Skip to main content
×
×
Home

Refined carbohydrate intake in relation to non-verbal intelligence among Tehrani schoolchildren

  • Amin Salehi Abargouei (a1) (a2), Naser Kalantari (a3), Nasrin Omidvar (a3), Bahram Rashidkhani (a3), Anahita Houshiar Rad (a4), Azizeh Afkham Ebrahimi (a5), Hossein Khosravi-Boroujeni (a1) and Ahmad Esmaillzadeh (a1) (a2)...
Abstract
Abstract Objective

Nutrition has long been considered one of the most important environmental factors affecting human intelligence. Although carbohydrates are the most widely studied nutrient for their possible effects on cognition, limited data are available linking usual refined carbohydrate intake and intelligence. The present study was conducted to examine the relationship between long-term refined carbohydrate intake and non-verbal intelligence among schoolchildren.

Design

Cross-sectional study.

Setting

Tehran, Iran.

Subjects

In this cross-sectional study, 245 students aged 6–7 years were selected from 129 elementary schools in two western regions of Tehran. Anthropometric measurements were carried out. Non-verbal intelligence and refined carbohydrate consumption were determined using Raven's Standard Progressive Matrices test and a modified sixty-seven-item FFQ, respectively. Data about potential confounding variables were collected. Linear regression analysis was applied to examine the relationship between non-verbal intelligence scores and refined carbohydrate consumption.

Results

Individuals in top tertile of refined carbohydrate intake had lower mean non-verbal intelligence scores in the crude model (P < 0·038). This association remained significant after controlling for age, gender, birth date, birth order and breast-feeding pattern (P = 0·045). However, further adjustments for mother's age, mother's education, father's education, parental occupation and BMI made the association statistically non-significant. We found a significant inverse association between refined carbohydrate consumption and non-verbal intelligence scores in regression models (β = −11·359, P < 0·001). This relationship remained significant in multivariate analysis after controlling for potential confounders (β = −8·495, P = 0·038).

Conclusions

The study provides evidence indicating an inverse relationship between refined carbohydrate consumption and non-verbal intelligence among Tehrani children aged 6–7 years. Prospective studies are needed to confirm our findings.

  • View HTML
    • Send article to Kindle

      To send this article to your Kindle, first ensure no-reply@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about sending to your Kindle. Find out more about sending to your Kindle.

      Note you can select to send to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be sent to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

      Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

      Refined carbohydrate intake in relation to non-verbal intelligence among Tehrani schoolchildren
      Available formats
      ×
      Send article to Dropbox

      To send this article to your Dropbox account, please select one or more formats and confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your Dropbox account. Find out more about sending content to Dropbox.

      Refined carbohydrate intake in relation to non-verbal intelligence among Tehrani schoolchildren
      Available formats
      ×
      Send article to Google Drive

      To send this article to your Google Drive account, please select one or more formats and confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your Google Drive account. Find out more about sending content to Google Drive.

