Hostname: page-component-7bb8b95d7b-2h6rp Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-09-19T01:32:01.719Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Concept innateness, concept continuity, and bootstrapping

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  19 May 2011

Susan Carey
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA 02138. scarey@wjh.harvard.edu

Abstract

The commentators raised issues relevant to all three important theses of The Origin of Concepts (henceforth TOOC). Some questioned the very existence of innate representational primitives, and others questioned my claims about their richness and whether they should be thought of as concepts. Some questioned the existence of conceptual discontinuity in the course of knowledge acquisition and others argued that discontinuity is much more common than was portrayed in TOOC. Some raised issues with my characterization of Quinian bootstrapping, and others questioned the dual factor theory of concepts motivated by my picture of conceptual development.

Type
Authors' Response
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2011

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Bowerman, M. (1974) Learning the structure of causative verbs: A study in the relationship of cognitive, semantic and syntactic development. In: Papers and reports of child language development, vol. 8, pp. 142–78. Department of Linguistics, Stanford University.Google Scholar
Carey, S. (1985) Conceptual change in childhood. Bradford Books, MIT Press.Google Scholar
Carey, S. (1994) Does learning a language require the child to reconceptualize the world? Lingua 92:143–67.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carey, S. (2009) The origin of concepts. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dewar, K. & Xu, F. (2010) Induction, overhypothesis, and the origin of abstract knowledge: Evidence from 9-month-old infants. Psychological Science 21:1871–77.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Diamond, R. & Carey, S. (1986) Why faces are and are not special: An effect of expertise. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 115:107117.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Feigenson, L. & Halberda, J. (2004) Infants chunk object arrays into sets of individuals. Cognition 91:173–90.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frank, M. C., Everett, D. L., Fedorenko, E. & Gibson, E. (2008) Number as cognitive technology: Evidence from Pirahã language and cognition. Cognition 108:819–24.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gauthier, I. & Tarr, M. J. (2002) Unraveling mechanisms for expert object recognition: Bridging brain activity and behavior. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 28(2):431–46.Google ScholarPubMed
Gordon, P. (2004) Numerical cognition without words: Evidence from Amazonia. Science 306(5695):496–99.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kellman, P. J. & Arterberry, M. E. (2000) The cradle of knowledge: Development of perception in infancy. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Kuhn, T. (1977) The essential tension: Selected studies in scientific tradition and change. University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuhn, T. (1982) Commensurability, comparability, communicatability. In: Proceedings of the Biennial Meeting of the Philosophy of Science Association, 2: Symposia and Invited Papers, pp. 669–88. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Laurence, S. & Margolis, E. (2002) Radical concept nativism. Cognition 86:2255.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Le Corre, M. & Carey, S. (2007) One, two, three, four, nothing more: An investigation of the conceptual sources of the counting principles. Cognition 105:395438.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leslie, A. M., Gelman, R. & Gallistel, C. R. (2007) Where integers come from. In: The innate mind, vol. 3: Foundations and the future, ed. Carruthers, P., Laurence, S. & Stich, S., pp. 109–38. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Lombrozo, T., Kelemen, D. & Zaitchik, D. (2007) Inferring design: Evidence of a preference for teleological explanations in patients with Alzheimer's disease. Psychological Science 18(11):9991006.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Machery, E. (2009) Doing without concepts. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Macnamara, J. (1986) A border dispute: The place of logic in psychology. MIT Press.Google Scholar
McLaren, I. P. L. (1997) Categorisation and perceptual learning: an analogue of the face inversion effect. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 50A:257–73.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nersessian, N. (1992) How do scientists think? Capturing the dynamics of conceptual change in science. In: Cognitive models of science, ed. Giere, R., pp. 344. University of Minnesota Press.Google Scholar
Pica, P., Lemer, C., Izard, V. & Dehaene, S. (2004) Exact and approximate arithmetic in an Amazonian indigene group. Science 306(5695):499503.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rozenblit, L. & Keil, F. (2002) The misunderstood limits of folk science: An illusion of explanatory depth. Cognitive Science 26:521–62.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sarnecka, B. & Carey, S. (2008) How counting represents number: What children must learn and when they learn it. Cognition 108(3):662–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shusterman, A., Carey, S. & Spelke, E. S. (2009) Two paths in the development of counting and cardinality. Symposium talk given at the Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research on Child Development, Denver, CO.Google Scholar
Spaepen, E., Coppola, M., Spelke, E., Carey, S. & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2011). Number without a language model. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 108(8):3163–68.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Walsh, V. (2003) A theory of magnitude: Common cortical metrics of time space and quantity. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 7(11):483–88.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weiskopf, D. A. (2008) The origins of concepts. Philosophical Studies 140:359–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wiser, M. & Carey, S. (1983) When heat and temperature were one. In: Mental Models, ed. Genter, D. & Stevens, A., pp. 267–97. Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Xu, F. & Tenenbaum, J. B. (2007) Word learning as Bayesian inference. Psychological Review 114:245–72.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zaitchik, D. & Solomon, G. E. A. (2008) Animist thinking in the elderly and in patients with Alzheimer's disease, Cognitive Neuropsychology 25(1)2737.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zaitchik, D. & Solomon, G. E. A. (2009) Conservation of species, volume, and belief in patients with Alzheimer's disease: The issue of domain specificity and conceptual impairment. Cognitive Neuropsychology 26(6):511–26.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed