford: Oxford Science Publications.Google Scholar
Leslie, Alan. 1994. ToMM, ToBy, and agency: Core architecture and domain specificity. In Hirschfeld & Gelman 1994a, 119–148.
Leslie, Alan. 1995. Pretending and believing: Issues in the theory of ToMM. Cognition 50: 193–220.Google Scholar
Leslie, Alan & Keeble, Stephanie. 1987. Do six-month-old infants perceive causality? Cognition 25: 265–288.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lewis, Michael & Haviland-Jones, Jeanette (eds.). 2000. Handbook of emotions. 2nd ed. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Lewontin, Richard. 1991. Biology as ideology: The doctrine of DNA. Concord, Ontario: Anansi Press.Google Scholar
Lewontin, Richard, Rose, Steven & Kamin, Leon. 1984. Not in our genes. New York: Pantheon.Google Scholar
Lipton, Jennifer & Spelke, Elizabeth. 2003. Origins of number sense: Large-number discrimination in human infants. Psychological Science 14: 396–401.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liu, Lydia. 2004. The clash of empires: The invention of China in modern world making. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Lloyd. Elisabeth. 1996. Science and anti-science: Objectivity and its real enemies. In Nelson, Lynn Hankinson & Nelson, Jack (eds.), Feminism, science, and the philosophy of science, 217–259. New York: Kluwer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lloyd, G. E. R. & Sivin, Nathan. 2003. The Way and the word: Science and medicine in early China and Greece. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Locke, John. 1690/1975. An essay concerning human understanding. Oxford: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Lodge, David. 1975. Changing places. London: Penguin Books.Google Scholar
Lodge, David. 1984. Small world: An academic romance. New York: Warner Books.Google Scholar
Lodge, David. 1988. Nice work. New York: Penguin Books.Google Scholar
Lodge, David. 2001. Thinks … A novel. London: Secker & Warburg.Google Scholar
Loehlin, John & Nichols, Robert. 1976. Heredity, environment, and personality: A study of 850 sets of twins. Austin: University of Texas Press.Google Scholar
Loewenstein, George, Weber, Elke, Hsee, Christopher & Welch, Ned. 2001. Risk as feelings. Psychological Bulletin 127: 267–286.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Loy, David. 1987. The cloture of deconstruction: A Mahayana critique of Derrida. International Philosophical Quarterly 27: 59–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lupfer, Michael, Tolliver, Donna & Jackson, Mark. 1996. Explaining life-altering occurrences: A test of the “god-of-the-gaps” hypothesis. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion 35: 379–391.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lyotard, Jean-François. 1984. The postmodern condition: A report on knowledge (trans. Geoff Bennington & Brian Massumi). Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.Google Scholar
MacCormac, Earl. 1976. Metaphor and myth in science and religion. Durham, NC: Duke University Press.Google Scholar
Mandler, Jean. 1992. How to build a baby, II: Conceptual primitives. Psychological Review 99: 587–604.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mandler, Jean. 2004a. The foundations of mind: Origins of conceptual thought. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Mandler, Jean. 2004b. Thought before language. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 8: 508–513.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mann, Charles. 2005. 1491: New revelations of the Americans before Columbus. New York: Knopf.
Marcus, Gary. 2006. Language: Startling starlings. Nature 440: 1117–1118.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marglin, Frederique. 1990. Smallpox in two systems of knowledge. In Marglin & Marglin 1990, 102–144.
Marglin, Frederique & Marglin, Stephen (eds.). 1990. Dominating knowledge: Development, culture and resistance. Oxford: Clarendon Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Markman, Ellen. 1989. Categorization and naming in children: Problems of induction. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Marks, Lawrence. 1975. On colored hearing synesthesia: Cross-modal translations of sensory dimensions. Psychological Bulletin 82: 303–331.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marr, David. 1982. Vision. San Francisco: W. H. Freeman.Google Scholar
Martin, Alex & Chao, Linda. 2001. Semantic memory and the brain: Structure and processes. Current Opinion in Neurobiology 11: 194–201.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martin, Alex, Ungerleider, Leslie & Haxby, James. 1996. Neural correlates of category-specific knowledge. Nature 379: 649–652.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martin, Alex, Ungerleider, Leslie & Haxby, James. 2001. Category specificity and the brain: The sensory-motor model of semantic representations of objects. In Gazzaniga 2001, 1023–1036.
Masuda, Takahiko & Nisbett, Richard. 2001. Attending holistically vs. analytically: Comparing the context sensitivity of Japanese and Americans. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 81: 922–934.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Matsuzawa, Tetsuro. 1985. Colour naming and classification in a chimpanzee (pan troglodytes). Journal of Human Evolution 14: 283–291.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maynard Smith, John. 1964. Group selection and kin selection. Nature 201: 1145–1147.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, Maynard John. 1974. The theory of games and the evolution of animal conflicts. Journal of Theoretical Biology 47: 209–221.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCarthy, Rosaleen & Warrington, Elizabeth. 1988. Evidence for modality specific meaning systems in the brain. Nature 88: 428–429.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCrink, Koleen & Wynn, Karen. 2004. Large-number addition and substraction by 9-month-old infants. Psychological Science 15: 776–791.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCutcheon, Russell. 2006a. “It's a lie. There's no truth to it! It's a sin!”: On the limits of the humanistic study of religion and the costs of saving others from themselves. Journal of the American Academy of Religion 74: 720–750.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCutcheon, Russell. 2006b. A response to Courtright. Journal of the American Academy of Religion 74: 755–756.CrossRef
McEwan, Ian. 2005. Saturday. Toronto: Alfred A. Knopf.Google Scholar
McFarling, Usha. 2003. Stalwart Galileo is vaporized near Jupiter. Los Angeles Times, September 22.Google Scholar
McNeill, David. 1992. Hand and mind: What gestures reveal about thought. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Mead, Margaret. 1928. Coming of age in Samoa: A psychological study of primitive youth for Western civilisation. New York: Blue Ribbon Books.Google Scholar
Medin, Douglas. 1989. Concept and conceptual structure. American Psychologist 44: 1469–1481
Medin, Douglas & Atran, Scott (eds.). 1999. Folkbiology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Medin, Douglas, Ross, Norbert, Atran, Scott, Cox, Douglas, Waukau, Hilary J., Coley, John, Proffitt, Julia & Blok, Sergey. 2006. Folkbiology of freshwater fish. Cognition 99: 237–273.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Megill, Allan. 1985. Prophets of extremity: Nietzsche, Heidegger, Foucault, Derrida. Berkeley: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Meltzoff, Andrew & Moore, Keith. 1977. Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates. Science 198: 74–78.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meltzoff, Andrew & Moore, Keith. 1994. Imitation, memory, and the representation of persons. Infant behavior and development 17: 83–99.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Melville, Stephen. 2004. Postmodernism and art. In Connor 2004, 82–96.