      Refined carbohydrate intake in relation to non-verbal intelligence among Tehrani schoolchildren
      Available formats
      ×
Copyright
Corresponding author
*Corresponding author: Email esmaillzadeh@hlth.mui.ac.ir
References
Hide All
1. Leiva Plaza B, Inzunza Brito N, Perez Torrejon H et al. (2001) The impact of malnutrition on brain development, intelligence and school work performance. Arch Latinoam Nutr 51, 6471.
2. Brown JL & Pollitt E (1996) Malnutrition, poverty and intellectual development. Sci Am 274, 3843.
3. Zhao L, Yu W, Jia F et al. (2009) The influence of the nutrient supplement for children on the nutrition and health status and intelligence of children of schooling age in the countryside of children of schooling age during 2 years. Wei Sheng Yan Jiu 38, 696699.
4. Stein AD, Wang M, DiGirolamo A et al. (2008) Nutritional supplementation in early childhood, schooling, and intellectual functioning in adulthood: a prospective study in Guatemala. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med 162, 612618.
5. Qian M, Wang D & Chen Z (2000) A preliminary meta-analysis of 36 studies on impairment of intelligence development induced by iodine deficiency. Zhonghua Yu Fang Yi Xue Za Zhi 34, 7577.
6. Gonzalez HF, Malpeli A, Etchegoyen G et al. (2007) Acquisition of visuomotor abilities and intellectual quotient in children aged 4–10 years: relationship with micronutrient nutritional status. Biol Trace Elem Res 120, 92101.
7. Pollitt E, Saco-Pollitt C, Jahari A et al. (2000) Effects of an energy and micronutrient supplement on mental development and behavior under natural conditions in undernourished children in Indonesia. Eur J Clin Nutr 54, Suppl. 2, S80S90.
8. Christian P, Murray-Kolb LE, Khatry SK et al. (2010) Prenatal micronutrient supplementation and intellectual and motor function in early school-aged children in Nepal. JAMA 304, 27162723.
9. Benton D (2001) Micro-nutrient supplementation and the intelligence of children. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 25, 297309.
10. Schoenthaler SJ, Bier ID, Young K et al. (2000) The effect of vitamin–mineral supplementation on the intelligence of American schoolchildren: a randomized, double-blind placebo-controlled trial. J Altern Complement Med 6, 1929.
11. Theodore RF, Thompson JMD, Waldie KE et al. (2009) Dietary patterns and intelligence in early and middle childhood. Intelligence 37, 506513.
12. Leigh Gibson E & Green MW (2002) Nutritional influences on cognitive function: mechanisms of susceptibility. Nutr Res Rev 15, 169206.
13. Messier C (2004) Glucose improvement of memory: a review. Eur J Pharmacol 490, 3357.
14. Dye L & Blundell J (2002) Functional foods: psychological and behavioural functions. Br J Nutr 88, Suppl. 2, S187S211.
15. Gilsenan MB, de Bruin EA & Dye L (2009) The influence of carbohydrate on cognitive performance: a critical evaluation from the perspective of glycaemic load. Br J Nutr 101, 941949.
16. Hoyland A, Lawton CL & Dye L (2008) Acute effects of macronutrient manipulations on cognitive test performance in healthy young adults: a systematic research review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 32, 7285.
17. Popkin BM & Nielsen SJ (2003) The sweetening of the world's diet. Obes Res 11, 13251332.
18. Gross LS, Li L, Ford ES et al. (2004) Increased consumption of refined carbohydrates and the epidemic of type 2 diabetes in the United States: an ecologic assessment. Am J Clin Nutr 79, 774779.
19. Liu S (2002) Intake of refined carbohydrates and whole grain foods in relation to risk of type 2 diabetes mellitus and coronary heart disease. J Am Coll Nutr 21, 298306.
20. McKeown NM, Meigs JB, Liu S et al. (2004) Carbohydrate nutrition, insulin resistance, and the prevalence of the metabolic syndrome in the Framingham Offspring Cohort. Diabetes Care 27, 538546.
21. Preuss HG, Fournier RD, Chieuh CC et al. (1986) Refined carbohydrates affect blood pressure and retinal vasculature in spontaneously hypertensive and Wistar-Kyoto rats. J Hypertens Suppl 4, issue 3, S459S462.
22. Fournier RD, Chiueh CC, Kopin IJ et al. (1986) Refined carbohydrate increases blood pressure and catecholamine excretion in SHR and WKY. Am J Physiol 250, E381E385.
23. Sherafat-Kazemzadeh R, Egtesadi S, Mirmiran P et al. (2010) Dietary patterns by reduced rank regression predicting changes in obesity indices in a cohort study: Tehran Lipid and Glucose Study. Asia Pac J Clin Nutr 19, 2232.
24. Goldbart AD, Row BW, Kheirandish-Gozal L et al. (2006) High fat/refined carbohydrate diet enhances the susceptibility to spatial learning deficits in rats exposed to intermittent hypoxia. Brain Res 1090, 190196.
25. Northstone K, Joinson C, Emmett P et al. (2011) Are dietary patterns in childhood associated with IQ at 8 years of age? A population-based cohort study. J Epidemiol Community Health (Epublication ahead of print version).
26. Willett W (1998) Nutritional Epidemiology. New York, Oxford: Oxford University Press.
27. Azadbakht LMP, Esmailzadeh A & Azizi F (2005) Dairy consumption is inversely associated with the prevalence of the metabolic syndrome in Tehranian adults. Am J Clin Nutr 82, 523530.
28. Dadkhah Piraghaj M, Amini M, Hooshyarrad A et al. (2008) Qualitative and quantitative survey of elementary students, Tehran. Iranian J Nutr Food Technol 3, 3134.
29. Baraheni MN (1974) Raven's progressive matrices as applied to Iranian children. Educ Psychol Meas 34, 983988.
30. World Health Organization (2001) Global Strategy for Infant and Young Child Feeding: The Optimal Duration of Exclusive Breastfeeding. Geneva: WHO.
31. Wesnes KA, Pincock C, Richardson D et al. (2003) Breakfast reduces declines in attention and memory over the morning in schoolchildren. Appetite 41, 329331.
32. Benton D, Maconie A & Williams C (2007) The influence of the glycaemic load of breakfast on the behaviour of children in school. Physiol Behav 92, 717724.
33. Ingwersen J, Defeyter MA, Kennedy DO et al. (2007) A low glycaemic index breakfast cereal preferentially prevents children's cognitive performance from declining throughout the morning. Appetite 49, 240244.
34. Stobart G (2008) Intelligence testing: how to create a monster. In Testing Times: The Uses and Abuses of Assessment, pp. 3056. London: Taylor & Francis.
35. Huang D, Dhawan T, Young S et al. (2011) Fructose impairs glucose-induced hepatic triglyceride synthesis. Lipids Health Dis 10, 20.
36. Molteni R, Barnard RJ, Ying Z et al. (2002) A high-fat, refined sugar diet reduces hippocampal brain-derived neurotrophic factor, neuronal plasticity, and learning. Neuroscience 112, 803814.
37. Park CR (2001) Cognitive effects of insulin in the central nervous system. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 25, 311323.
38. Trejo JL, Piriz J, Llorens-Martin MV et al. (2007) Central actions of liver-derived insulin-like growth factor I underlying its pro-cognitive effects. Mol Psychiatry 12, 11181128.
39. Diano S, Farr SA, Benoit SC et al. (2006) Ghrelin controls hippocampal spine synapse density and memory performance. Nat Neurosci 9, 381388.
40. Het S, Ramlow G & Wolf OT (2005) A meta-analytic review of the effects of acute cortisol administration on human memory. Psychoneuroendocrinology 30, 771784.
Recommend this journal

Email your librarian or administrator to recommend adding this journal to your organisation's collection.

Public Health Nutrition
  • ISSN: 1368-9800
  • EISSN: 1475-2727
  • URL: /core/journals/public-health-nutrition
Please enter your name
Please enter a valid email address
Who would you like to send this to? *
×

Keywords:

Metrics

Altmetric attention score

Full text views

Total number of HTML views: 15
Total number of PDF views: 105 *
Loading metrics...

Abstract views

Total abstract views: 420 *
Loading metrics...

* Views captured on Cambridge Core between September 2016 - 16th December 2017. This data will be updated every 24 hours.