Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-dfsvx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-28T09:13:09.769Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

11 - Aspects of motor development in Down syndrome

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 July 2011

Naznin Virji-Babul
Affiliation:
Down Syndrome Research Foundation
Anne Jobling
Affiliation:
University of Queensland
Digby Elliot
Affiliation:
Liverpool John Moores University
Daniel Weeks
Affiliation:
University of Letherbridge
Jean-Adolphe Rondal
Affiliation:
Université de Liège, Belgium
Juan Perera
Affiliation:
Universitat de les Illes Balears, Palma de Mallorca
Donna Spiker
Affiliation:
Stanford Research Institute International
Get access

Summary

Introduction

In the first year of life, typically developing infants make huge strides in motor development. They progress from a limited repertoire of spontaneous and reflex movements to more purposeful, goal-directed movements. Using their arms, they achieve greater balance in more upright positions and progress from sitting and crawling to standing and walking. The rate of motor development is influenced by a number of factors including the maturation of the nervous system, individual/genetic make-up, the ability to process sensory stimulation such as touch, sound, vestibular, muscle and joint sensations, and movement experience within different environmental contexts. While movement experience has always been recognized as important for motor learning, it is only recently that evidence of the central nature of action experience on cognitive development is being explored. As a consequence, there is increased appreciation that infants learn rapidly from active experience and are able to transfer this knowledge to viewing the actions of others (Sommerville et al., 2005).

The onset of locomotion is one of the major transitions in early development and results in changes not only in motor skill but also in perception, spatial cognition, and social and emotional development (Campos et al., 2000). As infants become more mobile and start to explore their environment, they learn not only about their own bodies but also about objects, places, and events that have consequences for mobile exploration. Walking has tremendous implications on all areas of development. The opportunities for exploration, play, and interaction with peers increase significantly.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2011

