Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-r6qrq Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-26T03:41:28.070Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

14 - Emotion

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  18 May 2019

Robert J. Sternberg
Affiliation:
Cornell University, New York
Wade E. Pickren
Affiliation:
Ithaca College, New York
Get access

Summary

This chapter traces the history of emotion in recent US psychology via examination of the provenance of an artifact published in 1964: a graphic of stick figures exhibiting particular emotional expressions, devised by clinical psychologist Lily Dimitrovsky to facilitate children’s reporting of emotions conveyed by voice. In accounting for this artifact’s emergence, both a macrohistory and a microhistory are advanced. In the macrohistorical account, two contrasting approaches to emotion were current circa 1900. The first, termed interoceptive, focused on the description and quantification of internal emotional phenomenology, while the second, termed exteroceptive, derived from Charles Darwin’s groundbreaking work on facial and postural emotional expression across species. After 1920, both of these approaches fused with motivational concepts, and the interoceptive approach to emotion underwent partial eclipse during that behavioristic era. However, after 1950, the resurgence of cognition and neuroscience in psychology coincided with a revival of emotional theory. The microhistory focuses on local contextual factors that determined the creation of the artifact in a syncretic, emotion-friendly intellectual environment, presaging the way emotion became, between 1965 and the present, a prime component of clinical psychological research and practice as well as a means of communication about social and cultural well-being.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2019