Menand, Louis. 2005. Dangers within and without. Modern Language Association, Profession 2005: 10–17.Google Scholar
Mercader, Julio, Barton, Huw, Gillespie, Jason, Harris, Jack, Kuhn, Steven, Tyler, Robert & Boesch, Christophe. 2007. 4,300-year-old chimpanzee sites and the origins of percussive stone technology. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 104: 3043–3048.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Merquoir, J. G. 1985. Foucault. Berkeley: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Michotte, Albert & Thinès, Georges. 1963. La causalité perceptive. Journal de Psychologie Normale et Pathologique 60: 9–36.Google Scholar
Miller, George. 1979/1993. Images and models, similes and metaphors. In Orton 1979/1993a, 357–400.
David, Milner A. & Goodale, Melvyn. 1995. The visual brain in action. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Mithen, Stephen. 1996. The prehistory of the mind: The cognitive origins of art and science. London: Thames & Hudson.Google Scholar
Mithen, Stephen. 1999. Symbolism and the supernatural. In Dunbar et al. 1999, 147–169.
Yuri, Miyamoto, Nisbett, Richard & Masuda, Takahiko. 2006. Culture and the physical environment: Holistic versus analytic perceptual affordances. Psychological Science 17: 113–119.Google Scholar
Moura, Antonio & Lee, Phyllis. 2004. Capuchin stone tool use in Caatinga dry forest. Science 306: 1909.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mulvenna, Catherine, Hubbard, Edward, Ramachandran, V. S. & Pollick, Frank. 2004. The relationship between synaesthesia and creativity. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience Suppl. 16: 188.Google Scholar
Muraven, Mark, Tice, Dianne & Baumeister, Roy. 1998. Self-control as a limited resource: Regulatory depletion patterns. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 74: 774–789.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nagel, Thomas. 1974. What is it like to be a bat? Philosophical Review 83: 435–450.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nanda, Meera. 1998. The epistemic charity of the social constructivist critics of science and why the Third World should refuse the offer. In Koertge 1998, 286–311.
Nanda, Meera. 2000. The science wars in India. In The Editors of Lingua Franca, 205–213.Google Scholar
Neisser, Ulric. 1976. Cognition and reality: Principles and implications of cognitive psychology. San Francisco: W. H. Freeman.Google Scholar
Nersessian, Nancy. 1992. In the theoretician's laboratory: Thought experimenting as mental modeling. Proceedings of the Philosophical Association of America 2: 291–301.Google Scholar
Nichols, Shaun & Stich, Stephen. 2003. Mindreading: An integrated account of pretence, self-awareness, and understanding other minds. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nieder, Andreas, Diester, Ilka & Tudusciuc, Oana. 2006. Temporal and spatial enumeration processes in the primate parietal cortex. Science 313: 1431–1436.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nietzsche, Friedrich. 1886/1966. Beyond good and evil (trans. Walter Kaufmann). New York: Vintage.Google Scholar
Niffenegger, Audrey. 2003. The time traveler's wife. New York: Harvest Books.Google Scholar
Nisbett, Richard. 2003. The geography of thought: How Asians and Westerners think differently – and why. London: Nicholas Brealey.Google Scholar
Nisbett, Richard, Peng, Kaiping, Choi, Incheol & Norenzayan, Ara. 2001. Culture and systems of thought: Holistic versus analytic cognition. Psychological Review 108: 291–310.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nisbett, Richard & Wilson, Timithy. 1977. Telling more than we can know: Verbal reports on mental processes. Psychological Review 84: 231–259.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nishida, Tomomi. 1987. Local traditions and cultural transmission. In Smuts, Barbara, Dorothy Cheney, Robert Seyfarth & Richard Wrangham (eds.), Primate Societies, 462–474. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Noë, Alva. 2004. Action in perception. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Noë, Alva & Thompson, Evan (eds.). 2002. Vision and mind: Selected readings in the philosophy of perception. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Norenzayan, Ara & Scott Atran. 2004. Cognitive and emotional processes in the cultural transmission of natural and nonnatural beliefs. In Schaller, Mark & Crandall, Christian (eds.), The psychological foundations of culture, 149–169. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Norenzayan, Ara, Atran, Scott, Faulkner, Jason & Schaller, Mark. 2006. Memory and mystery: The cultural selection of minimally counterintuitive narratives. Cognitive Science 30: 531–553.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Norris, Christopher. 1992. Uncritical theory: Postmodernism, intellectuals and the Gulf War. Amherst: University of Massachusetts Press.Google Scholar
Núũñez, Rafael & Freeman, Walter J. (eds.). 1999. Reclaiming cognition: The primacy of action, intention and emotion. Bowling Green, OH: Imprint Academic.Google Scholar
Nussbaum, Martha. 1988. Non-relative virtues: An Aristotelian account. In French, Peter (ed.), Midwest studies in philosophy. Vol. 13: Ethical theory: Character and virtue, 32–53. Notre Dame, IN: University of Notre Dame Press.Google Scholar
Nussbaum, Martha. 2001. Upheavals of thought: The intelligence of the emotions. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oakley, Todd. 2005. Force-dynamical dimensions of rhetorical effect. In Hampe 2005, 443–473.