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Arbib, M. A. (1981). Perceptual structures and distributed motor control. In Brooks, V. B. (ed.), Handbook of Physiology – Section 1: The Nervous System, Vol. 2: Motor Control, pp. 1449–1480. American Physiological Society.Google Scholar
Buccino, G., Binkofski, F., Fink, G. R., et al. (2001). Action observation activates premotor and parietal areas in a somatotopic manner: an fMRI study. European Journal of Neuroscience, 13, 400–404.Google Scholar
Campos, J. J., Anderson, D. I., Barbu-Roth, M. A., et al. (2000). Travel broadens the mind. Infancy, 1, 149–219.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Charlton, J., Ibsen, E., Lavelle, B. M. (2000). Control of manual skills in children with Down syndrome. In Weeks, D. J., Chua, R., Elliott, D. (eds.), Perceptual-Motor Behavior in Down Syndrome, pp. 25–48. Champaign: Human Kinetics.Google Scholar
Chumlea, W. C., Malina, R. M., Rarick, G. L., Seefeldt, V. D. (1979). Growth of short-bones of the hand in children with Down's syndrome. Journal of Mental Deficiency Research, 23, 137–150.Google ScholarPubMed
Cioni, M., Cocilovo, A., Rossi, F., Paci, D., Valle, M. S. (2001). Analysis of ankle kinetics during walking in individuals with Down syndrome. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 106, 470–478.2.0.CO;2>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dunst, C. J. (2000). Revisiting “rethinking early intervention”. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, 20, 95–104.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dyer, S., Gunn, P., Rauh, H., Berry, P. (1990). Motor development in Down's syndrome children: an analysis of the motor scale of the Bayley scale of infant development. In Vermeer, A. (ed.), Motor Development, Adapted Physical Activity and Mental Retardation, pp. 7–20. Basel: Karger.Google Scholar
Gallese, V., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., Rizzolatti, G. (1996). Action recognition in the premotor cortex. Brain, 119(2), 593–609.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Galli, M., Rigoldi, C., Brunner, R., Virji-Babul, N., Albertini, G. (2008). Joint stiffness and gait pattern evaluation in children with Down syndrome. Gait and Posture, 28(3), 502–506.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Guralnick, M. J. (1997). Second generation research in the field of early intervention. In Guralnick, M. J. (ed.), The Effectiveness of Early Intervention, pp. 271–305. Baltimore: Brookes.Google Scholar
Haley, S. M. (1986). Postural reactions in infants with Down syndrome: relationship to motor milestone development and age. Physical Therapy, 66, 17–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haley, S. M. (1987). Sequence of development of postural reactions by infants with Down syndrome. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 29, 674–679.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harris, S. R. (1985). Neuromotor development of infants with Down syndrome. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 27, 99–100.Google Scholar
Harris, S. R. (1988). Down's Syndrome. Papers and Abstracts for Professionals, 11(7), 1–4.Google Scholar
Henderson, S. E. (1985). Motor skill development. In Lane, D. & Stratford, B. (eds.), Current Approaches to Down's Syndrome, pp. 187–218. London: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.Google Scholar
Iacoboni, M., Koski, L. M., Brass, M., et al. (2001). Reafferent copies of imitated actions in the right superior temporal cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 98(24), 13995–13999.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Iarocci, G., Virji-Babul, N., Reebye, P. (2006). The Learn At Play Program (LAPP): merging family, developmental research, early intervention and policy goals for children with Down syndrome. Journal of Policy and Practice in Intellectual Disabilities, 3(1), 11–21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jeannerod, M. (1981). Intersegmental coordination during reaching at natural visual objects. In Long, J. & Baddeley, A. (eds.), Attention and Performance IX, pp. 153–168. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Jeannerod, M. (1984). The timing of nature prehension movements. Journal of Motor Behavior, 16, 235–254.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jobling, A. & Gavidia-Payne, S. (2002). Early schooling for infants and children with diverse abilities. In Ashman, A. & Eiken, J. (eds.), Educating Children with Diverse Abilities, pp. 116–159. Sydney: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
Jobling, A., Virji-Babul, N., Nichols, D. (2006). Children with Down syndrome: discovering the joy of movement. Journal of Physical Education, Recreation and Dance, 77(6), 34–38.CrossRef
Latash, M. L. (2000). Motor coordination in Down syndrome: the role of adaptive changes. In Weeks, D. J., Chua, R., Elliott, D. (eds.), Perceptual-Motor Behavior in Down Syndrome, pp. 199–223. Champaign: Human Kinetics.Google Scholar
Leonard, C. T. (1998). The Neuroscience of Human Movement. St. Louis: Mosby-Year Book.Google Scholar
Lollar, D. J., Simeonsson, R. J., Nanda, U. (2000). Measures of outcomes for children and youth. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 81, S46–S52.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lydic, J. S. & Steele, C. (1979). Assessment of the quality of sitting and gait patterns in children with Down's Syndrome. Physical Therapy, 59, 1489–1494.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacNeill-Shea, S. H. & Mezzomo, J. M. (1985). Relationship of ankle strength and hypermobility to squatting skills in children with Down syndrome. Physical Therapy, 65, 1658–1661.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meltzoff, A. N. & Brooks, R. (2008). Self-experience as a mechanism for learning about others: a training study in social cognition. Developmental Psychology, 44(5), 1257–1265.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Parker, A. W. & Bronks, R. (1980). Gait of children with Down syndrome. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 61, 345–351.Google ScholarPubMed
Parker, A. W., Bronks, R., Snyder, C. W., Jr. (1986). Walking patterns in Down's syndrome. Journal of Mental Deficiency Research, 30, 317–330.
Reid, G. & Block, M. E. (1996). Motor development and physical education. In Stratford, B. & Gunn, P. (eds.), New Approaches to Down Syndrome, pp. 309–340. London: Cassell.Google Scholar
Rizzolatti, G. & Craighero, L. (2004). The mirror-neuron system. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 27, 169–192.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rizzolatti, G., Fadiga, L., Gallese, V., Fogassi, L. (1996). Premotor cortex and the recognition of motor actions. Cognitive Brain Research, 3, 131–141.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Savelsbergh, G., Kamp, J., Ledebt, A., Planinsek, T. (2000). Information-movement coupling in children with Down syndrome. In Weeks, D. J., Chua, R., Elliott, D. (eds.), Perceptual-Motor Behavior in Down Syndrome, pp. 251–276. Champaign: Human Kinetics.Google Scholar
Sommerville, J. A.,Woodward, A. L., Needham, A. (2005). Action experience alters 3-month-old infants' perception of others' actions. Cognition, 96, B1–B11.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spiker, D. & Hopmann, M. R. (1997). The effectiveness of early intervention for children with Down syndrome. In Guralnick, M. J. (ed.), The Effectiveness of Early Intervention, pp. 271–305. Baltimore: Brookes.Google Scholar
Ulrich, B. D. & Ulrich, D. A. (1995). Spontaneous leg movements of infants with Down syndrome and nondisabled infants. Child Development, 66, 1844–1855.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ulrich, D. A., Ulrich, B. D., Angulo-Kinzler, R. M., Yun, J. (2001). Treadmill training of infants with Down syndrome: evidence-based developmental outcomes. Pediatrics, 108, E84.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ulrich, B. D., Ulrich, D. A., Collier, D. H. (1992). Alternating stepping patterns: hidden abilities of 11-month-old infants with Down syndrome. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 34, 233–239.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Virji-Babul, N. & Brown, M. (2004). Stepping over obstacles: anticipatory modifications in children with and without Down syndrome. Experimental Brain Research, 159, 487–490.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Virji-Babul, N., Hovorka, R., Jobling, A. (2006). Playground dynamics: perceptual-motor behaviour and peer interactions of young children with Down syndrome. Journal on Developmental Disabilities, 12(1, Suppl 2), 29–42.Google Scholar
Virji-Babul, N., Moiseev, A., Cheung, T., et al. (2008). Changes in mu rhythm during action observation and execution in adults with Down syndrome: implications for action representation. Neuroscience Letters, 436(2), 177–180.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Virji-Babul, N., Moiseev, A., Cheung, T., et al. (2010). Neural mechanisms underlying action observation in adults with Down syndrome. American Journal of Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities, 115, 113–127.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hofsten, C. (1991). Structuring of early reaching movements: a longitudinal study. Journal of Motor Behavior, 23, 280–292.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wolery, M. (1994). Instructional strategies for teaching children with special needs. In Wolery, M. & Wilbers, J. (eds.), Including Children with Special Needs in Early Childhood Programs, pp. 119–150. Washington, DC: National Association for Education of Young Children.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×