Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-5g6vh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-29T17:38:28.629Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

13 - Prelinguistic and early development, stimulation, and training in children with Down syndrome

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 July 2011

Jean-Adolphe Rondal
Affiliation:
Pontifical Salesian University
Jean-Adolphe Rondal
Affiliation:
Université de Liège, Belgium
Juan Perera
Affiliation:
Universitat de les Illes Balears, Palma de Mallorca
Donna Spiker
Affiliation:
Stanford Research Institute International
Get access

Summary

Language before birth

Language development in typically developing children begins three months before birth. By that time, the auditory system of the fetus/baby is already functional. It is tuned to the speech frequencies (400 to 4000 cycles per second). This is a unique feature of human ontogenesis corresponding to a species-specific predisposition for speech. During the waking periods, every acoustical stimulus exceeding 60 decibels is normally received by the baby's auditory apparatus and treated by the brain. The partial loss of intensity is owing to an energy absorption by the aquatic milieu surrounding the baby and the fact that her/his middle ear is filled with amniotic liquid. As a likely consequence of this exposure, the typically developing baby at birth demonstrates an ability to recognize the mother's voice and individuate it from other voices. This ability is purely prosodic. It relies on the unique tonal and rhythmic characteristics of the mother's voice. This is objectified using the techniques of cognitive–behavioral investigation in neonates (De Boysson-Bardies, 1996). Beyond the particular mother's voice (and through it), typically developing neonates demonstrate an ability to recognize the maternal language (again through its rhythmic characteristics); that is, they can differentiate between the one language that they have been exposed to in utero and other languages (Nazzi et al., 1998).

Young typically developing babies can also differentiate accentuated syllables from non-accentuated ones (Jusczyk et al., 1993). They recognize varied sequences of syllables (Saffran et al., 1996; Marcus et al., 1999).

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2011

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Berger, J. & Cunningham, C. (1983). The development of early vocal behaviours and interaction in Down's syndrome and non-handicapped infant mother. Developmental Psychology, 19, 322–331.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buckley, S. (2001). Literacy and language. In Rondal, J. A. & Buckley, S. (eds.), Speech and Language Intervention in Down Syndrome, pp. 132–153. London: Whurr.Google Scholar
Conners, F. (2001). Phonological working memory difficulty and related interventions. In Rondal, J. A. & Buckley, S. (eds.), Speech and Language Intervention in Down Syndrome, pp. 31–48. London: Whurr.Google Scholar
Castillo-Morales, R. (1991). Die orofaziale Regulationstherapie. Munich: Pflaum.Google Scholar
Andrade, D., Macho, V., Loura, C., Costa, R., Palha, M. (2008). Effects of rapid maxillary expansion in children with Down syndrome. Communication at the Seventh International Congress on Early Intervention in Down Syndrome and Related Genetic Conditions. Palma de Mallorca, Spain (unpublished).Google Scholar
Andrade, D., Tavares, B., Rebelo, P., Palha, M., Tavares, M. (1998). Placa modificada para tratamento de hipotonia oro-muscular em crianças com i dade compreendida entre os 2 meses e os 2 anos. Ortodontia, 3, 111–117.Google Scholar
Boysson-Bardies, B. (1996). Comment la parole vient aux enfants. Paris: Jacob.Google Scholar
Eilers, R., Oller, D., Bull, D., Gavin, W. (1985). Linguistic experience and infant perception. Journal of Child Language, 11, 467–475.Google Scholar
Eimas, P. (1996). The perception and representation of speech by infants. In Morgan, J. & Demuth, K. (eds.), Signal of Syntax, pp. 25–39. Mahwah: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Elliott, D., Weeks, D., Elliott, C. (1987). Cerebral specialization in individuals with Down's syndrome. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 92, 263–271.Google Scholar
Jusczyk, P., Cutler, L., Redanz, N. (1993). Infants' preferences for the predominant stress patterns of English words. Child Development, 64, 675–687.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marcus, G., Vijayan, S., Bandi Rao, S., Vishton, P. (1999). Rule learning by seven-month-old infants. Science, 283, 77–80.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mervis, C. & Becerra, A. (2001). Lexical development and intervention. In Rondal, J. A. & Buckley, S. (eds.), Speech and Language Intervention in Down Syndrome, pp. 63–85. London: Whurr.Google Scholar
Nazzi, T., Bertoncini, J., Mehler, J. (1998). Language discrimination by newborns: towards an understanding of the role of rhythm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 1–11.Google Scholar
Powell, G. & Clibbens, J. (2001). Augmentative communication. In Rondal, J. A. & Buckley, S. (eds.), Speech and Language Intervention in Down Syndrome, pp. 116–131. London: Whurr.Google Scholar
Rondal, J. A. (1995). Exceptional Language Development in Down Syndrome. Implications for the Cognition-Language Relationship. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rondal, J. A. & Edwards, S. (1997). Language in Mental Retardation. London: WhurrGoogle Scholar
Quartino, A. Rasore (2007). Medical therapies in the life span. In Rondal, J. A. & RasoreQuartino, A. (eds.), Therapies and Rehabilitation in Down Syndrome, pp. 43–62. Chichester: Wiley.Google Scholar
Saffran, J., Aslin, R., Newport, E. (1996). Statistical learning by 8-month-old infants. Science, 274, 1926–1928.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shi, R., Werker, J., Morgan, J. (1999). Newborn infants' sensitivity to perceptual cues to lexical and grammatical words. Cognition, 72, B11–B21.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stoel-Gammon, C. (2001). Speech acquisition and approaches to intervention. In Rondal, J. A. & Buckley, S. (eds.), Speech and Language Intervention in Down Syndrome, pp. 49–62. London: Whurr.Google ScholarPubMed
Tristao, R. & Feitosa, M. (2000). Percepçao auditiva e implicacôes para o desenvolvimento global e de linguagem en crianças com sindrome de Down. Arquivos Brasileiros de Psicologia, 52, 118–142.Google Scholar
Yoder, P. & Warren, S. (2001). Relative treatment effects of two prelinguistic communication interventions on language development in toddlers with developmental delays vary by maternal characteristics. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 44, 224–237.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×