Oden, David, Thompson, Roger & Premack, David. 2001. Can an ape reason analogically? Comprehension and production of analogical problems by Sarah, a chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes). In Gentner et al. 2001, 471–497.Google Scholar
O'Gorman, Rick, Wilson, David & Miller, Ralph. 2005. Altruistic punishment and helping differ in sensitivity to relatedness, friendship, and future interactions. Evolution and Human Behavior 26: 375–387.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Olds, Linda. 1991. Chinese metaphors of interrelatedness: Re-imagining body, nature, and the feminine. Contemporary Philosophy 13: 16–22.Google Scholar
Onishi, Kristine & Renée Baillargeon, . 2005. Do 15-month-old infants understand false belief? Science 308: 255–258.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Orlov, Tanya, Amit, Daniel, Yakovlev, Volodya, Zohary, Ehud & Hochstein, Shaul. 2006. Memory of ordinal number categories in macaque monkeys. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 18: 399–417.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ortman, Scott. 2000. Conceptual metaphor in the archaeological record: Methods and an example from the American Southwest. American Antiquity 65: 613–645.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ortony, Andrew (ed.). 1979/1993a. Metaphor and thought. 2nd ed. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Ortony, Andrew. 1979/1993b. The role of similarity in similes and metaphors. In Ortony 1979/1993a, 342–356.
Ortony, Andrew, Clore, Gerlad & Collins, Allan. 1990. The cognitive structure of emotion. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Pascual-Leone, Alvaro & Torres, Fernando. 1993. Plasticity in the sensorimotor cortex representation of the reading finger in Braille readers. Brain 116: 39–52.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paul, Elizabeth, Harding, Emma & Mendl, Michael. 2005. Measuring emotional processes in animals: The utility of a cognitive approach. Neuroscience and Behavioral Reviews 29: 469–491.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pecher, Diane, René Zeelenberg, & Barsalou, Lawrence. 2003. Verifying different-modality properties for concepts produces switching costs. Psychological Science 14: 119–124.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pecher, Diane & Zwaan, Rolf (eds.). 2005. Grounding cognition: The role of perception and action in memory, language and thinking. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peirce, Charles Sanders. 1868/1992. Some consequences of four incapacities. In Houser, Nathan & Kloesel, Christian (eds.), The essential Peirce: Selected philosophical writings. Vol. 1, 28–55. Bloomington: Indiana University Press. (Originally published in Journal of Speculative Philosophy 2: 140–157.)Google Scholar
Peirce, Charles Sanders. 1905/1992. What pragmatism is. In Houser, Nathan & Kloesel, Christian (eds.), The essential Peirce: Selected philosophical writings. Vol. 2, 331–345. Bloomington: Indiana University Press. (Originally published in The Monist 15: 161–181.)Google Scholar
Pennisi, Elizabeth. 2006. Social animals prove their smarts. Science 312: 1734–1738.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Penrose, Roger. 1989. The emperor's new mind: Concerning computers, minds, and the laws of physics. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Perani, Daniela & Abutalebi, Jubin. 2005. The neural basis of first and second language processing. Current Opinion in Neurobiology 15: 202–206.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Perner, Josef & Ruffman, Ted. 2005. Infants' insight into the mind: How deep? Science 308: 214–216.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pessoa, Luiz. 2005. To what extent are emotional visual stimuli processed without attention and awareness? Current Opinion in Neurobiology 15: 188–196.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Phillips, Ann & Wellman, Henry. 2005. Infants' understanding of object-directed action. Cognition 98: 137–155.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Piaget, Jean. 1954. The construction of reality in the child (trans. Margaret Cook). New York: Basic Books.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pica, Pierre, Lemer, Cathy, Véronique Izard, & Dehaene, Stanislas. 2004. Exact and approximate arithmetic in an Amazonian Indigene group. Science 306: 499–503.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pinker, Steven. 1994. The language instinct. New York: HarperCollins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pinker, Steven. 1997. How the mind works. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Pinker, Steven. 2002. The blank slate: The modern denial of human nature. New York: Viking.Google Scholar
Pinker, Steven & Paul Bloom. 1992. Natural language and natural selection. In Barkow et al. 1992, 451–494.
Polanyi, Michael. 1967. The tacit dimension. Garden City, NY: Doubleday.Google Scholar
Poldrack, Russell. 2006. Can cognitive processes be inferred from neuroimaging data? Trends in Cognitive Sciences 10: 59–63.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pollitt, Katha. 2000. Pomolotov cocktail and selected responses. In The Editors of Lingua Franca 2000, 96–100.Google Scholar
Popper, Karl. 1934/1959. The logic of scientific discovery. London: Hutchinson.Google Scholar
Poundstone, William. 1985. The recursive universe: Cosmic complexity and the limits of scientific knowledge. New York: Morrow.Google Scholar
Povinelli, Daniel. 2000. Folk physics for apes. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Povinelli, Daniel & Vonk, Jennifer. 2003. Chimpanzee minds: Suspiciously human? Trends in Cognitive Science 7: 157–160.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pratt, Chris & Bryant, Paula. 1990. Young children understand that looking leads to knowing. Child Development 61: 973–983.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Premack, David & Premack, Ann. 1983. The mind of an ape. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Premack, David & Ann Premack. 1995. Origins of human social competence. In Gazzaniga, Michael (ed.), The Cognitive neuroscience, 205–218. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Premack, David & Premack, Ann. 2003. Original intelligence: Unlocking the mysteries of who we are. New York: McGraw Hill.Google Scholar
Preston, Jesse & Epley, Nicholas. 2005. Explanations versus applications: The explanatory power of valuable beliefs. Psychological Science 16: 826–832.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Preston, Stephanie & Waal, Frans. 2002. Empathy: Its ultimate and proximate bases. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 25: 1–72.Google ScholarPubMed
Price, Michael. 2005. Punitive sentiment among the Shuar and in industrialized societies: Cross-cultural similarities. Evolution and Human Behavior 26: 279–287.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Prinz, Jesse. 2005. Passionate thoughts: The emotional embodiment of moral concepts. In Pecher & Zwaan 2005, 93–114.
