Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-r6qrq Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-27T01:51:09.290Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

2 - Past and present of cross-cultural psychology

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 June 2012

Gustav Jahoda
Affiliation:
University of Strathclyde, UK
Fons J. R. van de Vijver
Affiliation:
Universiteit van Tilburg, The Netherlands
Athanasios Chasiotis
Affiliation:
Universiteit van Tilburg, The Netherlands
Seger M. Breugelmans
Affiliation:
Universiteit van Tilburg, The Netherlands
Get access

Summary

Why old history?

Apart from the fact that it is interesting, history in my opinion also has some lessons for us. Before the modern age of increasing specialisation, people from a rich variety of backgrounds were concerned with what we now call problems of culture. The list includes philosophers, travellers and explorers, missionaries, historians, mathematicians and so on. When posing questions and seeking for answers they learned from each other, which in a broad and rather ill-defined sphere like that of culture is still very important. Admittedly their views tended to be speculative, but they should not be dismissed on that account. They were arrived at by inferences from the imperfect information at their disposal, a problem that has by no means disappeared.

When empirical methods came to be developed, much earlier than is generally realised, there remained the issue of bridging the gap between the relatively limited facts ascertained and their wider ramifications. For instance, during the nineteenth century many scholars entered a blind alley when they attributed human differences to race, and were convinced that they had empirically demonstrated this by means of craniology. Can we be certain that some of our own efforts will not in future come to be regarded in a similarly disparaging manner? So history teaches us humility, as well as providing a wider perspective enabling us to see our own contributions in the context of a larger canvas.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2011

