Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-45l2p Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-29T08:16:18.654Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

6 - The Use of the Electroencephalogram in Research on Cognitive Development

from SECTION ONE - CENTRAL SYSTEM: THEORY, METHODS, AND MEASURES

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  27 July 2009

Martha Ann Bell
Affiliation:
Associate Professor of Psychology Virginia Polytechnic Institute and State University
Christy D. Wolfe
Affiliation:
Visiting Assistant Professor of Psychological and Brain Sciences University of Louisville
Louis A. Schmidt
Affiliation:
McMaster University, Ontario
Sidney J Segalowitz
Affiliation:
Brock University, Ontario
Get access

Summary

INTRODUCTION

The field of developmental psychophysiology provides the methodology for examination of age-related changes in the functioning of the brain. The electroencephalogram (EEG) is an efficient, non-invasive, and relatively inexpensive method for studying brain development in infants and children and for relating brain development to changes in cognitive behaviors. Utilizing EEG allows for examination of these developmental changes without dramatic interference with normal ongoing behaviors. All of these characteristics make the EEG one of the more favorable methods for investigating brain-behavior relations with young populations (Casey & de Haan, 2002; Taylor & Baldeweg, 2002).

The EEG discussed in this chapter is sometimes called “quantitative EEG” and is used for basic research on brain activity during cognition or emotion and for basic research on brain maturation. Typically, quantitative EEGs used for basic research are digital records that are converted from the time domain to the frequency domain by means of spectral analysis, yielding spectral power at specific frequencies, or by means of phase coherence analysis, yielding the degree to which the EEG signals at two distinct scalp locations are in phase at a specific frequency. This quantitative methodology differs from the traditional use of the EEG in the clinical setting to localize seizures or tumors. It also differs from event-related potentials, or ERPs, which are brain electrical responses that are time locked to a specific set of stimuli. ERP methodology and research is reviewed in Chapters 2, 3, and 4 of this volume.

Type
Chapter
Information
Developmental Psychophysiology
Theory, Systems, and Methods
, pp. 150 - 170
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2007