Prinz, Jesse. 2006. Gut reactions: A perceptual theory of emotion. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pulvermüller, Friedemann. 1999. Words in the brain's language. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22: 253–336.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Putnam, Hilary. 1973. Reductionism and the nature of psychology. Cognition 2: 131–146.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Putnam, Hilary. 1981. The “corroboration” of theories. In Hacking 1981, 60–79.
Putnam, Hilary. 1990. Realism with a human face (ed. James Conant). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Putnam, Hilary. 1999. The threefold cord: Mind, body, and world. New York: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
Pylyshyn, Zenon. 1980. Computation and cognition: Issues in the foundation of cognitive science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3: 111–134.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pylyshyn, Zenon. 1981. The imagery debate: Analogue media vs. tacit knowledge. Psychological Review 88: 16–45.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pylyshyn, Zenon. 2003. Mental imagery: In search of a theory. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 25: 157–237.Google Scholar
Quine, Willard Van Orman. 1951. Two dogmas of empiricism. The Philosophical Review 60: 20–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Quine, Willard Van Orman. 1960. Word and object. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Quine, Willard Van Orman. 1969. Epistemology naturalized. In Quine, W. V. O. (ed.), Ontological relativity and other essays, 69–90. New York: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
Quinn, Naomi. 1991. The cultural basis of metaphor. In Fernandez 1991, 56–93.
Rakison, David & Diane, Poulin-Dubois. 2001. Developmental origin of the animate–inanimate distinction. Psychological Bulletin 127: 209–228.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ramachandran, V. S. 2004. A brief tour of human consciousness: From imposter poodles to purple numbers. New York: PI Press.Google Scholar
Ramachandran, V. S. & Blakeslee, Sandra. 1998. Phantoms in the brain. New York: Quill.Google Scholar
Ramachandran, V. S. & Hubbard, Edward. 2001a. Psychophysical investigations into the neural basis of synaesthesia. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences 268: 979–983.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ramachandran, V. S. & Hubbard, Edward. 2001b. Synaesthesia: A window into perception, thought and language. Journal of Consciousness Studies 8: 3–34.Google Scholar
Ramachandran, V. S. & Hubbard, Edward. 2003. Hearing colors, tasting shapes. Scientific American (May): 53–59.Google ScholarPubMed
Rauschecker, Josef. 1995. Compensatory plasticity and sensory substitution in a cerebral cortex. Trends in Neurosciences 18: 36–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Redelmeier, Donald & Kahneman, Daniel. 1996. Patients' memories of painful medical treatments: Real-time and retrospective evaluations of two minimally invasive procedures. Pain 66: 3–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Redelmeier, Donald, Katz, Joel & Kahneman, Daniel. 2003. Memories of colonoscopy: A randomized trial. Pain 104: 187–194.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Regier, Terry. 1996. The human semantic potential: Spatial language and constrained connectionism. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Rensink, Ron, Regan, J. O' & Clark, J.. 1997. To see or not to see: The need for attention to perceive changes in scenes. Psychological Science 8: 368–373.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Richards, I. A. 1936. The philosophy of rhetoric. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Richards, Janet Radcliffe. 1996. Why feminist epistemology isn't. In Paul Gross et al. 1996, 385–412.
Richardson, Daniel, Spivey, Michael, Barsalou, Lawrence & McRae, Ken. 2003. Spatial representations activated during real-time comprehension of verbs. Cognitive Science 27: 767–780.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Richardson, Daniel, Spivey, Michael, Edelman, Shimon & Naples, Adam. 2001. Language is spatial: Experimental evidence for image schemas of concrete and abstract verbs. Proceedings of the Twenty-third Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, 873–878. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Richerson, Peter & Boyd, Robert. 2004. Not by genes alone: How culture transformed human evolution. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ricoeur, Paul. 1977. The rule of metaphor (trans. Robert Czerny). Toronto: University of Toronto Press.Google Scholar
Ricoeur, Paul. 1981. Creativity in language: Word, polysemy, and metaphor. In Reagan, Charles & Stewart, David (eds.), The philosophy of Paul Ricoeur: An anthology of his work, 120–133. Boston: Beacon Press.Google Scholar
Rizzolatti, Giacomo & Arbib, Michael. 1998. Language within our grasp. Trends in Neuroscience 21: 188–194.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rizzolatti, Giacomo & Craighero, Laila. 2004. The mirror neuron system. Annual Review of Neuroscience 27: 169–192.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rizzolatti, Giacomo, Fadiga, Luciano, Gallese, Vittorio & Fogassi, Leonardo. 1996. Premotor cortex and the recognition of motor actions. Cognitive Brain Research 3: 131–141.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rizzolatti, Giacomo, Fogassi, Leonardo & Gallese, Vittorio. 2001. Neurophysiological mechanisms underlying the understanding and imitation of action. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 2: 661–670.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Robbins, Bruce & Ross, Andrew. 2000. Response: Mystery Science Theater. In The Editors of Lingua Franca 2000, 54–58.Google Scholar
Robertson, Lynn & Sagiv, Noam (eds.). 2005. Synaesthesia: Perspectives from cognitive neuroscience. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Röder, Brigitte, Teder-Sälejärvi, Wolfgang, Sterr, Anette, Rösler, Frank, Hillyard, Steven & Neville, Helen J.. 1999. Improved auditory special tuning in blind humans. Nature 400: 162–166.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rohrer, Tim. 1995. The metaphorical logic of (political) rape: George Bush and the new world order. Metaphor and Symbolic Activity 10: 113–131.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rohrer, Tim. 2001. Pragmatism, ideology and embodiment: William James and the philosophical foundations of cognitive linguistics. In Dirven, Rene (ed.), Language and ideology: Cognitive theoretical approaches. Vol. 1, 49–81. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Rohrer, Tim. 2005. Image schemata in the brain. In Hampe 2005, 165–196.