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Akkari, A., and Dasen, P. R. (2004). Pédagogies et pédagogues du sud. Paris: L'Harmattan.Google Scholar
Allen, M. W., Ng, S. H., Ikeda, K., Jawan, J. A., Sufi, A. H., Weilson, M., and Yang, K.-S. (2007). Two decades of change in cultural values and economic development in eight East Asian and Pacific Island nations. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 38, 247–69.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bagehot, W. (1872). Physics and politics. London: King.Google Scholar
Barry, H., Child, I. L., and Bacon, M. K. (1959). Relation of child training to subsistence economy. American Anthropologist, 61, 51–63.Google Scholar
Berry, J. W. (1976). Human ecology and cognitive style. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Berry, J. W. (1995). The descendants of a model: Comments on Jahoda (1995). Culture and Psychology, 3, 373–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berry, J. W. (1997). Preface. In Berry, J. W., Poortinga, Y. H. and Pandey, J. (eds.), Handbook of Cross-Cultural Psychology (vol. I, pp. x–xv). Boston, MA: Allyn and Bacon.Google Scholar
Bornstein, M., Toda, S., Azuma, H., Tamis-LeMonda, C., et al. (1990). Mother and infant activity and interaction in Japan and in the United States. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 13, 289–308.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brouwers, S. A., Hemert, D. A., Breugelmans, S. M., and Vijver, F. J. R. (2004). A historical analysis of empirical studies published in The Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology. The Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 35, 251–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burman, E. (2007). Between orientalism and normalization: Cross-cultural lessons from Japan for a critical history of psychology. History of Psychology, 10, 179–98.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caccioppo, J. T., and Berntson, G. G. (eds.) (2005). Social neuroscience: Key readings. New York: Psychology Press.
Camerer, C. F. (2003). Behavioral game theory. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Carothers, J. C. (1953). The African mind in health and disease. Geneva: World Health Organisation.Google Scholar
Casey, C., and Edgerton, R. B. (eds.) (2005). A companion to psychological anthropology. Oxford: Blackwell.CrossRef
Chakkarath, P. (2003). Kultur und Psychologie. Hamburg: Kovac.Google Scholar
Darwin, C. (1873/1965). The expression of emotions in man and animals. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Dasen, P. R. (2007). Book review. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 38, 531–2.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deregowski, J. B. (1980). Perception. In Triandis, H. C. and Lonner, W. (eds.), Handbook of cross-cultural psychology (vol.III, pp. 21–115). Boston, MA: Allyn and Bacon.Google Scholar
Eckensberger, L. H. (2008). Epilog. Dank – Ruckblick – Diskussion. In Plath, I., Graudenz, I. and Breit, H. (eds.), Kultur – Handlung – Demokratie. Wiesbaden: VS Verlag.Google Scholar
Fijneman, Y. A., Willemsen, M. E., and Poortinga, Y. H. (1996). Individualism–collectivism: An empirical study of a conceptual issue. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 27, 381–402.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Forde, D. (1934). Habitat, economy and society. London: Methuen.Google Scholar
Frois, L. (1585/1998). Européens et Japonais: Traité sur les contradictions et différences de mœurs. Paris: Chandeigne.Google Scholar
Galton, F. (1892). Hereditary genius (2nd edn). London: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Geertz, C. (1975). The interpretation of cultures. London: Hutchinson.Google Scholar
Gjerde, P. F. (2004). Culture, power, and experience: Toward a person-centered cultural psychology. Human Development, 47, 138–57.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greenfield, P. M., and Childs, C. P. (1977). Weaving, color terms and pattern representation: Cultural influences and cognitive development among the Zinacantecos of Southern Mexico. Inter-American Journal of Psychology, 11, 23–48.Google Scholar
Henrich, J., Boyd, R., Bowles, S., Camerer, C., Fehr, E., and Gintis, H. (2004). Foundations of human sociality. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., and Norenzayan, (2010). The weirdest people in the world?Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33, 61–83.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hofstede, G. (1980). Culture's consequences: International differences in work-related values. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Hofstede, G. (2001). Culture's consequences: Comparing values, behaviors, institutions and organizations across nations (2nd edn). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Hume, D. (1742/1894). Essays literary, moral and political. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Huntington, E. (1945). Mainsprings of civilization. New York: John Wiley.Google Scholar
Jahoda, G. (1969). Understanding the mechanism of bicycles: A cross-cultural study of developmental change after 13 years. International Journal of Psychology, 4, 103–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jahoda, G. (1993). The colour of a chameleon: Perspectives on concepts of ‘culture’. Cultural Dynamics, 6, 277–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jahoda, G. (1995). The ancestry of a model. Culture and Psychology, 1, 11–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jahoda, G. (1999). Images of savages. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Jahoda, G., and Krewer, B. (1997). History of cross-cultural and cultural psychology. In Berry, J. W., Poortinga, Y. H. and Pandey, J. (eds.), Handbook of cross-cultural psychology (vol. I, pp. 1–42). Boston: Allyn and Bacon.Google Scholar
Kagel, J., and Roth, A. (eds.) (1995). Handbook of experimental economics. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Kağitçibaşi, Ç. (1997). Individualism and collectivism. In Berry, J. W., Segall, M. H. and Kağitçibaşi, Ç. (eds.), Handbook of cross-cultural psychology (vol. III, pp. 1–49). Boston, MA: Allyn and Bacon.Google Scholar