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Bell, M. A. (1998a). The ontogeny of the EEG during infancy and childhood: Implications for cognitive development. In Barreau, B. (Ed.), Neuroimaging in child neuropsychiatric disorders (pp. 97–111).Berlin: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
Bell, M. A. (1998b). Search for valid infant EEG rhythms: Factor analysis of power data. Poster presented at the Annual Meeting of the Society for Psychophysiological Research, Denver.Google Scholar
Bell, M. A. (2001). Brain electrical activity associated with cognitive processing during a looking version of the A-not-B task. Infancy, 2, 311–330.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bell, M. A. (2002). Power changes in infant EEG frequency bands during a spatial working memory task. Psychophysiology, 39, 450–458.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bell, M. A., & Fox, N. A. (1992). The relations between frontal brain electrical activity and cognitive development during infancy. Child Development, 63, 1142–1163.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bell, M. A., & Fox, N. A. (1997). Individual differences in object permanence performance at 8 months: Locomotor experience and brain electrical activity. Developmental Psychobiology, 31, 287–297.3.0.CO;2-N>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bell, M. A., & Fox, N. A. (2003). Cognition and affective style: Individual differences in brain electrical activity during spatial and verbal tasks. Brain and Cognition, 53, 441–451.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berger, H. (1929). On the electroencephalogram of man. I. Arch. Psychiatr. Nervenkr, 87, 527–570.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berger, H. (1932). On the electroencephalogram of man. V. Arch. Psychiatr. Nervenkr, 98, 231–254.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burgess, A. P., & Gruzelier, J. H. (2000). Short duration power changes in the EEG during recognition memory for words and faces. Psychophysiology, 37, 596–606.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Casey, B. J., & Hann, M. (2002). Introduction: New methods in developmental science. Developmental Science, 5, 265–267.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davidson, R. J., Chapman, J. P., Chapman, L. J., & Henriques, J. B. (1990). Asymmetrical brain electrical activity discriminates between psychometrically-matched verbal and spatial cognitive tasks. Psychophysiology, 27, 528–543.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davidson, R. J., Jackson, D. C., & Larson, C. L. (2000). Human electroencephalography. In Cacioppo, J. T., Tassinary, L. G., & Berntson, G. G. (Eds.), Handbook of psychophysiology (2nd ed., pp. 27–52). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Diamond, A., Prevor, M. B., Callender, G., & Druin, D. P. (1997). Prefrontal cortex cognitive deficits in children treated early and continuously for PKU. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 62 (4, Serial No. 252).Google Scholar
Eeg-Olofsson, O. (1971). The development of the EEG in normal young persons from the age of 16 through 21 years. Neuropadiatrie, 3, 11–45.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Epstein, H. T. (1980). EEG developmental stages. Developmental Psychobiology, 13, 629–631.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fernandez, T., Harmony, T., Silva, J., Galan, L., Diaz-Comas, L., Bosch, J., Rodriguez, M., Fernandez-Bouzas, A., Yanez, G., Otero, G., & Marosi, E. (1998). Relationship of specific EEG frequencies at specific brain areas with performance. NeuroReport, 9, 3681–3687.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Field, T., Fox, N. A., Pickens, J., & Nawrocki, T. (1995). Relative right frontal EEG activation in 3- to 6-month-old infants of “depressed” mothers. Developmental Psychology, 31, 358–363.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fischer, K. W., & Rose, S. P. (1994) Dynamic development of coordination ofcomponents in brain and behavior: A framework for theory and research. In Dawson, G. & Fischer, K. W. (Eds.), Human behavior and the developing brain (pp. 3–45). New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
Hagne, I. (1968). Development of the waking EEG in normal infants during the first year of life. In Kellaway, P. & Petersen, I. (Eds.), Clinical electroencephalography of children (pp. 97–118). New York: Grune & Stratton.Google Scholar
Hagne, I. (1972). Development of the EEG in normal infants during the first year of life. Acta Pediatrica Scandinavia, Supplement 232, 25–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henry, J. R. (1944). Electroencephalograms of normal children. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 9, (3, Serial No. 39).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
John, E. R., Ahn, H., Prichep, L., Trepetin, M., Brown, D., & Kaye, H. (1980). Developmental equations for the electroencephalogram. Science, 210, 1255–1258.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jones, N. A., Field, T., Fox, N. A., Davalos, M., Lundy, B., & Hart, S. (1998). Newborns of mothers with depressive symptoms are physiologically less developed. Infant Behavior & Development, 21, 537–541.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klimesch, W., Doppelmayr, M., Schimke, H., & Ripper, B. (1997). Theta synchronization and alpha desynchronization in a memory task. Psychophysiology, 34, 169–176.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klimesch, W., Doppelmayr, M., Schwaiger, J., Auinger, P., & Winkler, T. (1999). “Paradoxical” alpha synchronization in a memory task. Cognitive Brain Research, 7, 493–501.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krause, C. M., Salminen, P. A., Sillanmaki, L., & Holopainen, I. E. (2001). Event-related desynchronization and synchronization during a memory task in children. Clinical Neurophysiology, 112, 2233–2240.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lindsley, D. B. (1939). A longitudinal study of the occipital alpha rhythm in normal children: Frequency and amplitude standards. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 55, 197–213.Google Scholar