Rorty, Amélie Oksenberg (ed.). 1980a. Essays on Aristotle's ethics. Berkeley: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Rorty, Amélie Oksenberg (ed.). 1980b. Explaining emotions. Berkeley: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Rorty, Richard. 1970. In defense of eliminative materialism. Review of Metaphysics 24: 112–121.Google Scholar
1979. Philosophy and the mirror of nature. Princeton: Princeton University Press.
Rorty, Richard. 1980. Pragmatism, relativism, and irrationalism. Proceedings and Addresses of the American Philosophical Association 53: 717, 719–738.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rorty, Richard. 1991. Objectivity, relativism, and truth. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Rosch, Eleanor. 1973. Natural categories. Cognitive Psychology 4: 328–350.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosch, Eleanor. 1975. Cognitive representations of semantic categories. Journal of Experimental Psychology 104: 192–233.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosch, Eleanor, Mervis, Carolyn, Gray, Wayne, Johnson, David & Boyes-Braem, Penny. 1976. Basic objects in natural categories. Cognitive Psychology 8: 382–439.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rose, Hilary & Rose, Steven. (eds.) 2000. Alas, poor Darwin! Arguments against evolutionary psychology. New York: Harmony Books.Google Scholar
Rosen, Edward. 1947. The naming of the telescope. New York: Henry Schuman.Google Scholar
Rosenzweig, Mark, Breedlove, S. Marc & Watson, Neil. 2005. Biological psychology: An introduction to behavioral and cognitive neuroscience. 4th ed. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer Associates.Google Scholar
Rozin, Paul & Carol Nemeroff. 1990. The laws of sympathetic magic. In Stigler, James, Schweder, Richard & Herdt, Gilbert (eds.), Cultural Psychology: The Chicago symposia on human development, 205–232. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ruffman, Ted, Garnham, Wendy, Import, Arlina & Connolly, Dan. 2001. Does eye gaze indicate knowledge of false belief? Charting transitions in knowledge. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 80: 201–224.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ruffman, Ted, Slade, Lance & Redman, Jessica. 2005. Young infants' expectations about hidden objects. Cognition 97: 35–43.
Rumbaugh, Duane & Washburn, David. 2003. Intelligence of apes and other rational beings. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ruse, Michael. 1998. Taking Darwin seriously: A naturalistic approach to philosophy. 2nd ed. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar
Ruse, Michael. 2001. Can a Darwinian be a Christian? The relationship between science and religion. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Ryle, Gilbert. 1949. The concept of mind. London: Hutchinson.Google Scholar
Ryle, Gilbert. 1971. “The thinking of thoughts: What is ‘le penseur’ doing?” In Collected papers. Vol. 2, 480–496. London: Hutchinson.Google Scholar
Sacks, Oliver. 1995. An anthropologist on Mars: Seven paradoxical tales. New York: Alfred Knopf.Google Scholar
Sandler, Wendy, Meir, Irit, Padden, Carol & Aronoff, Mark. 2005. The emergence of grammar: Systematic structure in a new language. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 102: 2661–2665.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sanz, Crickette, Morgan, Dave & Gulick, Steve. 2004. New insights into chimpanzees, tools, and termites from the Congo basin. The American Naturalist 164: 567–581.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schick, Theodore (ed.). 2000. Readings in the philosophy of science: From positivism to postmodernism. London: Mayfield.Google Scholar
Neil, Schmid D.. 2002. Yuanqi: Medieval Buddhist narratives from Dunhuang. Ph.D. diss., University of Pennsylvania.Google Scholar
Schooler, Jonathan & Engstler-Schooler, Tanya. 1990. Verbal overshadowing of visual memories: Some things are better left unsaid. Cognitive Psychology 22: 36–71.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schooler, Jonathan & Schreiber, Charles. 2004. Experience, meta-consciousness, and the paradox of introspection. Journal of Consciousness Studies 11: 17–39.Google Scholar
Schreiber, Charles & Kahneman, Daniel. 2000. Determinants of the remembered utility of aversive sounds. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 129: 27–42.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Searle, John. 1980. Minds, brains, and programs. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3: 417–457.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Searle, John. 1983. Intentionality. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Searle, John. 1992. The rediscovery of the mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Searle, John. 1995. The construction of social reality. New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
Searle, John. 1997. The mystery of consciousness. New York: New York Review of Books.Google Scholar
Searle, John. 2004. Mind: A brief introduction. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Sebanz, Natalie, Bekkering, Harold & Günther Knoblich, . 2006. Joint action: Bodies and minds moving together. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 10: 70–76.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Segerstrååle, Ullica. 2000. Defenders of the truth: The battle for sociobiology and beyond. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Sellars, Wilfrid. 1963. Science, perception, and reality. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Senghas, Ann, Kita, Sotaro & Asli Özyürek, . 2004. Children creating core properties of language: Evidence from an emerging sign language in Nicaragua. Science 305: 1779–1782.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shapere, Dudley. 1981. Meaning and scientific change. In Hacking 1981, 28–59.
Shen, Vincent. 2003. Some thoughts on intercultural philosophy and Chinese philosophy. Journal of Chinese Philosophy 30: 357–372.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shepard, Roger & Cooper, Lynn. 1982. Mental images and their transformations. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Silverman, Irwin & Jean Choi. 2005. Locating places. In Buss 2005, 177–199.