Kardiner, A. (1939). The individual and his society. New York: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
Kroeber, A. L., and Kluckhohn, C. (1952). Culture: A critical review of concepts and definitions. Cambridge, MA: Peabody Museum, vol. 47, no. 1.Google Scholar
Loades, R. H., and Rich, S. G. (1917). Binet tests on South African natives – Zulus. Pedagogical Seminary, 24, 373–83.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Madsen, E. A., Tunney, R. J., Fieldman, G., Plotkin, H. C., Dunbar, R. I. M., Richardson, J.-M., and McFarland, D. (2007). Kinship and altruism: A cross-cultural experimental study. British Journal of Psychology, 98, 339–59.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Markova, I. (2003). Dialogicality and social representations. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Martin, A. L. (1915). Experiments with Binet-Simon tests upon African children, chiefly Kaffirs. Training School Bulletin, 12, 122–3.Google Scholar
Matsumoto, D. (1996). Culture and psychology. Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole.Google Scholar
McDougall, W. (1927). The group mind. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Montesquieu, Charles de Secondat (1964). Œuvres complètes. Paris: Editions du Seuil.Google Scholar
Munroe, R. L., and Munroe, R. H. (1977). Land, labor and the child's cognitive performance among the Logoli. American Ethnologist, 4, 309–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Myers, C. S. (1901). Smell: Reports of the Cambridge Anthropological Expedition to Torres Strait (vol. II, Part II, pp. 169–85). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Nadel, S. F. (1939). The application of intelligence tests in the anthropological field. In Bartlett, F. C., Ginsberg, M., Lindgren, E. J. and Thouless, R. H. (eds.), The study of society: Methods and problems. London: Kegan Paul, Trench, Trubner.Google Scholar
Ohnuki-Tierney, E. (2001). Historicization of the culture concept. History and Anthropology, 12, 213–54.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ombredane, A. (1954). L'exploration de la mentalité des Noirs congolais au moyen d'une épreuve projective: Le Congo T.A.T. Brussels: Institut Royal Colonial Belge.Google Scholar
Pearson, K. (1914–30). The life, letters and labours of Francis Galton (3 vols.). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1928/1995). Genetic logic and sociology. In Smith, L. (ed.), Sociological studies (pp. 184–214). London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Poortinga, Y. H. (1996). Indigenous psychology: Scientific ethnocentrism in a new guise? In Pandey, J., Sinha, D. and Bhawuk, D. P. S. (eds.), Asian contributions to cross-cultural psychology. New Delhi: Sage.Google Scholar
Poortinga, Y. H. (1997). Towards convergence. In Berry, J. W., Poortinga, Y. H. and Pandey, J. (eds.), Handbook of cross-cultural psychology (vol. I, pp. 347–87). Boston, MA: Allyn and Bacon.Google Scholar
Porteus, S. D. (1917). Mental tests with delinquents and Australian Aboriginal children. Psychological Review, 24, 32–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Porteus, S. D. (1937). Primitive intelligence and environment. New York: Macmillan.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rivers, W. H. R. (1901). Introduction and vision. Reports of the Cambridge Anthropological Expedition to Torres Strait, vol. II, part I. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Segall, M. H., Campbell, D. T., and Herskovits, M. J. (1966). The influence of culture on visual perception. Indianapolis, IN: Bobbs-Merill.Google Scholar
Slotkin, J. S. (1965). Readings in early anthropology. Chicago, IL: Aldine.Google Scholar
Smith, P. B., Bond, M. H., and Kağitçibaşi, Ç. (2006). Understanding social psychology across cultures. London: Sage.Google Scholar
Soekefeld, M. (1999). Debating self, identity, and culture in anthropology. Current Anthropology, 40, 417–47.Google Scholar
Sonke, C. J., Boxtel, G. J. M., Griesel, D. J., and Poortinga, Y. H. (2008). Brain wave concomitants of cross-cultural differences in scores on simple cognitive tasks. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 39, 37–54.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Takano, Y., and Sogon, S. (2008). Are Japanese more collectivist than Americans?Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 39, 237–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Toomela, A. (2003). Culture as a semiosphere: On the role of culture in the culture–individual relationship. In Josephs, I. E. (ed.), Dialogicality in development (pp. 129–63). Westport, CT: Praeger.Google Scholar
Triandis, H. C. (1980). Introduction. In Triandis, H. C. and Lambert, W. W. (eds.), Handbook of cross-cultural psychology (vol. I, pp. 1–14). Boston, MA: Allyn and Bacon.Google Scholar
Triandis, H. (1990). Cross-cultural studies of individualism and collectivism. In Dienstbier, R. A. and Berman, J. J. (eds.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation 1989 (pp. 41–133). Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Tylor, E. B. (1871/1958). Primitive culture. New York: Harper.Google Scholar
Tylor, E. B. (1889). On a method of investigating the development of institutions. Journal of the Royal Anthropological Institute of Great Britain and Ireland, 18, 245–72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vijver, F. J. R. (1991). Culture and the development of children's actions. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Lange, P. A. M. (ed.) (2006). Bridging social psychology: Benefits of transdisciplinary approaches. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Woodworth, R. S. (1910). Racial differences in mental traits. Science, 31, 171–86.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wundt, W. (1863). Vorlesungen über die Menschen – und Tierseele. Leipzig: Voss.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×