Marshall, P. J., Bar-Haim, Y., & Fox, N. A. (2002). Development of the EEG from 5 months to 4 years of age. Clinical Neurophysiology, 113, 1199–1208.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Matousek, M., & Petersen, I. (1973). Automatic evaluation of EEG background activity by means of age-dependent EEG quotients. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 55, 603–612.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mizuno, T., Yamaguchi, N., Iinuma, K., & Arakawa, T. (1970). Maturation of patterns of EEG: Basic waves of healthy infants under 12 months of age. Tohoku Journal of Experimental Medicine, 102, 91–98.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mundy, P.Card, J., & Fox, N. A. (2000). EEG correlates of the development of infant joint attention skills. Developmental Psychobiology, 36, 325–338.3.0.CO;2-F>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nelson, C. A. (1995). The ontogeny of human memory: A cognitive neuroscience perspective. Developmental Psychology, 31, 723–738.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, C. A., & Monk, C. S. (2001). The use of the event-related potentials in the study of cognitive development. In Nelson, C. A. & Luciana, M. (Eds.), Handbook of developmental cognitive neuroscience (pp. 125–136). Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Orekhova, E. V., Stroganova, T. A., & Posikera, I. N. (1999). Theta synchronization during sustained anticipatory attention in infants over the second half of the first year of life. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 32, 151–172.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Orekhova, E. V., Stroganova, T. A., & Posikera, I. N. (2001). Alpha activity as an index of cortical inhibition during sustained internally controlled attention in infants. Clinical Neurophysiology, 112, 740–749.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petersen, I., & Eeg-Olofsson, O. (1971). The development of the electroencephalogram in normal children from the age of 1 through 15 years: Non-paroxysmal activity. Neuropediatric, 2, 247–301.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pivik, R. T., Broughton, R. J., Coppola, R., Davidson, R. J., Fox, N. A., & Nuwer, M. R. (1993). Guidelines for the recording and quantitative analysis of electroencephalographic activity in research contexts. Psychophysiology, 30, 547–558.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Posner, M. I., & Rothbart, M. K. (2000). Developing mechanisms of self-regulation. Development and Psychopathology, 12, 427–441.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roberts, J. E., & Bell, M. A. (2000). Sex differences on a mental rotation task: Variations in EEG hemispheric activation between children and college students. Developmental Neuropsychology, 17, 199–223.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roberts, J. E., & Bell, M. A. (2002). The effects of age and sex on mental rotation performance, verbal performance, and brain electrical activity. Developmental Psychobiology, 40, 391–407.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roberts, T. A., & Kraft, R. H. (1990). Developmental differences in the relationship between reading comprehension and hemispheric alpha patterns: An EEG study. Journal of Educational Psychology, 81, 322–328.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schacter, D. L. (1977). EEG theta waves and psychological phenomena: A review and analysis. Biological Psychology, 5, 47–82.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schmid, R. G., Tirsch, W. S., & Scherb, H. (2002). Correlation between spectral EEG parameters and intelligence test variables in school-age children. Clinical Neurophysiology, 113, 1647–1656.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, J. R. (1938a). The electroencephalogram during normal infancy and childhood: I. Rhythmic activities present in the neonate and their subsequent development. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 53, 431–453.Google Scholar
Smith, J. R. (1938b). The electroencephalogram during normal infancy and childhood: II. The nature and growth of the alpha waves. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 53, 455–469.Google Scholar
Smith, J. R. (1939). The “occipital” and “pre-central” alpha rhythms during the first two years. Journal of Psychology, 7, 223–226.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, J. R. (1941). The frequency growth of the human alpha rhythms during normal infancy and childhood. Journal of Psychology, 11, 177–198.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stern, R. M., Ray, W. J., & Quigley, K. S. (2001). Psychological recording (2nd ed.). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Stroganova, T. A., Orekhova, E. V., & Posikera, I. N. (1998). Externally and internally controlled attention in infants: An EEG study. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 30, 339–351.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stroganova, T. A., Orekhova, E. V., & Posikera, I. N. (1999). EEG alpha rhythm in infants. Clinical Neurophysiology, 110, 997–1012.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taylor, M. J., & Baldeweg, T. (2002). Application of EEG, ERP, and intracranial recordings to the investigation of cognitive functions in children. Developmental Science, 5, 318–334.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baal, G. C. M., Geus, E. J. C., & Boomsma, D. I. (1996). Genetic architecture of EEG power spectra in early life. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 98, 502–514.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wolfe, C. D., & Bell, M. A. (2004). Working memory and inhibitory control in early childhood: Contributions from electrophysiology, temperament, and language. Developmental Psychobiology, 44, 68–83.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×