Simmons, Kyle & Barsalou, Lawrence. 2003. The similarity-in-topography principle: Reconciling theories of conceptual deficits. Cognitive Neuropsychology 20: 451–486.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kyle, Simmons W., Pecher, Diane, Hamann, Stephan, René Zeelenberg, & Barsalou, Lawrence. 2003. fMRI evidence for modality-specific processing of conceptual knowledge on six modalities. Meeting for the Society of Cognitive Neuroscience. New York.Google Scholar
Simon, Herbert. 1956. Rational choice and the structure of the environment. Psychological Review 63: 129–138.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simons, Daniel & Chabris, Christopher. 1999. Gorillas in our midst: Sustained inattentional blindness for dynamic events. Perception 28: 1059–1074.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simons, Daniel & Levin, Daniel. 1997. Change blindness. Trends in Cognitive Science 1: 261–267.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sinha, Chris & Lopez, Kristine Jensen. 2000. Language, culture and the embodiment of spatial cognition. Cognitive Linguistics 11: 17–41.Google Scholar
Slingerland, Edward. 2000. Why philosophy is not ‘extra’ in understanding the Analects, a review of Brooks and Brooks, The original Analects. Philosophy East and West 50. 1: 137–141, 146–147.Google Scholar
Slingerland, Edward. 2003. Effortless action: Wu-wei as conceptual metaphor and spiritual ideal in early China. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Slingerland, Edward. 2004a. Conceptions of the self in the Zhuangzi: Conceptual metaphor analysis and comparative thought. Philosophy East and West 54: 322–342.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Slingerland, Edward. 2004b. Conceptual metaphor theory as methodology for comparative religion. Journal of the American Academy of Religion 72: 1–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Slingerland, Edward. 2005. Conceptual blending, somatic marking, and normativity: A case example from ancient Chinese. Cognitive Linguistics 16: 557–584.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Slingerland, Edward, Blanchard, Eric & Boyd-Judson, Lyn. 2007. Collision with China: Conceptual metaphor analysis, somatic marking, and the EP-3 incident. International Studies Quarterly 51: 53–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jason, Slone D.. 2004. Theological incorrectness: Why religious people believe what they shouldn't. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Smilek, Daniel, Dixon, Mike, Cudahy, Cera & Merikle, Philip. 2001. Synaesthetic photisms influence visual perception. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 13: 930–936.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, Carol, Snir, Joseph & Grosslight, Lorraine. 1992. Using conceptual models to facilitate conceptual change: The case of weight/density differentiation. Cognition and Instruction 9: 221–283.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, William Cantwell. 1959. The comparative study of religion – Whither and why? In Eliade, Mircea & Kitagawa, Joseph (eds.), The history of religions: Essays in methodology, 31–58. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Sokal, Alan. 1996. Transgressing the boundaries: Toward a transformative hermeneutics of quantum gravity. Social Text 46/47: 217–252.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sokal, Alan. 2000a. Revelation: A physicist experiments with cultural studies. In The Editors of Lingua Franca, 49–53.Google Scholar
Sokal, Alan. 2000b. Why I wrote my parody. In The Editors of Lingua Franca, 127–129.Google Scholar
Sokal, Alan & Bricmont, Jean. 1998. Fashionable nonsense: Postmodern intellectuals' abuse of science. New York: Picador.Google Scholar
Solomon, Robert (ed.). 2003. What is an emotion? Classic and contemporary readings. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Solomon, Robert. 2004. Thinking about feeling: Contemporary philosophers on emotion. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Song, Hyun-joo, Renée Baillargeon, & Fisher, Cynthia. 2005. Can infants attribute to an agent a disposition to perform a particular action? Cognition 98: B45–B55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spelke, Elizabeth. 1985. Preferential looking methods as tools for the study of cognition in infancy. In Gottlieb, Gilbert & Krasnegor, Norman (eds.), Measurement of audition and vision in the first year of postnatal life, 323–363. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Spelke, Elizabeth. 1991. Physical knowledge in infancy: Reflections on Piaget's theory. In Carey & Gelman 1991, 133–169.
Spelke, Elizabeth. 1994. Initial knowledge: Six suggestions. Cognition 50: 433–447.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spelke, Elizabeth, Ann Phillips & Amanda Woodward. 1995. Infants' knowledge of object motion and human action. In Sperber et al. 1995, 44–78.
Sperber, Daniel 1985. Anthropology and psychology: Towards an epidemiology of representations. Man 20: 73–89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sperber, Daniel. 1994. The modularity of thought and the epidemiology of representations. In Hirschfeld & Gelman 1994a, 39–67.
Sperber, Daniel. 1996. Explaining culture: A naturalistic approach. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar
Sperber, Daniel & Hirschfeld, Lawrence. 2004. The cognitive foundations of cultural stability and diversity. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 8: 40–46.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sperber, Daniel, Premack, David & Premack, Ann (eds.). 1995. Causal cognition: A multidisciplinary debate. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Spivey, Michael & Geng, Joy. 2001. Oculomotor mechanisms activated by imagery and memory: Eye movements to absent images. Psychological Research 65: 235–241.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spivey, Michael, Daniel Richardson & Monica Gozalez-Marquez. 2005. On the perceptual-motor and image-schematic infrastructure of language. In Pecher & Zwaan 2005, 246–281.
Stanfield, Robert & Zwaan, Rolf. 2001. The effect of implied orientation derived from verbal context on picture recognition. Psychological Science 121: 153–156.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Steen, Francis & Owens, Stephanie. 2001. Evolution's pedagogy: An adaptationist model of pretense and entertainment. Journal of Cognition and Culture 1: 289–321.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sternberg, Robert (ed.). 1988. The nature of creativity: Contemporary psychological perspectives. Cambridge: Cambridge University PressGoogle Scholar
Sternberg, Robert. 1999. Handbook of creativity. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Stich, Stephen. 1990. The fragmentation of reason. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Styron, William. 1992. Darkness visible. New York: Random House.Google Scholar
Sugiyama, Lawrence, Tooby, John & Cosmides, Leda. 2002. Cross-cultural evidence of cognitive adaptations for social exchange among the Shiwiar of Ecuadorian Amazonia. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 99: 11537–11542.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sweetser, Eve. 1987. The definition of “lie.” In Holland, Dorothy & Quinn, Naomi (eds.), Cultural models in language and thought, 43–66. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sweetser, Eve. 1990. From etymology to pragmatics: Metaphorical and cultural aspects of semantic structure. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sweetser, Eve & Dancygier, Barbara. 2005. Mental spaces in grammar: Conditional constructions. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Swindale, Nicholas. 2001. Cortical cartography: What's in a map? Current Biology 11: R764–767.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Talmy, Leonard. 1988. Force dynamics in language and cognition. Cognitive Science 12: 49–100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Talmy, Leonard. 2000. Towards a cognitive semantics. (2 vols.) Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Tanel, Daniel, Damasio, Hannah & Damasio, Antonio. 1997. A neural basis for the retrieval of conceptual knowledge. Neuropsychologia 35: 1319–1327.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Taub, Sarah. 2001. Language from the body: Iconicity and metaphor in American Sign Language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Taylor, Charles. 1989. Sources of the self: The makings of modern identity. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Taylor, Charles. 1992. The ethics of authenticity. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Terranova, Tiziana. 2004. Communication beyond meaning: On the cultural politics of information. Social Text 22: 51–73.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thomas, Thach W., Goodkin, Howard & Keating, James. 1992. The cerebellum and the adaptive coordination of movement. Annual Review of Neuroscience 15: 403–442.Google Scholar
The Editors of Lingua Franca (eds.). 2000. The Sokal hoax: The sham that shook the Academy. Lincoln: University of Nebraksha Press.
Thompson, Evan. 2007. Mind in life: Biology, phenomenology, and the sciences of mind. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Thornhill, Randy & Palmer, Craig. 2000. A natural history of rape: Biological bases of sexual coercion. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Tillich, Paul. 1957. The dynamics of faith. New York: Harper.Google Scholar
Tomasello, Michael, Call, Josep & Hare, Brian. 2003. Chimpanzees understand psychological states – The question is which ones and to what extent. Trends in Cognitive Science 7: 153–156.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tooby, John & Cosmides, Leda. 1990. The past explains the present: Emotional adaptations and the structure of ancestral environments. Ethology and Sociobiology 11: 375–424.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tooby, John & Leda Cosmides. 1992. Psychological foundations of culture. In Barkow et al. 1992, 19–136.
Tooby, John & Leda Cosmides. 2005. Conceptual foundations of evolutionary psychology. In Buss 2005, 5–67.
Tranel, Daniel & Damasio, Antonio. 1993. The covert learning of affective valence does not require structures in hippocampal system or amygdala. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 5: 79–88.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tranel, Daniel, Damasio, Hannah & Damasio, Antonio. 1997. A neural basis for the retrieval of conceptual knowledge. Neuropsychologia 35: 1319–1327.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Trivers, Robert. 1971. The evolution of reciprocal altruism. Quarterly Review of Biology 46: 35–57.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trivers, Robert. 1972. Parental investment and sexual selection. In Campbell, Bernard (ed.), Sexual selection and the descent of man, 136–179. Chicago: Aldine Atherton.Google Scholar
Trivers, Robert. 1974. Parent-offspring conflict. American Zoologist 14: 249–264.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tsao, Doris, Freiwald, Winrich, Tootell, Roger & Livingstone, Margaret. 2006. A cortical region consisting entirely of face-selective cells. Science 311: 670–674.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Turnbull, Oliver, Evans, Cathryn, Bunce, Alys, Carzolio, Barbara & O'Connor, Jane. 2005. Emotion-based learning and central executive resources: An investigation of intuition and the Iowa Gambling Task. Brain and Cognition 57: 244–247.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Turnbull, Oliver & Evans, Cathryn. 2006. Preserved complex emotion-based learning in amnesia. Neuropsychologia 44: 300–306.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tversky, Amos. 1977. Features of similarity. Psychological Review 84: 327–352.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tversky, Amos & Kahneman, Daniel. 1974. Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. Science 185: 1124–1131.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Umiltà, Alessandra, Kohler, Evelyne, Gallese, Vittorio, Fogassi, Leonardo, Fadiga, Luciano, Keysers, Christian & Rizzolatti, Giacomo. 2001. I know what you are doing: A neurophysiological study. Neuron 31: 155–165.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ungerleider, Leslie & Mortimer Mishkin. 1982. Two cortical visual systems. In Ingle, D. J., Goodale, M. A. & Mansfield, R. J. W. (eds.), Analysis of visual behavior, 549–586. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Fraassen, Bas. 1980. The scientific image. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vanhaeren, Marian, d'Errico, Francesco, Stringer, Chris, James, Sarah, Todd, Jonathan & Mienis, Henk. 2006. Middle Paleolithic shell beads in Israel and Algeria. Science 23: 1785–1788.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Varela, Francisco, Thompson, Evan & Rosch, Eleanor. 1991. The embodied mind: Cognitive science and human experience. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Vonnegut, Kurt Jr. 1982. Palm Sunday. New York: Dell.Google Scholar
Vosniadou, Stella. 1994. Universal and culture-specific properties of children's mental models of the earth. In Hirshfeld & Gelman 1994, 412–430.