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

American Psychological Association (2014). APA Dictionary of Psychology (2nd ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Baker, D. B. (Ed.) (2003). Thick Description and Fine Texture: Studies in the History of Psychology. Akron, OH: University of Akron Press.Google Scholar
Beldoch, M. (1964). Sensitivity to expression of emotional meaning in three modes of communication. In Davitz, J. R., The Communication of Emotional Meaning (pp. 3142). New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Berlyne, D. E. (1973). The vicissitudes of aplopathematic and thelamatoscopic pneumatology (or The hydrography of hedonism). In Berlyne, D. E. & Madsen, K. B. (Eds.), Pleasure, Reward, and Preference: Their Nature, Determinants, and Role in Behavior (pp. 134). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Berry, M. (2018, March 16). A composer of dark explosions turns 100. New York Times. www.nytimes.com/2018/03/16/arts/music/bernd-alois-zimmermann.html.Google Scholar
Biederman, I., & Vessel, E. A. (2006). Perceptual pleasure and the brain. American Scientist, 94, 249255.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boring, E. G. (1915). The sensations of the alimentary canal. American Journal of Psychology, 26, 157.Google Scholar
Boring, E. G. (1929). A History of Experimental Psychology. New York: Century.Google Scholar
Boring, E. G. (1946). Mind and mechanism. American Journal of Psychology, 59, 173192.Google Scholar
Boring, E. G., & Titchener, E. B. (1923). A model for the demonstration of facial expression. American Journal of Psychology, 34, 471485.Google Scholar
Bushman, B. J., Baumeister, R. F., & Phillips, C. M. (2001). Do people aggress to improve their mood? Catharsis beliefs affect regulation opportunity and aggressive responding. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 1732.Google Scholar
Cannon, W. B. (1915). Bodily Changes in Pain, Hunger, Fear, and Rage. New York: Appleton.Google Scholar
Cannon, W. B. (1927). The James-Lange theory of emotions: A critical examination and an alternate theory. American Journal of Psychology, 39, 106124.Google Scholar
Carroll, D. W. (2017). Purpose and Cognition: Edward Tolman and the Transformation of American Psychology. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Damasio, A. (1994). Descartes’ Error: Reason, Emotion, and the Human Brain. New York: Avon.Google Scholar
Darwin, C. (1872). The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals. London: John Murray.Google Scholar
Davitz, J. R. (1964). The Communication of Emotional Meaning (with Beldoch, M., Blau, S., Dimitrovsky, L., Levitt, E. A., Levy, P. K., Mattis, S., & Turner, J. leB.). New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Davitz, J. R. (1969). The Language of Emotion. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Davitz, J., & Davitz, L. (1959). The communication of feelings by content-free speech. Journal of Communication, 9, 613.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Devonis, D. C. (1989). The concept of conscious pleasure in the history of American psychology, 1890–1980. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of New Hampshire.Google Scholar
Devonis, D. C. (2000). Leonard Thompson Troland. In Kazdin, A. (Ed.), Encyclopedia of Psychology (Vol. 8, pp. 124125). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Devonis, D. C. (2017). Neal Miller. In Zeigler-Hill, V. & Shackelford, T. (Eds.) Encyclopedia of Personality and Individual Differences. New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Dimitrovsky, L. (1964). The ability to identify the emotional meaning of vocal expressions at successive age levels. In Davitz, J. R., The Communication of Emotional Meaning (pp. 6986). New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Dixon, T. (2003). From Passions to Emotions: The Creation of a Secular Psychological Category. London: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Dror, O. E. (2017). Deconstructing the “two factors”: The historical origins of the Schachter–Singer theory of emotions. Emotion Review, 9, 716.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duncker, K. (1941). On pleasure, emotion, and striving. Philosophical and Phenomenological Research, 1, 391430.Google Scholar
Ehrenreich, J. ,, T., Fairholme, C. P., Buzzella, B. A., Ellard, K. K., & Barlow, D. H. (2007). The role of emotion in psychological therapy. Clinical Psychology (New York), 14, 422428.Google ScholarPubMed
Ekman, P., Friesen, W., & Ellsworth, P. (1972). Emotion in the Human Face: Guidelines for Research and an Integration of Findings. New York: Pergamon.Google Scholar
El-Hai, J. (2007). The Lobotomist: A Maverick Medical Genius and His Tragic Quest to Rid the World of Mental Illness. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Ellis, A., & Ellis, D. J. (2011). Rational Emotive Behavior Theory (Theories of Psychotherapy). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Freud, S. (1914). The Moses of Michelangelo. In Strachey, J., (Ed.), The Standard Edition of the Complete Psychological Works of Sigmund Freud (Vol. 13, pp. 209238). London: Hogarth Press and the Institute of Psychoanalysis.Google Scholar
Gendron, M., & Barrett, L. (2009). Reconstructing the past: A century of ideas about emotion in psychology. Emotion Review, 1, 316339.Google Scholar
Goodenough, F. (1926). A new approach to the measurement of intelligence in young children. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 33, 185211.Google Scholar
Harlow, H. F. (1958). The nature of love. American Psychologist, 13, 673685.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hebb, D. O. (1946). Emotion in man and animal: An analysis of the intuitive processes of recognition. Psychological Review, 53, 88106.Google Scholar
Henle, M. (1984). Robert M. Ogden and gestalt psychology in America. Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences, 20, 919.Google Scholar
James, W. (1894). Marshall’s pain and pleasure. Nation, 59, 4951.Google Scholar
Johnson-Laird, & Oatley, K. (1989). The language of emotions: An analysis of a semantic field. Cognition and Emotion, 3, 81123.Google Scholar
Johnston, E., & Olson, L. (2015). The Feeling Brain: The Biology and Psychology of Emotions. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
LeDoux, J. (1996). The Emotional Brain: The Mysterious Underpinnings of Emotional Life. New York: Simon & Schuster.Google Scholar
LeDoux, J. (2015). Feelings: What are they and how does the brain make them? Daedalus, 144, 96111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levitt, E. (1967). The Psychology of Anxiety. Indianapolis, IN: Bobbs-Merrill.Google Scholar
Lopez, L. D., Reschke, P. J., Knothe, J. M., & Walle, E. A. (2017). Postural communication of emotion: Perception of distinct poses of five discrete emotions. Frontiers of Psychology, 8, 710. www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5432628.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Machover, K. (1949). Personality Projection in the Drawing of the Human Figure (A Method of Personality Investigation). Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marsh, A. (2017). The Fear Factor: How One Emotion Connects Altruists, Psychopaths, and Everyone In-Between. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Marshall, H. R. (1894). Pain, Pleasure, and Aesthetics: An Essay Concerning the Psychology of Pain and Pleasure. with Special Reference to Aesthetics. London and New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Meehl, P. E. (1975). Hedonic capacity: Some conjectures. Bulletin of the Menninger Clinic, 39, 295307.Google Scholar
Meehl, P. E. (1987). ‘Hedonic capacity’ ten years later: Some clarifications. In Clark, D. C. & Fawcett, J. (Eds.), Anhedonia and Affect Deficit States. New York: PMA Publishing.Google Scholar
Meyer, M. (1933). That whale among the fishes: The theory of emotions. Psychological Review, 40, 292300.Google Scholar
Napoli, D. (1981). Architects of Adjustment: The History of the Psychological Profession in the United States. Port Washington, NY: Kennikat Press.Google Scholar
Norman, D. (1992). Turn Signals are the Facial Expressions of Automobiles: Notes of a Technology Watcher. New York: Addison-Wesley.Google Scholar
Normandin, S., & Wolfe, C. T. (2013). Vitalism and the scientific image: an introduction. In Normandin, S. & Wolfe, C. T. (Eds.), Vitalism and the Scientific Image in Post-Enlightenment Life Science, 1800–2010 (pp. 115). New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Olds, J. (1956). Pleasure centers in the brain. Scientific American, 195,(4) 105116.Google Scholar
Plutchik, R. (2002). Emotions and Life: Perspectives from Psychology, Biology, and Evolution. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Reymert, M. L. (Ed.) (1928). Feelings and Emotions: The Wittenberg Symposium. Worcester, MA: Clark University Press.Google Scholar
Russell, J. A. (1980). A circumplex model of affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 11611178.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Salovey, P., & Mayer, J. D. (1990). Emotional intelligence. Imagination, Cognition, and Personality, 9, 185211.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shaffer, L. (1944). The Psychology of Adjustment: An Objective Approach to Mental Hygiene. Washington, DC: US Armed Forces Institute. (Originally published 1936).Google Scholar
Stearns, P. (2018). An emotional America. American Interest. www.the-american-interest.com/2018/01/08/an-emotional-america.Google Scholar
Sternberg, R. J. (1986). A triangular theory of love. Psychological Review, 93, 119135.Google Scholar
Sundararajan, L. (2015). Understanding Emotion in Chinese Culture: Thinking through Psychology. New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Tolman, E. C. (1932). Purposive Behavior in Animals and Men. Berkeley: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Tolman, E. C. (1948). Cognitive maps in rats and men. Psychological Review, 55, 189208.Google Scholar
Tomkins Institute (n.d.). Nine affects, present at birth, combine with life experience to form emotion and personality. Tomkins Institute. www.tomkins.org/what-tomkins-said/introduction/nine-affects-present-at-birth-combine-to-form-emotion-mood-and-personality.Google Scholar
Tweney, R. (2003). Whatever happened to the brass and glass? The rise of statistical “instruments” in psychology, 1900–1950. In Baker, D. (Ed.), Thick Description and Fine Texture: Essays in the History of Psychology (pp. 123142). Akron, OH: University of Akron Press.Google Scholar
Verhaeghe, J., Gheysen, R., & Enzlin, P. (2013). Pheromones and their effect on women’s mood and sexuality. Facts, Views, & Visions: Issues in Obstetrics, Gynecology, and Reproductive Health, 5, 189195. www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3987372.Google Scholar
Watson, J. B. (1919). A schematic outline of the emotions. Psychological Review, 26, 165196.Google Scholar
Wiggins, J. S. (1996). An informal history of the interpersonal circumplex tradition. Journal of Personality Assessment, 66, 217233.Google Scholar
Woodworth, R. S. (1928). How emotions are identified and classified. In Reymert, M. L. (Ed.), Feelings and Emotions: The Wittenberg Symposium (pp. 222227). Worcester, MA: Clark University Press.Google Scholar
Zajonc, R. B. (1980). Feeling and thinking: Preferences need no inferences. American Psychologist, 35, 151175.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

  • Emotion
  • Edited by Robert J. Sternberg, Cornell University, New York, Wade E. Pickren, Ithaca College, New York
  • Book: The Cambridge Handbook of the Intellectual History of Psychology
  • Online publication: 18 May 2019
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108290876.015
Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

  • Emotion
  • Edited by Robert J. Sternberg, Cornell University, New York, Wade E. Pickren, Ithaca College, New York
  • Book: The Cambridge Handbook of the Intellectual History of Psychology
  • Online publication: 18 May 2019
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108290876.015
Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

  • Emotion
  • Edited by Robert J. Sternberg, Cornell University, New York, Wade E. Pickren, Ithaca College, New York
  • Book: The Cambridge Handbook of the Intellectual History of Psychology
  • Online publication: 18 May 2019
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108290876.015
Available formats
×