Wang, Su-Hua, Renée Baillargeon, & Paterson, Sarah. 2005. Detecting continuity violations in infancy: A new account and new evidence from covering and tube events. Cognition 95: 129–173.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ward, Jamie, Huckstep, Brett & Tsakanikos, Elias. 2006. Sound-colour synaesthesia: To what extent does it use cross-modal mechanisms common to us all? Cortex 42: 264–280.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Warrington, Elizabeth & McCarthy, Rosaleen. 1983. Category-specific access dysphasia. Brain 106: 859–878.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Warrington, Elizabeth & Shallice, Tim. 1984. Category-specific semantic impairments. Brain 107: 829–854.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Watson, Burton (trans.). 2003. Zhuangzi: Basic writings. New York: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
Wegner, Daniel. 2002. The illusion of conscious will. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Weiskrantz, Lawrence (ed.). 1988. Thought without language. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Weiss, Gail & Haber, Honi Fern (eds.). 1999. Perspectives on embodiment: The intersections of nature and culture. New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Weld, Susan. 1999. Chu law in action: Legal documents from tomb 2 at Baoshan. In Cook, Constance & Major, John (eds.), Defining Chu: Image and reality in early China, 77–97. Honolulu: University of Hawaii Press.Google Scholar
Wellman, Henry. 1990. The child's theory of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Wellman, Henry, Cross, David & Watson, Julanne. 2001. Meta-analysis of theory-of-mind development: The truth about false belief. Child Development 72: 655–684.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wexler, Mark, Kosslyn, Stephen & Berthoz, Alain. 1998. Motor processes in mental rotation. Cognition 68: 77–94.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wheatley, Thalia & Haidt, Jonathan. 2005. Hypnoptic disgust makes moral judgments more severe. Psychological Science 16: 780–784.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whiten, Andrew. 2005a. The second inheritance system of chimpanzees and humans. Nature 437: 52–55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whiten, Andrew. 2005b. Animal culture is real but needs to be clearly defined. Nature 438: 1078.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whiten, Andrew, Horner, Victoria & Waal, Frans. 2005. Conformity to cultural norms of tool use in chimpanzees. Nature 439: 1–4.Google Scholar
Whorf, Benjamin Lee. 1956. Language, thought, and reality: Selected writings of Benjamin Lee Whorf. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Wiemer-Hastings, Katja & Xu, Xu. 2005. Content differences for abstract and concrete concepts. Cognitive Science 29: 719–736.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wiggins, David. 1980. Deliberation and practical reason. In A. Rorty 1989a, 221–240.
Wilcox, Phyllis. 2001. Metaphor in American Sign Language. Washington, DC: Gallaudet University Press.Google Scholar
Williams, Bernard. 1985. Ethics and the limits of philosophy. Cambridge MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Willingham, Daniel & Dunn, Elizabeth. 2003. What neuroimaging and brain localization can do, cannot do, and should not do for social psychology. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 85: 662–671.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilson, Edward O. 1975/2000. Sociobiology: The new synthesis (25th anniversary ed., 2000). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Wilson, E.O. 1978. On human nature. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Wilson, Edward O.. 1998. Consilience: The unity of knowledge. New York: Alfred Knopf.Google Scholar
Wilson, Margaret. 2002. Six views of embodied cognition. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 9: 625–636.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilson, Timothy. 2002. Strangers to ourselves: Discovering the adaptive unconscious. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Wilson, Timothy, Kraft, Dolores & Dunn, Dana. 1989. The disruptive effects of explaining attitudes: The moderating effect of knowledge about the attitude object. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 25: 379–400.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilson, Timothy & Nisbett, Richard. 1978. The accuracy of verbal reports about the effects of stimuli on evaluations and behavior. Social Psychology 41: 118–131.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilson, Timothy & Schooler, Jonathan. 1991. Thinking too much: Introspection can reduce the quality of preferences and decisions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 60: 181–192.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilson, William, Graeme Halford, Brett Gray & Steven Phillips. 2001. The STAR-2 model for mapping hierarchically structured analogs. In Gentner et al. 2001, 125–159.
Winawer, Jonthan, Witthoft, Nathan, Huk, Alex & Boroditsky, Lera. 2005. Common mechanisms for processing of perceived, inferred, and imagined visual motion. Journal of Vision 5: 491.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Winter, Steven. 2001. A clearing in the forest: Law, life, and mind. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Wolf, Hans-Georg. 1994. Folk model of the “internal self” in light of the contemporary view of metaphor: The self as subject and object. Frankfurt: Peter Lang Publishing.Google Scholar
Wynn, Karen. 1992. Addition and subtraction by human infants. Nature 358: 768.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Xu, Fei & Spelke, Elizabeth. 2000. Large number discrimination in 6-month-old infants. Cognition 74: B1–B11.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yazdi, Amir, German, Tim, Defeyter, Margaret Anne & Siegal, Michael. 2006. Competence and performance in belief-desire reasoning across two cultures: The truth, the whole truth and nothing but the truth about false belief? Cognition 100: 343–368.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yuan, Jinmei. 2006. The role of time in the structure of Chinese logic. Philosophy East and West 56. 1: 136–152.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ning, Yu. 1998. The contemporary theory of metaphor: A perspective from Chinese. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Ning, Yu. 2003. Chinese metaphors of thinking. Cognitive Linguistics 14: 141–165.Google Scholar
Zajonc, Robert. 1980. Feeling and thinking: Preferences need no inferences. American Psychologist 35: 151–175.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zajonc, Robert. 1984. On the primacy of affect. American Psychologist 39: 117–123.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zimbardo, Philip, Laberge, Stephen & Butler, Lisa. 1993. Psychophysiological consequences of unexplained arousal: A posthypnotic suggestion paradigm. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 102: 466–473.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zwaan, Rolf. 2004. The immersed experiencer: Toward an embodied theory of language comprehension. In Ross, Brian (ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation. Vol. 44, 35–62. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Zwaan, Rolf & Carol Madden. 2005. Embodied sentence comprehension. In Pecher & Zwaan 2005, 224–245.
Zwaan, Rolf, Stanfield, Robert & Yaxley, Richard. 2002. Language comprehenders mentally represent the shapes of objects. Psychological Science 13: 168–171.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

  • References
  • Edward Slingerland, University of British Columbia, Vancouver
  • Book: What Science Offers the Humanities
  • Online publication: 05 June 2012
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511841163.012
Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

  • References
  • Edward Slingerland, University of British Columbia, Vancouver
  • Book: What Science Offers the Humanities
  • Online publication: 05 June 2012
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511841163.012
Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

  • References
  • Edward Slingerland, University of British Columbia, Vancouver
  • Book: What Science Offers the Humanities
  • Online publication: 05 June 2012
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511841163.012
Available formats
×